首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   386篇
  免费   51篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   9篇
  2020年   1篇
  2019年   1篇
  2018年   5篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   24篇
  2014年   18篇
  2013年   13篇
  2012年   28篇
  2011年   16篇
  2010年   19篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   28篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   28篇
  2005年   23篇
  2004年   21篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   23篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   4篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
排序方式: 共有437条查询结果,搜索用时 421 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
Purple bacteria have peripheral light-harvesting (PLH) complexes adapted to high-light (LH2) and low-light (LH3, LH4) growth conditions. The latter two have only been fully characterised in Rhodopseudomonas acidophila 7050 and Rhodopseudomonas palustris CGA009, respectively. It is known that LH4 complexes are expressed under the control of two light sensing bacteriophytochromes (BphPs). Recent genomic sequencing of a number of Rps. palustris strains has provided extensive information on PLH genes. We show that both LH3 and LH4 complexes are present in Rps. palustris and have evolved in the same operon controlled by the two adjacent BphPs. Two rare marker genes indicate that a gene cluster CL2, containing LH2 genes and the BphP RpBphP4, was internally transferred within the genome to form a new operon CL1. In CL1, RpBphP4 underwent gene duplication to RpBphP2 and RpBphP3, which evolved to sense light intensity rather than spectral red/far-red intensity ratio. We show that a second LH2 complex was acquired in CL1 belonging to a different PLH clade and these two PLH complexes co-evolved together into LH3 or LH4 complexes. The near-infrared spectra provide additional support for our conclusions on the evolution of PLH complexes based on genomic data.  相似文献   
57.
In the present study, the insulin secretory capacity ofTC6-F7 cells in microcapsules was evaluated. The cell mass within capsules was found to expand in a three-dimensional fashion, in contrast to cells seeded on plates that grew as a monolayer. In invitro studies, both free and encapsulated cells were found to secreteinsulin in the absence of glucose, at 13.6 ± 1.1 and 14.5 ± 0.9 ng · 106cells1 · 60 min1, respectively, withthe response rising to a maximum of 26.0 ± 0.8 and 31 ± 2.3 ng · 106cells1 · 60 min1 in the presence of16.8 mM glucose. Encapsulated cells were able to produceCa2+ responses in the presence ofKCl (50 mM) and BAY K 8644 (100 µM). In in vivo studies,intraperitoneal transplantation of 3.0 ×106 microencapsulated cellsinto mice (n = 5) withstreptozotocin-induced diabetes resulted in the restoration ofnormoglycemia up to 57 days. Insulin concentrations rose from 0.4 ± 0.1 ng/ml before the graft administration to 2.2 ± 0.8 ng/ml afterthe transplantation in the normoglycemic recipients. An oral glucosechallenge in transplant recipients demonstrated a flat glucoseresponse, suggesting extremely high glucose clearance rates. These datademonstrate the potential use of the immunoisolated -cell lines forthe treatment of diabetes.

  相似文献   
58.
In order to evaluate the influence of the number of catches in pheromone-baited traps on the percentage of larval infestation, six delta traps equipped with sex attractant were placed in each of three regions in Central Greece (Farkadona, Farsala, Almyros) in cotton fields from 20 June until 30 September 1995. The collection of fruiting bodies took place weekly and the counting of adults in the traps was carried out each day. The population fluctuation in all three regions was similar with their peak during the first weeks of August. In Farkadona the infestation level was low (1% at the first sampling of August) with a maximum of 9% in the last sampling of September. In Farsala and Almyros, the infestation level was already high (10% and above) in early August. There was a significant positive linear correlation between the number of moth catches and the infestation percentage from first- and second-stage larvae on the first (R = 0694) and second (R = 0.7399) boll-feeding generations.  相似文献   
59.
Self-deception, paranoia, and overconfidence involve misbeliefs about the self, others, and world. They are often considered mistaken. Here we explore whether they might be adaptive, and further, whether they might be explicable in Bayesian terms. We administered a difficult perceptual judgment task with and without social influence (suggestions from a cooperating or competing partner). Crucially, the social influence was uninformative. We found that participants heeded the suggestions most under the most uncertain conditions and that they did so with high confidence, particularly if they were more paranoid. Model fitting to participant behavior revealed that their prior beliefs changed depending on whether the partner was a collaborator or competitor, however, those beliefs did not differ as a function of paranoia. Instead, paranoia, self-deception, and overconfidence were associated with participants’ perceived instability of their own performance. These data are consistent with the idea that self-deception, paranoia, and overconfidence flourish under uncertainty, and have their roots in low self-esteem, rather than excessive social concern. The model suggests that spurious beliefs can have value–self-deception is irrational yet can facilitate optimal behavior. This occurs even at the expense of monetary rewards, perhaps explaining why self-deception and paranoia contribute to costly decisions which can spark financial crashes and devastating wars.  相似文献   
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号