首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   741篇
  免费   77篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   15篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   12篇
  2015年   14篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   18篇
  2012年   30篇
  2011年   31篇
  2010年   15篇
  2009年   16篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   19篇
  2005年   19篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   14篇
  2001年   20篇
  2000年   28篇
  1999年   21篇
  1998年   20篇
  1997年   9篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   13篇
  1993年   11篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   21篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   21篇
  1982年   13篇
  1981年   15篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   10篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   7篇
  1972年   20篇
  1971年   12篇
  1970年   6篇
  1969年   11篇
  1968年   14篇
排序方式: 共有818条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
741.
Group foraging can be beneficial for ungulates by decreasing the time required for vigilance, but it can also prove costly because of competition. To determine responses to gregarious behaviour, we studied foraging activity and vigilance of impala ( Aepyceros melampus ) near Kruger National Park, South Africa. We measured time spent foraging, vigilant, moving, grooming, engaging in social interactions and determined herd size and group distribution (i.e. density). We calculated accepted food abundance (AFA), food ingestion rate, steps per minute and percent vigilance for female, bachelor male and herd male impala. There was no relationship between herd size and vigilance, but vigilance decreased with increasing density ( t 1,311 = 4.91, P  <0.0001). Additionally, AFA decreased ( t 1,61 = 5.96, P  <0.0001) and steps per minute increased ( t 1,311 = 14.38, P  <   0.0001) as more individuals fed in close proximity to each other. Impala could be altering their behaviour to accommodate a perceived change in resources because of intraspecific competition and these adjustments might be related more to the distribution of herd members than to herd size. Further studies should examine the behaviour of gregarious animals in relation to the distribution of herd members in addition to group size.  相似文献   
742.
743.
1. Soluble proteins were recovered from male Schistosoma mansoni after homogenization in Tris-HCl buffer containing 0.6 M KCl and 1.0% Triton X-100 followed by preparative electrophoresis on SDS-gel. 2. Polyclonal antibodies produced in mice against the soluble fraction were used in comparative analysis of S. mansoni and S. japonicum using immunoblots and immunoprecipitation of in vitro translated polypeptides. 3. Small molecular weight polypeptide (20-22 kdalton), identified by infected mouse serum (IMS) on immunoblots, was predominant in females and was not cross-reactive with heterologous IMS. 4. A 41-43 kdalton polypeptide which appeared as a doublet on immunoblots performed with polyclonal antiserum 4M, was predominant in males of both species although the polypeptides of S. mansoni showed slower electrophoretic mobility, and therefore the larger size (43 kdalton), than that of S. japonicum. 5. Comparison of fluorograms of the immunoprecipitates of in vitro translated polypeptides indicated that IMS of S. mansoni precipitated two, 30 and 94 kdalton, polypeptides while the IMS of S. japonicum identified at 72 kdalton polypeptide. Antisera 1M, 2M and 4M also showed similarities and differences in polypeptides of in vitro translation products of the two species of Schistosoma.  相似文献   
744.
745.
746.
747.
748.
749.
750.
Odor identification by males and females: predictions vs performance   总被引:4,自引:4,他引:0  
Cain  William S. 《Chemical senses》1982,7(2):129-142
More than 200 males and females estimated the identifiabilityof 80 common odorous objects (e.g., chocolate, beer, mustard,rubber). The two genders agreed rather closely in their estimates.The groups also agreed closely in their relative judgementsof whether males or females could identify the objects better.Both groups anticipated that males would be superior for onlya small number of substances, mainly substances that seem stereotypically‘male’ (e.g., cigar butts, beer, machine oil). Thegroups anticipated female superiority for not only stereotypically‘female’ substances (e.g., Ivory soap, Johnson'sbaby powder, nail polish remover), but also for virtually allfoods, including foods presumably consumed equally by both sexes(e.g., potato chips, Juicy Fruit gum, grape drink). The resultssuggested the existence of a second stereotype, namely thatfemales will be superior at identifying all substances not clearlyin the male domain. An experiment that explored the performanceof 46 males and females over five sessions revealed generalfemale superiority. The superiority extended to odors considered‘male’. Males seemed educable and could apparentlyovercome their disadvantage eventually with merely some helpin the retrieval of odor names. Blockage of retrieval seemsa strong limiting factor in odor identification for femalesas well as for males. Irrespective of gender, when persons havethe veridical name of a well-known odor made available by oneor another means, they exhibit considerable talent at identifyingobjects by smell. 1Supported by NIH Grant ES 00592  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号