首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   141篇
  免费   7篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   36篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   3篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   5篇
  2006年   5篇
  2004年   3篇
  2002年   1篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   1篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
排序方式: 共有150条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Cutaneous ridges are present at the surface of the skin in many odontocetes, and although often quite faint, may be observed with the naked eye. We have taken surface impressions and measured the ridges of individuals of seven odontocete species, and observed cutaneous ridges on three additional species. In the delphinids and the one Physeter neonate studied, spatial periods of the ridges varied from 0.4 mm–1.7 mm and trough-to-peak heights from less rhan 10μm to about 60μm. Two Delphinapterus (monodontids) had ridges significantly larger than the Physeter and most delphinids, with spatial periods of 1.9–2.4 mm and heights 80–120 μm. We found the ridges distributed over much of the surface of the body, but relatively faint or absent on most of the head, the control surfaces, and the ventral region in some species. In all of the animals we observed, the ridges ran in an approximately circumferential direction around the body trunk rostral to the dorsal fin or mid-body area, but varied somewhat in direction in the caudal region and in other isolated areas. While the function of the cutaneous ridges has not been established, we speculate that they may play some role in tactile sensing, in the hydrodynamic characteristics of an animal, or both.  相似文献   
73.
Responses of 66 neurons in primary somatosensory cortex (SI) of three anesthetized monkeys (Macaca mulatto) were characterized with grating patterns of 550- to 2900-mm groove width (Gw) and 250-mm ridge width, and/or pairs of 3-mm-wide ridges (bars) spaced 1-20 mm apart. Surfaces were stroked across single fingertips at parametrically varied levels of force ('25-150 g) and velocity ('25-100 mm/sec). The average firing rates (AFRs) of many cells varied with Gw, but force and velocity altered response functions (e.g., from linear to plateau or inverted). Slowly adapting (SA) cells were more sensitive to force, rapidly adapting (RA) cells to velocity. Force and velocity affected all cells sensitive to Gw, which suggests that response independence (e.g., AFR correlated with Gw but not force or velocity) may require active touch

Discharge intervals of many cells replicated stimulus temporal period. This temporal fidelity in SAs far exceeded examples reported for active touch. However, discharge burst duration and AFR increased with Gw, supporting a neural rate rather than temporal code for roughness. Force and velocity altered the Gw at which some cells fired once in phase to stimulus cycle (“tuning point”). Responses to bar edges suggest cortical replication of peripheral mechanoreceptor sensitivity to skin curvature, leading to this temporal fidelity in some cortical cells. Graded RA responses to Gw without obvious stimulus temporal replication may reflect early stages of integrative processing in supra- and infragranular layers that blur obvious temporal patterning and lead to a rate code correlated with spatial variation and proportional to perceived roughness  相似文献   
74.
HP and HPR are related and contiguous genes in strong linkage disequilibrium (LD), encoding haptoglobin and haptoglobin-related protein. These bind and chaperone free Hb for recycling, protecting against oxidation. A copy number variation (CNV) within HP (Hp1/Hp2) results in different possible haptoglobin complexes which have differing properties. HPR rs2000999 (G/A), identified in meta-GWAS, influences total cholesterol (TC) and LDL-cholesterol (LDL-C). We examined the relationship between HP CNV, HPR rs2000999, Hb, red cell count (RCC), LDL-C and TC in the British Women's Heart and Health Study (n=2779 for samples having CNV, rs2000999, and phenotypes). Analysing single markers by linear regression, rs2000999 was associated with LDL-C (β=0.040 mmol/L, p=0.023), TC (β=-0.040 mmol/L, p=0.019), Hb (β=-0.044 g/dL, p=0.028) and borderline with RCC (β=-0.032 × 10(12)/L, p=0.066). Analysis of CNV by linear regression revealed an association with Hb (Hp1 vs Hp2, β=0.057 g/dL, p=0.004), RCC (β=0.045 × 10(12)/L, p=0.014), and showed a trend with LDL-C and TC. There were 3 principal haplotypes (Hp1-G 36%; Hp2-G 45%; Hp2-A 18%). Haplotype comparisons showed that LDL-C and TC associations were from rs2000999; Hb and RCC associations derived largely from the CNV. Distinct genotype-phenotype effects are evident at the genetic epidemiological level once LD has been analysed, perhaps reflecting HP-HPR functional biology and evolutionary history. The derived Hp2 allele of the HP gene has apparently been subject to malaria-driven positive selection. Haptoglobin-related protein binds Hb and apolipoprotein-L, i.e. linking HPR to the cholesterol system; and the HPR/apo-L complex is specifically trypanolytic. Our analysis illustrates the complex interplay between functions and haplotypes of adjacent genes, environmental context and natural selection, and offers insights into potential use of haptoglobin or haptoglobin-related protein as therapeutic agents.  相似文献   
75.
In this study, we compared changes in corticomotor excitability under various task conditions engaging the index finger of each hand. Functional demands were varied, from simple execution to demanding sensory exploration. In a first experiment, we contrasted facilitation in the first dorsal interosseus (FDI) by monitoring changes in motor evoked potentials (MEPs) when participants (young adults, n = 18) performed either a simple button pressing (BP) task or a more demanding tactile exploration (TE) task (i.e., discrimination of raised letters). This experiment showed a large effect of task conditions (p < 0.01) on MEP amplitude but no effect of “Hand”, while latency measurements were unchanged. In fact, MEPs were on average 40% larger during TE (2410 ± 1358 µV) than during BP (1670 ± 1477 µV). The two tasks produced, however, different patterns of electromyographic (EMG) activity, which could have accounted for some of the differences observed. A second experimental session involved a subset of participants (10/18) tested in third task condition: finger movement (FM). The latter task consisted of scanning a smooth surface with the tip of the index finger to reproduce the movements seen with the TE task. The addition of this third condition task confirmed that MEP facilitation seen during TE reflected task-specific influences and not differences in background EMG activity. These results, altogether, provide further insights into the effect of task conditions on corticomotor excitability. Our findings, in particular, stress the importance of behavioural context and tactile exploration in leading to selective increase in corticomotor excitability during finger movements.  相似文献   
76.
77.
KOHN  ROBERT; ANSLEY  CRAIG F. 《Biometrika》1986,73(2):467-473
  相似文献   
78.
In Part I and Part II of these two companion papers (henceforth called Part I and Part II), we develop and evaluate a variational Bayesian expectation maximization (VBEM) method for model inversion of our multi-area extended neural mass model (MEN). In this paper, we develop the VBEM method to estimate posterior distributions of parameters of MEN. We choose suitable prior distributions for the model parameters in order to use properties of a conjugate-exponential model in implementing VBEM. Consequently, VBEM leads to analytically tractable forms. The proposed VBEM algorithm starts with initialization and consists of repeated iterations of a variational Bayesian expectation step (VB E-step) and a variational Bayesian maximization step (VB M-step). Posterior distributions of the model parameters are updated in the VB M-step. Distribution of the hidden state is updated in the VB E-step. We develop a variational extended Kalman smoother (VEKS) to infer the distribution of the hidden state in the VB E-step and derive the forward and backward passes of VEKS, analogous to the Kalman smoother. In Part I, we evaluate and validate the VBEM method using simulation studies.  相似文献   
79.
Sensitivity to time, including the time of reward, guides the behaviour of all organisms. Recent research suggests that all major reward structures of the brain process the time of reward occurrence, including midbrain dopamine neurons, striatum, frontal cortex and amygdala. Neuronal reward responses in dopamine neurons, striatum and frontal cortex show temporal discounting of reward value. The prediction error signal of dopamine neurons includes the predicted time of rewards. Neurons in the striatum, frontal cortex and amygdala show responses to reward delivery and activities anticipating rewards that are sensitive to the predicted time of reward and the instantaneous reward probability. Together these data suggest that internal timing processes have several well characterized effects on neuronal reward processing.  相似文献   
80.
Although the early antidepressant trials which included severely ill and hospitalized patients showed substantial drug‐placebo differences, these robust differences have not held up in the trials of the past couple of decades, whether sponsored by pharmaceutical companies or non‐profit agencies. This narrowing of the drug‐placebo difference has been attributed to a number of changes in the conduct of clinical trials. First, the advent of DSM‐III and the broadening of the definition of major depression have led to the inclusion of mildly to moderately ill patients into antidepressant trials. These patients may experience a smaller magnitude of antidepressant‐placebo differences. Second, drug development regulators, such as the U.S. Food and Drug Administration and the European Medicines Agency, have had a significant, albeit underappreciated, role in determining how modern antidepressant clinical trials are designed and conducted. Their concerns about possible false positive results have led to trial designs that are poor, difficult to conduct, and complicated to analyze. Attempts at better design and patient selection for antidepressant trials have not yielded the expected results. As of now, antidepressant clinical trials have an effect size of 0.30, which, although similar to the effects of treatments for many other chronic illnesses, such as hypertension, asthma and diabetes, is less than impressive.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号