首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   86227篇
  免费   7650篇
  国内免费   42篇
  2023年   239篇
  2022年   629篇
  2021年   1557篇
  2020年   883篇
  2019年   1113篇
  2018年   1445篇
  2017年   1247篇
  2016年   2145篇
  2015年   3640篇
  2014年   4061篇
  2013年   4833篇
  2012年   6563篇
  2011年   6516篇
  2010年   4183篇
  2009年   3814篇
  2008年   5452篇
  2007年   5466篇
  2006年   5251篇
  2005年   5047篇
  2004年   4904篇
  2003年   4674篇
  2002年   4414篇
  2001年   881篇
  2000年   638篇
  1999年   976篇
  1998年   1239篇
  1997年   826篇
  1996年   754篇
  1995年   662篇
  1994年   619篇
  1993年   668篇
  1992年   565篇
  1991年   529篇
  1990年   469篇
  1989年   406篇
  1988年   427篇
  1987年   361篇
  1986年   326篇
  1985年   404篇
  1984年   521篇
  1983年   405篇
  1982年   504篇
  1981年   491篇
  1980年   416篇
  1979年   304篇
  1978年   324篇
  1977年   283篇
  1976年   271篇
  1975年   209篇
  1974年   251篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 431 毫秒
171.
172.
Among adult females and males of African antelope impala are unique in their performance of reciprocal allogrooming. The occurrence of this behaviour in neonatal impala fawns was explored in a free-ranging impala herd at the San Diego Wild Animal Park where 5 dam-reared fawns were observed from birth through 10 weeks of age. One-way maternal grooming and reciprocal allogrooming with the dam and non dam partners emerged as distinct behavioural systems. Maternal grooming, directed mostly to the anogenital area, was typical of that seen in other ungulates, and sharply declined over the first two weeks. Reciprocal allogrooming, characterized by alternate exchanges of grooming bouts with a partner in the same manner as in adults, was seen as early as 3–8 d after birth. All fawns were grooming with unrelated adult females by the end of the second week. By week 2 virtually every measure of reciprocal allogrooming by fawns (grooming delivered per hour, reciprocity, and percent of encounters initiated) was as high as for adults. The appearance of this reciprocal allogrooming pattern, especially at such an early age, appears to be unique among ungulates, and possibly mammals in general. Three hand-reared impala fawns, deprived of the opportunity to interact with older herdmates, but having access to impala fawns and heterospecific fawns, were observed from 1–3 mo of age. The hand-reared impala showed no alteration in the occurrence of reciprocal allogrooming behaviour compared with the dam-reared control fawns, indicating that allogrooming experience with older animals was not required for the appearance of reciprocal allogrooming at an early age. Interestingly, hand-reared fawns persisted in grooming heterospecific fawns despite the fact that heterospecifics rarely reciprocated grooming. We postulate that the strong predisposition for impala young to groom others may be related to the threat of tick infestation in the impala's ecotone habitat.  相似文献   
173.
174.
175.
Utilization of the organic components of senescent Spartina alterniflora Loisel detritus by the grass shrimp, Palaemonetes pugio Holthuis, was investigated in laboratory feeding studies. A 14C-dimethyl sulfate labeling procedure was utilized to label the sterilized detritus. The formed covalent bond is stable to autoclaving, freezing and extremes of pH. An ingestion rate of 2.0 × 10?4CS·h?1 was determined for P. pugio. Estimates for rate of incorporation, gross growth efficiency, and total biomass incorporated are presented. An inverse relationship was demonstrated between shrimp size and rates of ingestion and incorporation.  相似文献   
176.
177.
178.
179.
Trade and Trade-Offs: Using Resources, Making Choices, and Taking Risks. M. Estellie Smith. Prospect Heights, 1L: Waveland Press, 2000.250 pp.  相似文献   
180.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号