排序方式: 共有98条查询结果,搜索用时 9 毫秒
41.
42.
Little is known about space use by Common Ravens (Corvus corax) in coastal old‐growth redwood (Sequoia sempervirens) forests in northern California despite their identification as nest predators of federally threatened Marbled Murrelets (Brachyramphus marmoratus). We used radio‐telemetry to examine home range sizes and space use of breeding Common Ravens in Redwood National and State Parks (RNSP), California, in 2009 (N = 3) and 2010 (N = 8). Mean home range size was 182.5 ha and no home ranges were >2 km from roads or human‐use areas. Ravens exhibited high site fidelity between years, and we found little overlap in the home ranges of adjacent ravens. Areas where ranges did overlap were centered on anthropogenic food sources near territory boundaries. Areas most frequently used by ravens were near roads, old‐growth edge, bare ground, and in mixed hardwood and prairie habitats; areas near human‐use areas and in old‐growth habitat were used less by ravens. Mean perch height of ravens (52 m, N = 38) in redwood trees was similar to the mean height of Marbled Murrelet nests in RNSP (48 m). In RNSP, Marbled Murrelet nests that have been located were in old‐growth forest within 2 km of roads. The attraction to anthropogenic resources with frequent use of roads and old‐growth edges and their tendency to perch high in the canopy may increase the likelihood of ravens locating and predating nests of murrelets in RNSP. 相似文献
43.
FREDRIK HAAS MARIE A. POINTER† NICOLA SAINO‡ ERS BRODIN NICHOLAS I. MUNDY† BENGT HANSSON§ 《Molecular ecology》2009,18(2):294-305
The all black carrion crow ( Corvus corone corone ) and the grey and black hooded crow ( Corvus corone cornix ) meet in a narrow hybrid zone across Europe. To evaluate the degree of genetic differentiation over the hybrid zone, we genotyped crows from the centre and edges of the zone, and from allopatric populations in northern (Scotland–Denmark–Sweden) and southern Europe (western–central northern Italy), at 18 microsatellites and at a plumage candidate gene, the MC1R gene. Allopatric and edge populations were significantly differentiated on microsatellites, and populations were isolated by distance over the hybrid zone in Italy. Single-locus analyses showed that one locus, CmeH9, differentiated populations on different sides of the zone at the same time as showing only weak separation of populations on the same side of the zone. Within the hybrid zone there was no differentiation of phenotypes at CmeH9 or at the set of microsatellites, no excess of heterozygotes among hybrids and low levels of linkage disequilibrium between markers. We did not detect any association between phenotypes and nucleotide variation at MC1R , and the two most common haplotypes occurred in very similar frequencies in carrion and hooded crows. That we found a similar degree of genetic differentiation between allopatric and edge populations irrespectively of their location in relation to the hybrid zone, no differentiation between phenotypes within the hybrid zone, and neither heterozygote excess nor consistent linkage disequilibrium in the hybrid zone, is striking considering that carrion and hooded crows are phenotypically distinct and sometimes recognised as separate species. 相似文献
44.
J. T. Pearson H. Tazawa 《Journal of comparative physiology. B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology》1999,169(4-5):256-262
The developmental patterns of mean heart rate (MHR) and instantaneous heart rate (IHR) were investigated in embryos and chicks
of altricial Corvuscorone and Corvus macrorhynchos. The MHR of embryos increased linearly with time from 250 beats · min−1 at mid-incubation to 290 beats · min−1 in hatchlings. MHR during the pipping period was maximal, but only marginally higher than in hatchlings. MHR was stable at
about 290–300 beats · min−1 during the 1st week after hatching. Spontaneous heart rate (HR) decelerations and accelerations were found in embryos and
chicks, disturbing the baseline HR with increasing frequency during development. However, the IHR accelerations developed
later and were less frequent than in precocial species. IHR and body temperature decreased during mild cold exposure (23–25 °C)
and IHR accelerations were reduced in nestlings during the 1st week. We suggest that the development of parasympathetic control
of HR in crows occurs at 60% of incubation, similar to precocial embryos, but sympathetic control may be delayed and suppressed
in contrast to precocial embryos.
Accepted: 3 March 1999 相似文献
45.
LomaJohn T. Pendergraft Adrienne L. Lehnert John M. Marzluff 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》2020,126(2):229-245
Crows and other birds in the family Corvidae regularly share information to learn the identity and whereabouts of dangerous predators, but can they use social learning to solve a novel task for a food reward? Here, we examined the factors affecting the ability of 27 wild-caught American crows to solve a common string pulling task in a laboratory setting. We split crows into two groups; one group was given the task after repeatedly observing a conspecific model the solution and the other solved in the absence of conspecific models. We recorded the crows’ estimated age, sex, size, body condition, level of nervousness, and brain volume using DICOM images from a CT scan. Although none of these variables were statistically significant, crows without a conspecific model and large brain volumes consistently mastered the task in the minimum number of days, whereas those with conspecific models and smaller brain volumes required varying and sometimes a substantial number of days to master the task. We found indirect evidence that body condition might also be important for motivating crows to solve the task. Crows with conspecific models were no more likely to initially solve the task than those working the puzzle without social information, but those that mastered the task usually copied the method most frequently demonstrated by their knowledgeable neighbors. These findings suggest that brain volume and possibly body condition may be factors in learning new tasks and that crows can use social learning to refine their ability to obtain a novel food source, although they must initially learn to access it themselves. 相似文献
46.
Å. Berg 《Bird Study》2013,60(3):355-366
This study investigated the importance of habitat quality and habitat heterogeneity for the abundance and diversity of breeding birds in continuous forest and in forest fragments surrounded by farmland in central Sweden. Positive correlations were found between species number and area, volume of Aspen Populus tremula and habitat heterogeneity. Spatial segregation of habitats at a relatively fine-grained scale is suggested to allow for the co-occurrence of more species. The abundance of at least 18 of the species in this study was influenced by fragmentation, and nine of these species preferred fragments to forest sites. The total density of birds was higher in fragments than in forest sites, probably because several fragment species forage in farmland surrounding the sites and a few also forage at edges. Nine species were more common in forest sites than in fragments, but only one species was restricted to continuous forest. However, several fragments were relatively close to forests (150 m) and forest was common in larger scale contexts. The abundance of most species (25 of 33 species) in this study was correlated with habitat quality variables (i.e. variables measuring the size, volume and diversity of ‘tree species’). Among these habitat variables the most important was the occurrence of deciduous trees which seemed to be important for 14 species. The second most important habitat factor seemed to be the diameter of trees, which was positively correlated with the abundance of eight species of which five are hole-nesters. Among coniferous trees, six species were positively correlated with the volume of Norway Spruce Picea abies, whereas no species seemed to be correlated with the volume of Pine Pinus sylvestris. 相似文献
47.
Individual specialization in the use of foraging tools occurs in hunter-gatherer societies but is absent in non-human primate tool use. 'Parallel tool industries' in hunter-gatherers are mainly based on strict sexual division of labour that is highly reliant on social conformity. Here, we show that 12 individuals in a population of New Caledonian crows on Maré Island had strong preferences for either stick tools or pandanus tools. Eight of the 12 crows had exclusive preferences. The individual specialization that we found is probably associated with different foraging niches. However, in spite of sexual size dimorphism there was no significant association between the sex of crows and their tool preferences. Our findings demonstrate that highly organized, strict sexual division of labour is not a necessary prerequisite for the evolution of parallel tool industries. 相似文献
48.
49.
50.