全文获取类型
收费全文 | 797篇 |
免费 | 39篇 |
国内免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
882篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 24篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 50篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 37篇 |
2018年 | 36篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 28篇 |
2015年 | 42篇 |
2014年 | 48篇 |
2013年 | 43篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 47篇 |
2008年 | 41篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 18篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有882条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Rolf Ergon 《Ecology and evolution》2018,8(8):4031-4041
There is a potential problem in present quantitative genetics evolutionary modeling based on reaction norms. Such models are state‐space models, where the multivariate breeder's equation in some form is used as the state equation that propagates the population state forward in time. These models use the implicit assumption of a constant reference environment, in many cases set to zero. This zero‐point is often the environment a population is adapted to, that is, where the expected geometric mean fitness is maximized. Such environmental reference values follow from the state of the population system, and they are thus population properties. The environment the population is adapted to, is, in other words, an internal population property, independent of the external environment. It is only when the external environment coincides with the internal reference environment, or vice versa, that the population is adapted to the current environment. This is formally a result of state‐space modeling theory, which is an important theoretical basis for evolutionary modeling. The potential zero‐point problem is present in all types of reaction norm models, parametrized as well as function‐valued, and the problem does not disappear when the reference environment is set to zero. As the environmental reference values are population characteristics, they ought to be modeled as such. Whether such characteristics are evolvable is an open question, but considering the complexity of evolutionary processes, such evolvability cannot be excluded without good arguments. As a straightforward solution, I propose to model the reference values as evolvable mean traits in their own right, in addition to other reaction norm traits. However, solutions based on an evolvable G matrix are also possible. 相似文献
52.
现代神经科学研究指出,大脑是外部世界的“预测器”,它能根据先验知识和当前信息对即将到来的感觉信息进行主动估计,从而完成与外部世界的高效交互。预测性编码是描述预期作用机制的主要理论模型,梳理其在解释视、听觉神经现象方面的研究进展,可为深入理解大脑工作模式提供新的理论基础。本文简述了预测性编码的内容;从常用范式、典型现象、面临争议等方面梳理预期与感觉输入相互作用的典型研究;从有预期无刺激的神经表征、预期相关神经振荡模式两方面简述预期独立于刺激的内源性神经表征;进而回顾了支持预测性编码中分级结构的神经生理证据及重要神经结构。最后,本文从深化理论研究、助力疾病诊疗、启发脑-机接口技术等方面对预测性编码相关研究的发展进行了展望。深入理解预测性编码在视、听觉神经活动中的计算模型及神经表征,有望为揭示大脑感知觉神经活动工作模式开辟新途径。 相似文献
53.
Although extraocular light can entrain the circadian rhythms of invertebrates and nonmammalian vertebrates, almost all studies show that the mammalian circadian system can only be affected by light to the eyes. The exception is a recent study by Campbell and Murphy that reported phase shifts in humans to bright light applied with fiber-optic pads behind the knees (popliteal region). We tested whether this extraocular light stimulus could accelerate the entrainment of circadian rhythms to a shift of the sleep schedule, as occurs in shift work or jet lag. In experiment 1, the sleep/dark episodes were delayed 8h from baseline for 2 days, and 3h light exposures were timed to occur before the temperature minimum to help delay circadian rhythms. There were three groups: (1) bright (about 13,000 lux) extraocular light from fiber-optic pads, (2) control (dim light, 10-20 lux), and (3) medium-intensity (about 1000 lux) ocular light from light boxes. In experiment 2, the sleep/dark episodes were inverted, and extraocular light was applied either before the temperature minimum to help delay circadian rhythms or after the temperature minimum to help advance rhythms. Circadian phase markers were the salivary dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) and the rectal temperature minimum. There was no evidence that the popliteal extraocular light had a phase-shifting effect in either experiment. Possible reasons for phase shifts in the Campbell and Murphy study and not the current study include the many differences between the protocols. In the current study, there was substantial sleep deprivation before the extraocular light was applied. There was a large shift in the sleep/dark schedule, rather than allowing subjects to sleep each day from midnight to noon, as in the Campbell and Murphy study. Also, when extraocular light was applied in the current protocol, subjects did not experience a change from sleeping to awake, a change in posture (from lying in bed to sitting in a chair), or a change in ocular light (from dark to dim light). Further research is necessary to determine the conditions under which extraocular light might produce phase shifts in human circadian rhythms. (Chronobiology International, 17(6), 807-826, 2000). 相似文献
54.
When three traits make a line: evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation through linear reaction norms in stochastic environments 下载免费PDF全文
Genetic assimilation emerges from selection on phenotypic plasticity. Yet, commonly used quantitative genetics models of linear reaction norms considering intercept and slope as traits do not mimic the full process of genetic assimilation. We argue that intercept–slope reaction norm models are insufficient representations of genetic effects on linear reaction norms and that considering reaction norm intercept as a trait is unfortunate because the definition of this trait relates to a specific environmental value (zero) and confounds genetic effects on reaction norm elevation with genetic effects on environmental perception. Instead, we suggest a model with three traits representing genetic effects that, respectively, (i) are independent of the environment, (ii) alter the sensitivity of the phenotype to the environment and (iii) determine how the organism perceives the environment. The model predicts that, given sufficient additive genetic variation in environmental perception, the environmental value at which reaction norms tend to cross will respond rapidly to selection after an abrupt environmental change, and eventually becomes equal to the new mean environment. This readjustment of the zone of canalization becomes completed without changes in genetic correlations, genetic drift or imposing any fitness costs of maintaining plasticity. The asymptotic evolutionary outcome of this three‐trait linear reaction norm generally entails a lower degree of phenotypic plasticity than the two‐trait model, and maximum expected fitness does not occur at the mean trait values in the population. 相似文献
55.
The ‘uncanny valley’ response is a phenomenon involving the elicitation of a negative feeling and subsequent avoidant behaviour in human adults and infants as a result of viewing very realistic human-like robots or computer avatars. It is hypothesized that this uncanny feeling occurs because the realistic synthetic characters elicit the concept of ‘human’ but fail to satisfy it. Such violations of our normal expectations regarding social signals generate a feeling of unease. This conflict-induced uncanny valley between mutually exclusive categories (human and synthetic agent) raises a new question: could an uncanny feeling be elicited by other mutually exclusive categories, such as familiarity and novelty? Given that infants prefer both familiarity and novelty in social objects, we address this question as well as the associated developmental profile. Using the morphing technique and a preferential-looking paradigm, we demonstrated uncanny valley responses of infants to faces of mothers (i.e. familiarity) and strangers (i.e. novelty). Furthermore, this effect strengthened with the infant''s age. We excluded the possibility that infants detect and avoid traces of morphing. This conclusion follows from our finding that the infants equally preferred strangers’ faces and the morphed faces of two strangers. These results indicate that an uncanny valley between familiarity and novelty may accentuate the categorical perception of familiar and novel objects. 相似文献
56.
Gibberellins: perception,transduction and responses 总被引:40,自引:0,他引:40
Richard Hooley 《Plant molecular biology》1994,26(5):1529-1555
57.
Most models for incomplete data are formulated within the selection model framework. This paper studies similarities and differences of modeling incomplete data within both selection and pattern-mixture settings. The focus is on missing at random mechanisms and on categorical data. Point and interval estimation is discussed. A comparison of both approaches is done on side effects in a psychiatric study. 相似文献
58.
Stephen J. Boulter 《Biology & philosophy》2004,19(2):243-261
In this paper I present a transcendental argument based on the findings of cognitive psychology and neurophysiology which invites two conclusions: First and foremost, that a pre-condition of visual perception itself is precisely what the Aristotelian and other commonsense realists maintain, namely, the independent existence of a featured, or pre-packaged world; second, this finding, combined with other reflections, suggests that, contra McDowell and other neo-Kantians, human beings have access to things as they are in the world via non-projective perception. These two conclusions taken together form the basis of Aristotelian metaphysical realism and a refutation of the neo-Kantian two-factor approach to perception. 相似文献
59.
Tubiana M 《Radiation and environmental biophysics》2000,39(1):3-16
The majority of the public in industrial countries believes that pollution and low doses of radiation are threats to good
health. As a matter of fact, when these putative risks are compared to those originating from lifestyle, they appear very
small. In particular, the risks associated with low doses of irradiation, even when they are assessed with the most pessimistic
models, appear extremely small. Public anxiety is fuelled by the uncertainty regarding the magnitude of this risk and the
use of the linear no threshold (LNT) hypothesis, which gives credence to the concept that even the smallest doses are harmful.
There are a number of scientific and epidemiological data currently under debate that are not consistent with the LNT hypothesis.
For example, no difference in the incidence of cancers or of birth defects has been observed between regions with low or high
natural irradiation. This inconsistency between perceptions and data underlines the role of psychological factors studied
since 1957 which should be placed in the perspective of the public’s present attitude toward risk and technology. Social amplification
or attenuation of risk may occur in several ways. Fearful concern about radiation began in 1955, with the beginning of the
Cold War, when the possibility of a nuclear holocaust appeared very real. Analysis of the data shows that these fears of technology
could have a detrimental effect; they should therefore be investigated and understood.
Received: 13 October 1999 / Accepted in revised form: 27 December 1999 相似文献
60.
A new method will be presented which allows the perception of body odors in humans to be studied objectively. The analysis
of body odor‐evoked potentials was used to investigate if and how the human brain is able to differentiate self from non‐self
body odor for the first time. Six subjects (three females) participated in two experimental sessions. In each session, two
body odors (axillary hair) were presented within an olfactory oddball paradigm. One of the odors was collected from the subject
and the other from an odor donor of the same sex. In the first session the subjects' attention was distracted to a secondary
task (passive paradigm), in the second session the subjects were asked to actively differentiate the odors (active paradigm).
For the EEG recordings the odors were presented within a constantly flowing airstream. The results show that the subjects
could hardly differentiate the body odors subjectively. However, it could be demonstrated that the central nervous processing
of one's own odor was faster than the processing of the chemosensory non‐self signal. Moreover, in the active paradigm, the
potentials appeared to be larger when the subjects perceived their own body odor. The conclusion is reached that the measurement
of chemosensory event‐related potentials (CSERP) is the method of choice for the investigation of HLA‐associated body odors.
This revised version was published online in July 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献