首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   255篇
  免费   22篇
  国内免费   42篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   12篇
  2021年   12篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   11篇
  2018年   23篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   8篇
  2015年   12篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   19篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   10篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   13篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   8篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   12篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
排序方式: 共有319条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
261.
北京香山公园自然景观价值二十年变迁   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
成程  肖燚  饶恩明 《生态学报》2014,34(20):6020-6027
自然景观是由自然环境、物质和景象构成,具有观赏、游览、休闲、疗养等效用和价值的风景综合体或景物。然而,人们在开发利用自然资源时,并没有将自然景观的价值纳入到费用效益分析中,这种对自然景观价值的忽略和低估导致了各种不可逆转的环境问题。如果赋予自然景观合适的经济价值并探究其价值变化,能为其开发利用提供决策支持。以北京香山公园为例,构建了价值评估的指标体系:即自然景观价值为使用价值与非使用价值之和。并对这两种价值分别运用个体旅行费用法和条件价值法进行评估,得出2012年香山公园的使用价值为19.25亿元,非使用价值为3.46亿元。对香山公园在1992—2012年20年间的自然景观价值变化进行深入的研究发现,其景观价值由1992年的0.175亿元增长到2012年的22.71亿元,增长近130倍,速率为1.13亿元/a,游客量的加倍增长、人们游览支出的提高,以及支付意愿的增长是产生该变化的主要原因。随着环境污染和生态破坏的日益加剧以及人们对健康生活的迫切向往,香山公园自然景观价值仍将持续增长,景区当局应全面认识香山公园所承载的价值,在发展旅游的同时也要积极探索解决现有生态环境问题的新思路。  相似文献   
262.
《Cell》2022,185(7):1240-1256.e30
  1. Download : Download high-res image (225KB)
  2. Download : Download full-size image
  相似文献   
263.
Neural activity persisting for one to hundreds of seconds has been postulated to be a substrate of memory. This review article illustrates examples of such activity in limbic system structures including the hippocampus, postsubiculum, and the anterodorsal thalamus. These neuronal responses include better known correlates with the spatial position as well as with head direction of the animal relative to its environment as well as other lesser known examples. Since head direction responses are greater when the animal is actively moving than when passively rotated, it has been proposed that there might be a general mechanism where the behavioral state of the animal can provide modulatory gating of such persistent signals. This would regulate the relative influence of these signals on downstream structures. Neural network attractor models of the head direction cell system are presented to demonstrate how these responses might originate, as well as the dynamics by which they are updated during movements.  相似文献   
264.
Orientation of the lateral roots of trees   总被引:6,自引:2,他引:4  
  相似文献   
265.
Adult Atlantic salmon (55–116 cm total length) with PIT-tags ( n = 318) and both PIT-tags and gastric radio-tags ( n = 127) in a regulated river showed similar upstream migration success, yet radio-tagged fish migrated faster than fish with only PIT-tags. Regurgitation rate of radio-tags was 9%.  相似文献   
266.

Objectives

Long-term home range stability presumably emerges because familiarity with an area improves fitness through increased foraging efficiency, reduced predation risk, or reduced costs of intergroup aggression. While the use of spatial memory by primates has been widely demonstrated, few studies have examined whether long-term space use creates opportunities for interannual reuse of spatial knowledge. Here we examine the ranging behavior of western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) to assess the degree of long-term site fidelity and the foraging consequences of reuse of space.

Methods

We measured interannual home range overlap over a 10-year period for a single group of gorillas at the Mondika Research Center, using both grid-based and kernel density estimation. By plotting the total area used over time, we identified periods of home-range stability and expansion. We compared foraging and ranging behavior in familiar versus unfamiliar areas, considering fruit trees visited, dietary diversity, and daily path length, to determine whether the lack of spatial knowledge in unfamiliar areas was associated with foraging costs.

Results

Average interannual home range overlap by the group remained high throughout the study. During periods of home range expansion, daily path lengths increased but not the number of fruit trees visited, suggesting that reduced familiarity with the area led to decreased foraging efficiency because individuals lacked prior knowledge of where to find resources.

Discussion

Western gorillas at Mondika exhibit long-term home range stability, presumably reflecting a strategy that relies on the use of spatial memory to increase foraging efficiency that is favored by their reliance on ephemeral fruit resources.
  相似文献   
267.
One of the most contested areas in the field of animal cognition is non-human future-oriented cognition. We critically examine key underlying assumptions in the debate, which is mainly preoccupied with certain dichotomous positions, the most prevalent being whether or not ‘real’ future orientation is uniquely human. We argue that future orientation is a theoretical construct threatening to lead research astray. Cognitive operations occur in the present moment and can be influenced only by prior causation and the environment, at the same time that most appear directed towards future outcomes. Regarding the current debate, future orientation becomes a question of where on various continua cognition becomes ‘truly’ future-oriented. We question both the assumption that episodic cognition is the most important process in future-oriented cognition and the assumption that future-oriented cognition is uniquely human. We review the studies on future-oriented cognition in the great apes to find little doubt that our closest relatives possess such ability. We conclude by urging that future-oriented cognition not be viewed as expression of some select set of skills. Instead, research into future-oriented cognition should be approached more like research into social and physical cognition.  相似文献   
268.
269.
《Neuron》2021,109(17):2781-2796.e10
  1. Download : Download high-res image (301KB)
  2. Download : Download full-size image
  相似文献   
270.
1. The multivoltine butterfly Pieris brassicae (Linnaeus) inhabits most climatic zones in Europe and Asia. Its regional populations are adapted to the respective climatic conditions and hibernate in those regions. Yet how these adaptations are stabilised in spite of the species' regular migration events is unclear. 2. In this study, significant differences were found in the preferred flight direction of P. brassicae depending on the season and the butterfly's geographic origin. The individual flight behaviours indicated that flight direction is a heritable character. 3. The bivoltine population from northern Germany undertook a typical return flight in the second generation of the year. This reverse flight was induced by the developmental mode of the caterpillars. A diapause in the pupal stage triggered a northward migration, and a non‐diapause a southward migration. 4. In contrast to bivoltine populations, the second generation of multivoltine populations maintained the direction of the hibernating generation. 5. A unique situation was determined for the population from the northern coast of Spain. The North Sea seemed to be a migration barrier that forced an adaptation, with the first generation flying southeastward, and the second generation northeastward. 6. The flight pattern of at least some of the investigated populations suggests that, after their yearly migration, the descendants of a local population are able to reach the home range of their ancestors again.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号