首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   28篇
  免费   0篇
  28篇
  2011年   2篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   3篇
  2006年   1篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   1篇
  1998年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1985年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
排序方式: 共有28条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
12.
The calls of many Orthopteran species are comprised of a simple trill of pulses, the temporal pattern of which is important for call recognition. Male Neoconocephalus nebrascensis produce pulses with a temporal structure typical for the genus. However, they modify this pattern by grouping their pulses into verses, thereby creating a higher order temporal structure. The importance of the pulse pattern and verse structure for call recognition in N. nebrascensis was determined using a walking compensator. Females required the conspecific pulse pattern for call recognition, responding only when the intervals between pulses were short or absent. Females also required the verse structure for call recognition, and recognized the verse structure only when the amplitude modulation depth between verses and pauses exceeded 18 dB. We discuss that the verse recognition mechanism is a derived trait adapted for pre-mating isolation. We hypothesize that the unusually large amplitude modulation required for verse recognition forces males to synchronize their calls in order to preserve an attractive pattern. Call synchrony appears to be the outcome of cooperation, rather than competition, in this species.  相似文献   
13.
ABSTRACT. Positive phonotaxis to tape recorded conspecific song by males of the katydid Neoconocephalus affinis (Beauvois) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) was studied in the field, in Puerto Rico. N.affinis males stridulated loudly during evening hours, and most males were territorial. Aggression between neighbours occurred, albeit rarely, and playback experiments using tape recordings of N.affinis song showed that aggressive behaviour, in the form of jumps directed toward a loudspeaker, could be elicited from a male by song broadcast above a threshold sound intensity. Response thresholds for different males varied from 76 to 93 dB, intensities that corresponded to calculated inter-neighbour distances of 1.6-0.26 m, respectively. Insects that had lower response thresholds were on average more isolated from neighbours and also stridulated more persistently and at a higher sound intensity than insects with higher response thresholds. N.affinis males displayed no phonotaxis toward the song of Neoconocephalus maxillosus , a sympatric species whose song is similar to that of N.affinis in principal frequency but differs in wingstroke rate. Phonotaxis toward conspecific song could only be elicited from males while they were silent.  相似文献   
14.
Since population-level variation in female mating preferences can shape intraspecific communication systems within the context of sexual selection it is essential to quantify these preferences and their sources of variation. We calculated individual female response functions for four male calling song traits in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus, by performing untethered phonotaxis measurements on a spherical locomotor compensator (Kramer treadmill). Firstly, we quantify the population-level sources of phonotactic variation and correct for factors that adversely affect this measurement. Secondly, we develop methodology for the characterisation of individual female phonotactic response functions suitable for population-level analyses and demonstrate the applicability of our method with respect to recent literature on Orthopteran acoustic communication. Phonotaxis towards a preferred stimulus on different occasions is highly repeatable, with lower repeatabilities away from the most preferred signal traits. For certain male signal traits, female preference and selectivity are highly repeatable. Although phonotactic response magnitude deteriorated with age, preference functions of females remained the same during their lifetimes. Finally, the limitations of measuring phonotaxis using a spherical locomotor compensator are described and discussed with respect to the estimation of the selectivity of female response.  相似文献   
15.
The role of Juvenile Hormone III in phonotaxis, mating behavior and egg production of cricket females from Gryllus campestris, Gryllus bimaculatus, Teleogryllus commodus, and Acheta domesticus was investigated.
1.  Phonotaxis of crickets was measured on a Kramer spherical treadmill by means of an infrared system which allowed free walking at any speed and direction. Crickets were allatectomized during the adult or last nymphal stage, and the controls sham-operated. On each cricket, 2 to 9 phonotactic tests were performed during a period of 12 to 51 days; the allatectomized nymphs were tested as adults.
2.  All females deprived of their corpora allata during the adult stage continued to exhibit phonotaxis, whereas females operated as nymphs developed and maintained phonotaxis (Tables 1–4). Some of these females allatectomized or sham-operated during the adult or nymphal stage exhibited correct phonotactic behavior as well as undirected walking.
3.  At the end of the test series, all allatectomized females were found to be devoid of Juvenile Hormone, whereas the controls varied according to age from 0.2 to 2.0 pmol/10 l. The individual ecdysteroid levels differed unpredictably in allatectomized and control females (0.02 to 6.4 pmol/10 l), and no correlation was found between the number of eggs produced by allatectomized females and their ecdysteroid titer.
4.  All allatectomized females displayed sexual receptivity.
5.  Adults allatectomized during the last nymphal instar were still able as adults to develop single eggs in G. campestris and G. bimaculatus, while females of T. commodus did produce and store moderate numbers. However, in A. domesticus no eggs were produced, and terminal oocytes had reached 0.5 to 0.7 mm in length, which is the previtellogenic size of terminal oocytes in normal females at the age of one to two days.
  相似文献   
16.
During two-speaker phonotaxis tests performed with female midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and A. cisternasii), the occurrence of behavioural patterns tentatively considered to be associated with entering amplexus and making a mate choice was noted. The individual repeatability of choice was assessed as a measurement of reliability of female preference, and the individual repeatability of response was assessed as a measurement of the degree of female motivation. The tests were grouped in two categories, spectral and temporal, according to the acoustic variable addressed in the test. Females selecting the alternative chosen less often by the population responded less often in phonotaxis tests and thus could be less motivated. Female repeatability was significant for temporal tests, but not for spectral tests. The observation of behavioural patterns during phonotaxis tests appears to be a useful complement to mate choice studies in anurans. Electronic Publication  相似文献   
17.
1. We used laser vibrometry to study the vibrational frequency response of the eardrum of female gray tree frogs for different positions of the sound source in three-dimensional space. Furthermore, we studied the accuracy of 3-D phonotaxis in the same species for sounds with different frequency contents. 2. The directionality of the eardrum was most pronounced in a narrow frequency range between 1.3 and 1.8 kHz. 3. The average 3-D, horizontal and vertical jump error angles for phonotactic approaches with a sound similar to the natural advertisement call (1.1 and 2.2 kHz frequency components) were 23 degrees, 19 degrees and 12 degrees, respectively. 4. 3-D jump error angle distributions for the 1.4 + 2.2 kHz, 1.0 kHz and 2.0 kHz sounds were not significantly different from that for the 1.1 + 2.2 kHz sound. 5. The average 3-D jump error angle for the 1.4 kHz sound was 36 degrees, and the distribution was significantly different from that for the 1.1 + 2.2 kHz sound. Hence, phonotactic accuracy was poorer in the frequency range of maximum eardrum directionality. 6. Head scanning was not observed and is apparently unnecessary for accurate sound localization in three-dimensional space. 7. Changes in overall sound pressure level experienced by the frog during phonotactic approaches are not an important cue for sound localization.  相似文献   
18.
Cricket ears are sensitive to ultrasound as well as to lower, cricket-like sound frequencies. Ultrasound stimuli evoke negative phonotaxis in flying crickets, a behavior that has been interpreted as a defensive response against predation by echolocating bats. A recent study on a wing-dimorphic species, Gryllus texensis, showed that short-winged individuals, which are incapable of flight, are less sensitive to ultrasound, but not to lower sound frequencies, than their long-winged counterparts. The developmental decision to develop as a long- or short-winged individual is made during the last two larval instars, and there is some evidence suggesting that juvenile hormone (JH) has an instructive role, such that high levels of JH result in short-winged individuals. We show that treatment of last-instar larvae of a monomorphic long-winged species, Teleogryllus oceanicus, with a JH analog causes a decrease in sensitivity to ultrasound, but not to the lower sound frequency used for intraspecific communication.  相似文献   
19.
Auditory-evoked evasive manoeuvres in free-flying locusts and moths   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We presented free-flying locusts (Locusta migratoria L.) with sounds that varied in temporal structure and carrier frequency as they flew toward a light source in a flight room under controlled temperature and light conditions. Previous studies have shown tethered locusts react more often to trains of 30-kHz pulses than to pulse trains below 10 kHz. Further, this acoustic startle response has been suggested to function in bat-avoidance. We expected free-flying locusts to respond similarly; however, we found locusts responded to all sounds we presented, not just high-frequency, bat-like sounds. Response rates of turns, loops, and dives varied from 6% to 26% but were statistically independent of carrier frequency and/or pulse structure. Free-flying moths and tethered locusts were tested using a subset of our acoustic stimuli under the same temperature and light conditions as the free-flying locusts. Moth responses were carrier frequency dependent as were responses of tethered locusts positioned along the flight path observed in our free-flight trials. All responses were unaffected by a 90% reduction in room light. We conclude that locusts possess an acoustic startle response evocable in free flight, however, free-flying locusts do not show the same discrimination observed in tethered locusts under similar conditions.Abbreviations ASR acoustic startle response - dB SPL decibel sound pressure level (RMS re: 20 Pa)  相似文献   
20.
We compare the temporal and directional processing properties of an identified auditory interneuron, ON1, between species with calling songs containing relatively low and high pulse rates (Teleogryllus oceanicus and Gryllus texensis, respectively). Using information theory, we find that the ON1 of G. texensis encodes higher amplitude-modulation frequencies than that of T. oceanicus. Bilateral differences in ON1 responses are also more pronounced in G. texensis, particularly for rapid, G. texensis-like stimuli. We show that brief silent intervals in a pulse train, such as those that occur in the natural calling song of G. texensis, enhance the representation of the stimulus pulse pattern as well as bilateral differences in activity. Our results suggest that the characteristics of an identified neuron vary, across cricket species, in accordance with the temporal structures of their communication signals.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号