首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   11909篇
  免费   1318篇
  国内免费   10篇
  2021年   118篇
  2020年   89篇
  2019年   94篇
  2018年   110篇
  2017年   116篇
  2016年   195篇
  2015年   317篇
  2014年   354篇
  2013年   470篇
  2012年   554篇
  2011年   574篇
  2010年   361篇
  2009年   358篇
  2008年   534篇
  2007年   523篇
  2006年   512篇
  2005年   556篇
  2004年   517篇
  2003年   475篇
  2002年   489篇
  2001年   318篇
  2000年   287篇
  1999年   257篇
  1998年   193篇
  1997年   163篇
  1996年   128篇
  1995年   133篇
  1994年   123篇
  1993年   136篇
  1992年   209篇
  1991年   213篇
  1990年   201篇
  1989年   191篇
  1988年   189篇
  1987年   173篇
  1986年   145篇
  1985年   163篇
  1984年   165篇
  1983年   144篇
  1982年   143篇
  1981年   137篇
  1980年   134篇
  1979年   182篇
  1978年   162篇
  1977年   132篇
  1976年   129篇
  1975年   104篇
  1974年   100篇
  1973年   106篇
  1971年   81篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 312 毫秒
21.
Different batches of ABTS obtained from the same commercial source varied in their capacity to effect direct mutation in the strains of Salmonella typhimurium used routinely in the incorporation test of Ames. One batch, obtained in 1976, and another obtained early in 1979, both exhibited direct base-pair substitution and frame-shift activities. These activities, however, were absent from each of two batches obtained after 1979, and also from a highly purified preparation from a different source. The possible presence of the unsulphonated immediate precursor of ABTS as a mutagenic impurity is an unlikely explanation for the activity of the mutagenic preparations. It is more probable that the commercial synthesis generated other, mutagenic, impurities which remained in the batches obtained in 1976 and early in 1979, but were absent or were removed from later batches. The identity of these active impurities is unknown. Pure ABTS is neither a direct nor an indirect mutagen.  相似文献   
22.
23.
24.
25.
Horizontal gene transfer often leads to phenotypic changes within recipient organisms independent of any immediate evolutionary benefits. While secondary phenotypic effects of horizontal transfer (i.e., changes in growth rates) have been demonstrated and studied across a variety of systems using relatively small plasmids and phage, little is known about the magnitude or number of such costs after the transfer of larger regions. Here we describe numerous phenotypic changes that occur after a large-scale horizontal transfer event (∼1 Mb megaplasmid) within Pseudomonas stutzeri including sensitization to various stresses as well as changes in bacterial behavior. These results highlight the power of horizontal transfer to shift pleiotropic relationships and cellular networks within bacterial genomes. They also provide an important context for how secondary effects of transfer can bias evolutionary trajectories and interactions between species. Lastly, these results and system provide a foundation to investigate evolutionary consequences in real time as newly acquired regions are ameliorated and integrated into new genomic contexts.  相似文献   
26.
There have been numerous reports in the literature describing the diversity of microbial flora isolated from woodwind and brass instruments, with potential infection risks for players, especially when such instruments are shared. Steam disinfection has become established as a trusted method of decontamination; however, there have been no reports on the employment of this technology to disinfect parts of musical instruments, hence it was the aim of this study to examine the fate of bacterial and yeast pathogens on artificially contaminated trumpet mouthpieces and to evaluate whether such disinfection is an effective method of disinfection for such instrument parts. Trumpet mouthpieces were artificially contaminated with 18 microbial strains (17 bacteria from four genera (Enterococcus, Escherichia, Staphylococcus and Streptococcus) and one yeast (Candida)), each at an inoculum density of approximately 1·5 × 107 colony forming units and subjected to a disinfection cycle. The experiment was repeated including 50% (v/v) sterile sputum as soil. No bacteria or yeast organisms were recovered post disinfection, including following recovery and with nonselective cultural enrichment techniques.  相似文献   
27.
Previous work has shown that male flesh flies (Sarcophaga crassipalpis Macquart) exhibit an ontogeny of behaviour from eclosion through sexual maturity that includes extensive changes in the expression of aggressive, non‐aggressive interactive and non‐interactive behaviours. To determine how the presence of a female flesh fly influences the manifestation of these behaviours, male flesh flies of different ages post‐eclosion are paired with same‐age females and their behaviours are monitored in a simple arena during a 50‐min observation period. All flies are socially isolated until pairing. Although the levels of expression of aggressive and non‐aggressive interactive behaviours are depressed relative to previous findings in male‐opponent pairs, the ontogeny of aggression still occurs as indicated by a significant increase, with age, in the agonistic behaviour ‘hold’. Similar to male‐opponent pairs and individual males, the performance by males of the non‐interactive behaviours ‘walking’ and ‘standing’ diminishes, whereas ‘upside‐down’ increases with age. By contrast, ‘grooming’ shows a significant age‐related decline. No courtship behaviours are observed in the males, although the aggressive behaviour ‘hold’ is a significant transition to mating. Females show no obvious courtship or rejection behaviours, although the significant increase in ‘upside‐down’ with age could possibly be a behavioural gateway to mating. The results of this study indicate that extensive age‐related changes encompassing the entire behavioural repertoire are intrinsic to male flesh flies and persist under a variety of different social contexts.  相似文献   
28.
29.
We studied the behavioral and emotional dynamics displayed by two people trying to resolve a conflict. 59 groups of two people were asked to talk for 20 minutes to try to reach a consensus about a topic on which they disagreed. The topics were abortion, affirmative action, death penalty, and euthanasia. Behavior data were determined from audio recordings where each second of the conversation was assessed as proself, neutral, or prosocial. We determined the probability density function of the durations of time spent in each behavioral state. These durations were well fit by a stretched exponential distribution, with an exponent, , of approximately 0.3. This indicates that the switching between behavioral states is not a random Markov process, but one where the probability to switch behavioral states decreases with the time already spent in that behavioral state. The degree of this “memory” was stronger in those groups who did not reach a consensus and where the conflict grew more destructive than in those that did. Emotion data were measured by having each person listen to the audio recording and moving a computer mouse to recall their negative or positive emotional valence at each moment in the conversation. We used the Hurst rescaled range analysis and power spectrum to determine the correlations in the fluctuations of the emotional valence. The emotional valence was well described by a random walk whose increments were uncorrelated. Thus, the behavior data demonstrated a “memory” of the duration already spent in a behavioral state while the emotion data fluctuated as a random walk whose steps did not have a “memory” of previous steps. This work demonstrates that statistical analysis, more commonly used to analyze physical phenomena, can also shed interesting light on the dynamics of processes in social psychology and conflict management.  相似文献   
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号