全文获取类型
收费全文 | 533697篇 |
免费 | 59712篇 |
国内免费 | 345篇 |
专业分类
593754篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 5242篇 |
2017年 | 5107篇 |
2016年 | 6985篇 |
2015年 | 8805篇 |
2014年 | 10531篇 |
2013年 | 15300篇 |
2012年 | 17031篇 |
2011年 | 17411篇 |
2010年 | 11760篇 |
2009年 | 10777篇 |
2008年 | 15198篇 |
2007年 | 15744篇 |
2006年 | 14709篇 |
2005年 | 14101篇 |
2004年 | 13972篇 |
2003年 | 13326篇 |
2002年 | 12827篇 |
2001年 | 28370篇 |
2000年 | 28226篇 |
1999年 | 21987篇 |
1998年 | 6731篇 |
1997年 | 7311篇 |
1996年 | 6737篇 |
1995年 | 6213篇 |
1994年 | 5989篇 |
1993年 | 5960篇 |
1992年 | 17048篇 |
1991年 | 16302篇 |
1990年 | 15708篇 |
1989年 | 15214篇 |
1988年 | 13931篇 |
1987年 | 12948篇 |
1986年 | 12046篇 |
1985年 | 11821篇 |
1984年 | 9664篇 |
1983年 | 8092篇 |
1982年 | 5996篇 |
1981年 | 5377篇 |
1980年 | 5102篇 |
1979年 | 8944篇 |
1978年 | 6811篇 |
1977年 | 6285篇 |
1976年 | 5648篇 |
1975年 | 6237篇 |
1974年 | 6764篇 |
1973年 | 6542篇 |
1972年 | 5983篇 |
1971年 | 5434篇 |
1970年 | 4684篇 |
1969年 | 4407篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 12 毫秒
991.
992.
C Holliger S W Kengen G Schraa A J Stams A J Zehnder 《Journal of bacteriology》1992,174(13):4435-4443
Reductive dechlorination of 1,2-dichloroethane (1,2-DCA) to ethylene and chloroethane (CA) by crude cell extracts of Methanobacterium thermoautotrophicum delta H with H2 as the electron donor was stimulated by Mg-ATP. The heterodisulfide of coenzyme M (CoM) and 7-mercaptoheptanoylthreonine phosphate together with Mg-ATP partially inhibited ethylene production but stimulated CA production compared Mg-ATP alone. The pH optimum for the dechlorination was 6.8 (at 60 degrees C). Michaelis-Menten kinetics for initial product formation rates with different 1,2-DCA concentrations indicated the enzymatic character of the dechlorination. Apparent Kms for 1,2-DCA of 89 and 119 microM and Vmaxs of 34 and 20 pmol/min/mg of protein were estimated for ethylene and CA production, respectively. 3-Bromopropanesulfonate, a specific inhibitor for methyl-CoM reductase, completely inhibited dechlorination of 1,2-DCA. Purified methyl-CoM reductase, together with flavin adenine dinucleotide and a crude component A fraction which reduced the nickel of factor F430 in methyl-CoM reductase, converted 1,2-DCA to ethylene and CA with H2 as the electron donor. In this system, methyl-CoM reductase was also able to transform its own inhibitor 2-bromoethanesulfonate to ethylene. 相似文献
993.
994.
The effects of β-endorphin under the conditions of naloxone hydrochloride blockade of opiate receptors, as well as the effects of the selective agonists of μ-and δ-receptors DAGO and DADLE and the effects of melanocyte-potentiating factor (MPF), on the in vitro proliferative response of lymphocytes were studied. The dose-effect dependence indicated stimulating effects of β-endorphin, DAGO, and DADLE on the proliferative response in the presence of phytohemagglutinin (PHA). The tetrapeptide MPF, which is the C-terminal sequence of β-endorphin, had almost no effect on the proliferative activity of lymphocytes. β-Endorphin, naloxone, and the μ-and δ-receptor selective agonists enhanced the proliferative response of lymphocytes in an unfractionated cell culture, whereas β-endorphin, naloxone, and DAGO suppressed the proliferative activity of lymphocytes in the mononuclear fraction purified of monocytes. In both cases, the naloxone blockade of opiate receptors enhanced rather than eliminated the β-endorphin effect. 相似文献
995.
Evidence has been accumulating to support the process of reinforcement as a potential mechanism in speciation. In many species,
mate choice decisions are influenced by cultural factors, including learned mating preferences (sexual imprinting) or learned
mate attraction signals (e.g., bird song). It has been postulated that learning can have a strong impact on the likelihood
of speciation and perhaps on the process of reinforcement, but no models have explicitly considered learning in a reinforcement
context. We review the evidence that suggests that learning may be involved in speciation and reinforcement, and present a
model of reinforcement via learned preferences. We show that not only can reinforcement occur when preferences are learned
by imprinting, but that such preferences can maintain species differences easily in comparison with both autosomal and sex-linked
genetically inherited preferences. We highlight the need for more explicit study of the connection between the behavioral
process of learning and the evolutionary process of reinforcement in natural systems. 相似文献
996.
Current issues in fish welfare 总被引:11,自引:0,他引:11
F. A. Huntingford† C. Adams V. A. Braithwaite‡ S. Kadri T. G. Pottinger§ P. Sandøe¶ J. F. Turnbull 《Journal of fish biology》2006,68(2):332-372
Human beings may affect the welfare of fish through fisheries, aquaculture and a number of other activities. There is no agreement on just how to weigh the concern for welfare of fish against the human interests involved, but ethical frameworks exist that suggest how this might be approached. Different definitions of animal welfare focus on an animal's condition, on its subjective experience of that condition and/or on whether it can lead a natural life. These provide different, legitimate, perspectives, but the approach taken in this paper is to focus on welfare as the absence of suffering. An unresolved and controversial issue in discussions about animal welfare is whether non‐human animals exposed to adverse experiences such as physical injury or confinement experience what humans would call suffering. The neocortex, which in humans is an important part of the neural mechanism that generates the subjective experience of suffering, is lacking in fish and non‐mammalian animals, and it has been argued that its absence in fish indicates that fish cannot suffer. A strong alternative view, however, is that complex animals with sophisticated behaviour, such as fish, probably have the capacity for suffering, though this may be different in degree and kind from the human experience of this state. Recent empirical studies support this view and show that painful stimuli are, at least, strongly aversive to fish. Consequently, injury or experience of other harmful conditions is a cause for concern in terms of welfare of individual fish. There is also growing evidence that fish can experience fear‐like states and that they avoid situations in which they have experienced adverse conditions. Human activities that potentially compromise fish welfare include anthropogenic changes to the environment, commercial fisheries, recreational angling, aquaculture, ornamental fish keeping and scientific research. The resulting harm to fish welfare is a cost that must be minimized and weighed against the benefits of the activity concerned. Wild fish naturally experience a variety of adverse conditions, from attack by predators or conspecifics to starvation or exposure to poor environmental conditions. This does not make it acceptable for humans to impose such conditions on fish, but it does suggest that fish will have mechanisms to cope with these conditions and reminds us that pain responses are in some cases adaptive (for example, suppressing feeding when injured). In common with all vertebrates, fish respond to environmental challenges with a series of adaptive neuro‐endocrine adjustments that are collectively termed the stress response. These in turn induce reversible metabolic and behavioural changes that make the fish better able to overcome or avoid the challenge and are undoubtedly beneficial, in the short‐term at least. In contrast, prolonged activation of the stress response is damaging and leads to immuno‐suppression, reduced growth and reproductive dysfunction. Indicators associated with the response to chronic stress (physiological endpoints, disease status and behaviour) provide a potential source of information on the welfare status of a fish. The most reliable assessment of well‐being will be obtained by examining a range of informative measures and statistical techniques are available that enable several such measures to be combined objectively. A growing body of evidence tells us that many human activities can harm fish welfare, but that the effects depend on the species and life‐history stage concerned and are also context‐dependent. For example, in aquaculture, adverse effects related to stocking density may be eliminated if good water quality is maintained. At low densities, bad water quality may be less likely to arise whereas social interactions may cause greater welfare problems. A number of key differences between fish and birds and mammals have important implications for their welfare. Fish do not need to fuel a high body temperature, so the effects of food deprivation on welfare are not so marked. For species that live naturally in large shoals, low rather than high densities may be harmful. On the other hand, fish are in intimate contact with their environment through the huge surface area of their gills, so they are vulnerable to poor water quality and water borne pollutants. Extrapolation between taxa is dangerous and general frameworks for ensuring welfare in other vertebrate animals need to be modified before they can be usefully applied to fish. The scientific study of fish welfare is at an early stage compared with work on other vertebrates and a great deal of what we need to know is yet to be discovered. It is clearly the case that fish, though different from birds and mammals, however, are sophisticated animals, far removed from unfeeling creatures with a 15 s memory of popular misconception. A heightened appreciation of these points in those who exploit fish and in those who seek to protect them would go a long way towards improving fish welfare. 相似文献
997.
T. Moen M. Delghandi M. S. Wesmajervi J.-I. Westgaard K. T. Fjalestad 《Animal genetics》2009,40(6):993-996
A first genetic linkage map of the Atlantic cod ( Gadus morhua ) was produced, based on segregation data from 12 full-sib families of Norwegian origin. The map contained 174 single nucleotide polymorphism markers and 33 microsatellites, distributed on 25 linkage groups and had a length of 1225 cM. A significant difference in recombination rates between sexes was found, the average ratio of female:male recombination rates being 1.78 ± 1.62 (SD). 相似文献
998.
We have studied the immunomodulatory properties of epithelial cells from the small intestine on T cell immune function in vitro. Proliferation of lymph node cells stimulated either with antigen or with mitogen was inhibited by epithelial cells in a dose-dependent fashion. The epithelial cell-mediated suppression of lymphocyte proliferation was blocked by indomethacin, a cyclooxygenase pathway inhibitor, demonstrating that the suppressive effect of epithelial cells was related to prostaglandin secretion. Furthermore, the action of epithelial cell-secreted prostaglandin on lymphocytes was related to its effect on IL-2 as the suppressive effect of epithelial cells was abrogated by the addition of exogenous IL-2. As previously reported, epithelial cells constitutively express MHC class II and we found them able to present antigen in a class II-restricted fashion when their suppressive effects were blocked by indomethacin. Furthermore, epithelial cells activated by LPS secrete an IL-1 like molecule in a fashion analogous to other antigen-presenting cells. These results demonstrate that epithelial cells can both enhance and suppress in vitro T cell immune responses and further characterize the mechanisms by which intestinal epithelial cells may function in gut-associated immune responses. 相似文献
999.
The isolation and characterisation of mutants of Aspergillus nidulans showing resistance to MNNG is described. Such isolates were stable through prolonged subculture in the absence of the selective agent, and resistance segregated as an allele of a single gene in meiotic and mitotic analysis. MNNG-resistant strains showed an increase in resistance to EMS and UV irradiation but no cross-resistance to MMS was detected. Possible mechanisms of resistance to alkylating agents are discussed. 相似文献
1000.
Analytical errors in measuring radioactivity in cell proteins and their effect on estimates of protein turnover in L cells. 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Previous studies from this laboratory on protein turnover in 3H-labelled L-cell cultures have shown recovery of total 3H at the end of a 3-day experiment to be always significantly in excess of the 3H recovered at the beginning of the experiment. In this study we have critically reviewed a number of possible sources for this error in measuring radioactivity in cell proteins. 3H-labelled proteins, when dissolved in 0.3 M-NaOH and counted for radioactivity in a liquid-scintillation spectrometer, showed losses of 30-40% of the radioactivity; neither external or internal standardization compensated for this loss. Hydrolysis of these proteins with either Pronase or concentrated HCl significantly increased the measured radioactivity. In addition, approx. 5-10% of the cell protein is left on the plastic culture dish when cells are recovered in phosphate-buffered saline. To aggravate this latter loss further, this surface-adherent protein, after pulse labelling, contains proteins of high radioactivity that turn over rapidly and make a major contribution to the accumulating radioactivity in the medium. These combined errors can account for up to 60% of the total radioactivity in the cell culture. Similar analytical errors have been found in studies of other cell cultures. The effect of these analytical errors on estimates of protein turnover in cell cultures is discussed. 相似文献