首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   85733篇
  免费   7587篇
  国内免费   42篇
  2023年   237篇
  2022年   403篇
  2021年   1548篇
  2020年   880篇
  2019年   1108篇
  2018年   1440篇
  2017年   1241篇
  2016年   2139篇
  2015年   3631篇
  2014年   4048篇
  2013年   4812篇
  2012年   6546篇
  2011年   6486篇
  2010年   4174篇
  2009年   3802篇
  2008年   5431篇
  2007年   5437篇
  2006年   5240篇
  2005年   5037篇
  2004年   4895篇
  2003年   4669篇
  2002年   4405篇
  2001年   860篇
  2000年   623篇
  1999年   965篇
  1998年   1232篇
  1997年   821篇
  1996年   744篇
  1995年   653篇
  1994年   620篇
  1993年   668篇
  1992年   558篇
  1991年   523篇
  1990年   465篇
  1989年   405篇
  1988年   428篇
  1987年   353篇
  1986年   326篇
  1985年   404篇
  1984年   520篇
  1983年   412篇
  1982年   507篇
  1981年   491篇
  1980年   414篇
  1979年   311篇
  1978年   326篇
  1977年   283篇
  1976年   264篇
  1975年   209篇
  1974年   246篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
Among adult females and males of African antelope impala are unique in their performance of reciprocal allogrooming. The occurrence of this behaviour in neonatal impala fawns was explored in a free-ranging impala herd at the San Diego Wild Animal Park where 5 dam-reared fawns were observed from birth through 10 weeks of age. One-way maternal grooming and reciprocal allogrooming with the dam and non dam partners emerged as distinct behavioural systems. Maternal grooming, directed mostly to the anogenital area, was typical of that seen in other ungulates, and sharply declined over the first two weeks. Reciprocal allogrooming, characterized by alternate exchanges of grooming bouts with a partner in the same manner as in adults, was seen as early as 3–8 d after birth. All fawns were grooming with unrelated adult females by the end of the second week. By week 2 virtually every measure of reciprocal allogrooming by fawns (grooming delivered per hour, reciprocity, and percent of encounters initiated) was as high as for adults. The appearance of this reciprocal allogrooming pattern, especially at such an early age, appears to be unique among ungulates, and possibly mammals in general. Three hand-reared impala fawns, deprived of the opportunity to interact with older herdmates, but having access to impala fawns and heterospecific fawns, were observed from 1–3 mo of age. The hand-reared impala showed no alteration in the occurrence of reciprocal allogrooming behaviour compared with the dam-reared control fawns, indicating that allogrooming experience with older animals was not required for the appearance of reciprocal allogrooming at an early age. Interestingly, hand-reared fawns persisted in grooming heterospecific fawns despite the fact that heterospecifics rarely reciprocated grooming. We postulate that the strong predisposition for impala young to groom others may be related to the threat of tick infestation in the impala's ecotone habitat.  相似文献   
172.
173.
174.
Utilization of the organic components of senescent Spartina alterniflora Loisel detritus by the grass shrimp, Palaemonetes pugio Holthuis, was investigated in laboratory feeding studies. A 14C-dimethyl sulfate labeling procedure was utilized to label the sterilized detritus. The formed covalent bond is stable to autoclaving, freezing and extremes of pH. An ingestion rate of 2.0 × 10?4CS·h?1 was determined for P. pugio. Estimates for rate of incorporation, gross growth efficiency, and total biomass incorporated are presented. An inverse relationship was demonstrated between shrimp size and rates of ingestion and incorporation.  相似文献   
175.
176.
177.
178.
Trade and Trade-Offs: Using Resources, Making Choices, and Taking Risks. M. Estellie Smith. Prospect Heights, 1L: Waveland Press, 2000.250 pp.  相似文献   
179.
180.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号