首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   19478篇
  免费   2389篇
  国内免费   6篇
  2021年   236篇
  2020年   172篇
  2019年   210篇
  2018年   248篇
  2017年   217篇
  2016年   387篇
  2015年   613篇
  2014年   671篇
  2013年   838篇
  2012年   946篇
  2011年   967篇
  2010年   605篇
  2009年   610篇
  2008年   811篇
  2007年   897篇
  2006年   792篇
  2005年   751篇
  2004年   744篇
  2003年   688篇
  2002年   661篇
  2001年   456篇
  2000年   470篇
  1999年   480篇
  1998年   270篇
  1997年   224篇
  1996年   243篇
  1995年   214篇
  1994年   214篇
  1993年   231篇
  1992年   362篇
  1991年   351篇
  1990年   328篇
  1989年   294篇
  1988年   303篇
  1987年   296篇
  1986年   273篇
  1985年   313篇
  1984年   285篇
  1983年   228篇
  1982年   207篇
  1981年   191篇
  1980年   172篇
  1979年   233篇
  1978年   205篇
  1977年   172篇
  1976年   201篇
  1975年   191篇
  1973年   196篇
  1972年   182篇
  1970年   165篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Summary A positive relationship was established between energy expenditure and pulse rate of echolocation for 8 pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) when hanging at rest in a respirometry chamber at 28 °C. The least squares fit equation: Energy expenditure (J·–1·h–1)=110.09+ 40.3 pulse rate (n/s) explained 14% of the minute by minute variation in energy expenditure. For a 6 g bat therefore each pulse costs approximately 0.067 Joules to produce. The net cost of echolocation at 10 pulses per second for a 6 g pipistrelle bat was predicted to be 9.5 × BMR with a range of 7.0–12.2 × BMR. We suggest that since a major portion of the cost of echolocation may result from contraction of the pectoralis and scapularis groups of muscles, the cost of echolocation is reduced for flying animals which contract these muscles anyway during flight. This may account for the high incidence of echolocation systems amongst flying vertebrates, when compared with terrestrial species.  相似文献   
42.
Flows of biomass and respiratory carbon were studied in a series of propylene-oxide sterilized soil microcosms. One-half of the microcosms received three pulsed additions of 200 ppm glucose-carbon to mimic rhizosphere carbon inputs. Biotic variables were: bacteria (Pseudomonas) alone, or amoebae (Acanthamoeba) and nematodes (Mesodiplogaster) singly, or both combined in the presence of bacteria.Over the 24-day experiment, respiration was significantly higher in the microcosms containing the bacterial grazers. Biomass accumulation by amoebae was significantly higher than that by nematodes. The nematodes respired up to 30-fold more CO2 per unit biomass than did amoebae. Similar amounts of carbon flowed into both respiratory and biomass carbon in microcosms with fauna, compared with the bacteria-alone microcosms. However, partitioning of available carbon by the microfauna varied considerably, with little biomass production and relatively more CO2-C produced in the nematode-containing microcosms. The amoebae, in contrast, allocated more carbon to tissue production (about 40% assimilation efficiency) and correspondingly less to CO2.  相似文献   
43.
44.
45.
46.
Often we must balance being prepared to act quickly with being prepared to suddenly stop. The stop signal task (SST) is widely used to study inhibitory control, and provides a measure of the speed of the stop process that is robust to changes in subjects’ response strategy. Previous studies have shown that preparation affects inhibition. We used fMRI to separate activity that occurs after a brief (500 ms) warning stimulus (warning-phase) from activity that occurs during responses that follow (response-phase). Both of these phases could contribute to the preparedness to stop because they both precede stop signals. Warning stimuli activated posterior networks that signal the need for top-down control, whereas response phases engaged prefrontal and subcortical networks that implement top-down control. Regression analyses revealed that both of these phases affect inhibitory control in different ways. Warning-phase activity in the cerebellum and posterior cingulate predicted stop latency and accuracy, respectively. By contrast, response-phase activity in fronto-temporal areas and left striatum predicted go speed and stop accuracy, in pre-supplementary motor area affected stop accuracy, and in right striatum predicted stop latency and accuracy. The ability to separate hidden contributions to inhibitory control during warning-phases from those during response-phases can aid in the study of models of preparation and inhibitory control, and of disorders marked by poor top-down control.  相似文献   
47.
48.
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号