首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1205093篇
  免费   199957篇
  国内免费   30165篇
  1435215篇
  2018年   13143篇
  2017年   12341篇
  2016年   16961篇
  2015年   22272篇
  2014年   25865篇
  2013年   36098篇
  2012年   40197篇
  2011年   41131篇
  2010年   29688篇
  2009年   31357篇
  2008年   37041篇
  2007年   37879篇
  2006年   34888篇
  2005年   33768篇
  2004年   33440篇
  2003年   31801篇
  2002年   31475篇
  2001年   56897篇
  2000年   55384篇
  1999年   47164篇
  1998年   20657篇
  1997年   20021篇
  1996年   18764篇
  1995年   18171篇
  1994年   17425篇
  1993年   16986篇
  1992年   38796篇
  1991年   37686篇
  1990年   37083篇
  1989年   35871篇
  1988年   33212篇
  1987年   31039篇
  1986年   28941篇
  1985年   28465篇
  1984年   23517篇
  1983年   19723篇
  1982年   15720篇
  1981年   14063篇
  1980年   13402篇
  1979年   21618篇
  1978年   17034篇
  1977年   15643篇
  1976年   14278篇
  1975年   15428篇
  1974年   16881篇
  1973年   16584篇
  1972年   15280篇
  1971年   13961篇
  1970年   12099篇
  1969年   11683篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
The experimental literature on learning by Primate and non-Primate mammals is reviewed, with the aim of identifying peculiarly Primate features of learning. The evidence indicates that quantitative comparisons of learning by Primates and other mammals are intrinsically equivocal and uninformative because of the impossibility of equating experimental conditions for members of different species. The comparative results of early learning set studies were seriously misleading because the test conditions discriminated against representatives of species in which vision is not a dominant modality.Analyses of transfer between different learning tasks strongly suggest that rhesus macaques differ qualitatively from non-Primates like cats in that they develop generalized, trans-situationally valid response strategies during training on a particular problem which can facilitate learning in other situations. Non-Primate mammals appear not to develop such strategies under the same circumstances. There is also evidence that monkeys are more able than cats to discard previously learned strategies when they are no longer maximally profitable.  相似文献   
982.
N S Swack  G D Hsiung 《In vitro》1974,10(5-6):260-267
  相似文献   
983.
984.
985.
986.
987.
Summary Two methods for determining the hydrocarbon-metabolizing enzyme activity of cultured mammalian cells were compared. The method designed to measure benzo[a]an-thracene-induced aryl hydrocarbon hydroxylase activity could detect and quantify enzyme activities in low passage rodent cells, but could not reproducibly detect levels in intermediate or high passage mouse, rat, or human cells. The method designed to measure the ability of a cell to convert benzo[a]pyrene from an organic-soluble to an aqueous acetone-soluble form proved to be more reproducible. This technique, when modified, was demonstrated to be an effective screening test for the detection of those lines with higher levels of hydrocarbon-metabolizing enzymes. Supported by the Council for Tobacco Research and Contract NIH 70-2068 within the Virus Cancer Program, National Cancer Institute, National Institutes of Health.  相似文献   
988.
989.
990.
Ba(2+) current through the L-type Ca(2+) channel inactivates essentially by voltage-dependent mechanisms with fast and slow kinetics. Here we found that slow inactivation is mediated by an annular determinant composed of hydrophobic amino acids located near the cytoplasmic ends of transmembrane segments S6 of each repeat of the alpha(1C) subunit. We have determined the molecular requirements that completely obstruct slow inactivation. Critical interventions include simultaneous substitution of A752T in IIS6, V1165T in IIIS6, and I1475T in IVS6, each preventing in additive manner a considerable fraction of Ba(2+) current from inactivation. In addition, it requires the S405I mutation in segment IS6. The fractional inhibition of slow inactivation in tested mutants caused an acceleration of fast inactivation, suggesting that fast and slow inactivation mechanisms are linked. The channel lacking slow inactivation showed approximately 45% of the sustained Ba(2+) or Ca(2+) current with no indication of decay. The remaining fraction of the current was inactivated with a single-exponential decay (pi(f) approximately 10 ms), completely recovered from inactivation within 100 ms and did not exhibit Ca(2+)-dependent inactivation properties. No voltage-dependent characteristics were significantly changed, consistent with the C-type inactivation model suggesting constriction of the pore as the main mechanism possibly targeted by Ca(2+) sensors of inactivation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号