首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2697篇
  免费   233篇
  国内免费   68篇
  2024年   5篇
  2023年   25篇
  2022年   32篇
  2021年   68篇
  2020年   86篇
  2019年   103篇
  2018年   78篇
  2017年   77篇
  2016年   84篇
  2015年   123篇
  2014年   113篇
  2013年   119篇
  2012年   91篇
  2011年   102篇
  2010年   93篇
  2009年   137篇
  2008年   156篇
  2007年   196篇
  2006年   122篇
  2005年   85篇
  2004年   87篇
  2003年   93篇
  2002年   87篇
  2001年   91篇
  2000年   94篇
  1999年   82篇
  1998年   86篇
  1997年   51篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   38篇
  1994年   64篇
  1993年   51篇
  1992年   40篇
  1991年   47篇
  1990年   25篇
  1989年   28篇
  1988年   28篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   17篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
排序方式: 共有2998条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Summary When foraging has costs, it is generally adaptive for foragers to adjust their foraging effort in response to changes in the population density of their food. If effort decreases in response to increased food density, this can result in a type-2 functional response; intake rate increases in a negatively accelerated manner as prey density increases. Unlike other mechanisms for type-2 responses, adaptive foraging usually involves a timelag, because foraging behaviours do not often change instantaneously with changes in food density or risks. This paper investigates predator-prey models in which there are explicit dynamics for the rate of adaptive change. Models appropriate to both behavioural and evolutionary change are considered. Both types of change can produce cycles under similar circumstances, but under some evolutionary models there is not sufficient genetic variability for evolutionary change to produce cycles. If there is sufficient variability, the remaining conditions required for cycles are surprisingly insensitive to the nature of the adaptive process. A predator population that approaches the optimum foraging strategy very slowly usually produces cycles under similar conditions as does a very rapidly adapting population.  相似文献   
32.
The community of Gamboa is located on Itacuruçá Island, Sepetiba Bay (State of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil) and includes 26 families, mostly of artisanal fishermen who use paddled or motor canoes, and encircling nets for fishing. In this study, predictions from optimal foraging theory (patch model), in particular of patch residence time, are compared to the observed behavior of fishermen on fishing trips. Fishermen's strategies differ depending on their intended prey. They spend more time in patches and use fewer patches for shrimp than for fish. Gamboa's fishermen tend to leave a patch later than predicted by the model. The difficulty in evaluating stock availability, the comparatively few patches available for shrimp, and the competitive aspects of fishing contribute to the explanation of this behavior.  相似文献   
33.
Abstract.
  • 1 In nest-building wasps there is a continuum from species that provision each offspring cell with one large prey item (single provisioning) to species that provision each cell with many small items (multiple provisioning). The significance of number of prey per cell was examined during two seasons in Ammophila sabulosa (L.), a species that provisions between one and five caterpillars per cell.
  • 2 There was no difference between the total weight of prey in singly-provisioned and multiply-provisioned cells: individual caterpillars placed in multiply-provisioned cells were smaller.
  • 3 Small caterpillars were captured and transported to the nest faster than large ones.
  • 4 If the first prey provisioned was large, the cell was usually then permanently closed. If the first prey was small, additional prey were captured, and females appeared to become more selective: additional prey were rarely large.
  • 5 It is suggested that the flexible provisioning strategy used by A.sabulosa allows a wide range of prey sizes to be utilized without affecting maternal control of offspring size and sex. This could be important when available prey sizes vary temporally and spatially.
  • 6 There was a male-biased first generation investment ratio. Female-producing cells were provisioned with a higher total prey weight than male-producing cells, and conversion of prey weight to adult weight was more efficient for females.
  • 7 Total prey weight provisioned is probably a good indirect measure of parental time investment in A.sabulosa. Other measures, particularly number of prey provisioned, will be less accurate.
  相似文献   
34.
Foraging habitat selection of breeding great reed warblers was studied at a shore of Lake Biwa. The foraging grounds of parent warblers during the nesting period were not restricted to the breeding territory of the reed marsh, their nestling habitat. The paddy field outside the reed marsh was used communally by them throughout the breeding season. Females with early stage nestlings did not visit the paddy field whereas when nestlings were older than 3 days, more than half of their total food was collected there. Females with nests adjacent to the paddy field tended to exploit the paddy field more often than those with nests distant from it. Monogamously mated females tended to exploit the paddy field more often than polygynously mated females. Food collected in the paddy field was larger than that in the reed marsh and parent birds were prepared to travel longer distances to exploit the rich source of food in the paddy field. The importance of the communal foraging ground outside the reed marsh as a background of the polygynous mating system of this species is discussed.  相似文献   
35.
Summary Two laboratory experiments were conducted to examine the ovipositional preferences of the egg parasitoidOoencyrtus nezarae Ishii (Hymenoptera: Encyrtidae) for parasitized and unparasitizedMegacopta punctatissimum Montandon (Hemiptera: Plataspidae). Females that had never oviposited or that had not oviposite for 3 days preferred recently parasitized hosts more than unparasitized hosts. The preference for recently parasitized hosts appeared to be mediated by the punctures in already parasitized hosts made by the ovipositor of the first female. Survival of the parasitoid progeny was lower in recently parasitized hosts than in unparasitized hosts. However, handling time of parasitized hosts was extremely short relative to that of unparasitized hosts, because the superparasitizing female could use the punctures made by the previous females. It is concluded that the females preferred the parasitized hosts over unparasitized hosts because the benefit of saving time and energy for drilling was more than the cost of progeny survival.  相似文献   
36.
Summary Foraging efficiency and intraspecific competition were compared between wild adult and immature rooks Corvus frugilegus with respect to flock size. Behavioural time budgets, and observations of prey selection and prey energetic values revealed that adult rooks in large flocks (> 50 individuals) consumed smaller, less profitable prey, but allocated more time to feeding and fed at a faster rate and with greater success than adults in small flocks. By contrast, immature rooks in flocks of more than 30 individuals allocated proportionally less time to feeding, fed at a lower rate and fed with no increase in success rate than when foraging in smaller flocks. Agonistic encounters and the avoidance of adults by immature rooks appeared responsible for such inefficient foraging. Hence immature rooks showed a preference for smaller flocks (< 50 individuals) with low adult: immature ratios while adults preferred larger flocks (> 50 individuals). We discuss the possible influence of competitive disadvantages on immature rook distribution, flock composition and post-natal dispersal.  相似文献   
37.
高原鼢鼠挖掘取食活动的能量代价及其最佳挖掘取食行为   总被引:3,自引:1,他引:2  
高原鼢鼠(Myospalax baileyi)是青藏高原典型的地下鼠,它主要挖掘取食杂类草之地下根茎,在挖掘过程中形成取食洞道。本文根据此特点,建立了挖掘取食过程中能量代价(E_(?))与取食道结构参数(土丘间距S、侧道长L、取食道半径b及洞道深度D)之间以及取食获能E_(fg)与S和b之间的两个函数关系。分别以挖掘效率极大和取食效率极大为优化目标,导出同样的结果,即相同的最佳土丘间距S_(opt)。通过实验观测,计算出在矮嵩草(Kobresiahumilis)草甸生境条件下,最佳土丘间距S_(opt)=127.4厘米,与野外实测土丘间距(126.30±47.85厘米)无显著差异(t=0.312,P>0.50),表明高原鼢鼠的挖掘及取食行为符合最佳化原则。  相似文献   
38.
Optimal foraging theory is being used increasingly as a means of understanding human foraging behavior. One of the central assumptions of optimal foraging theory is that prey items or patches are encountered sequentially and as a Poisson process. Using empirical data gathered on the Barí hunters of Venezuela, we assess the validity of this central assumption. We compare our observed distribution of encounter frequencies with an expected Poisson distribution, utilizing chisquare tests and graphic representations. The results are strikingly consonant with the expected Poisson distribution and lend support to the applicability of optimal foraging models to human hunting behavior.  相似文献   
39.
Quantitative data on the ways in which the different phases of the stoplight parrotfish (Sparisomaviride Bonnaterre) distribute their time among various activities in different habitats are presented. Individuals spent from 84–97% of their diurnal time swimming, feeding, and hovering. Additionally, large adults spent a significant amount of time sheltering among crevices. Phase-related differences in these activities are statistically significant, as are differences in duration and rates of change of the activities. Large individuals spent more time swimming, while small individuals spent more time hovering. In addition, large individuals performed longer bouts of activity and switched activities less frequently than small individuals. Adult males and females spent approximately equal proportions of time in each of the activity states. Stochastic analyses of behavioural sequences show second order Markov chain dependencies, suggesting that preceding activity states affect subsequent behaviour. Possible relationships between behavioural sequencing and the species foraging strategy are discussed, and it is suggested that the sequence of behavioural activities can provide an estimation of the distribution of food resources in the environment.  相似文献   
40.
Synopsis Laboratory and field data suggest that several species of parasitic lampreys forage primarily at night, but data forPetromyzon marinus are equivocal. Nocturnal foraging may minimize predation risk. This may be particularly important to those species of lampreys that inhabit shallow streams. Nocturnal foraging also may enhance foraging efficiency if potential hosts at night are quiescent and more easily approached. Future experiments should be designed to avoid several potential sources of bias.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号