全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2656篇 |
免费 | 217篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
2877篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 27篇 |
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 32篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 112篇 |
2014年 | 142篇 |
2013年 | 143篇 |
2012年 | 217篇 |
2011年 | 209篇 |
2010年 | 121篇 |
2009年 | 123篇 |
2008年 | 182篇 |
2007年 | 169篇 |
2006年 | 156篇 |
2005年 | 173篇 |
2004年 | 140篇 |
2003年 | 143篇 |
2002年 | 149篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 45篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有2877条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
C. Louise Milligan Tony P. Farrell 《Journal of comparative physiology. B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology》1986,156(4):583-590
Summary Exhausting activity in the sea raven resulted in a pronounced extracellular acidosis, which consisted of a large, short-lived respiratory component and a small, longer-lived metabolic component. Thi disturbance had been corrected by 12 h. White muscle experienced a pronounced intracellular acidosis of chiefly metabolic origin, with pHi dropping from a resting value of 7.51 to a low of 7.10 immediately post-activity. The recovery of pHi was associated with a reduction in muscle lactate. Despite the large increase in
, cardiac muscle pHi remained constant postactivity, actually showing an alkalosis at 30 min into recovery. Maintenance of cardiac muscle pHi was achieved by an accumulation of HCO
3
–
intracellularly. 相似文献
5.
6.
Sheldon T 《BMJ (Clinical research ed.)》1994,309(6962):1107
7.
Mutisia subspinosa Cav. is described and illustrated and its relationships with other members of Mutisia sect. Guariruma (Cass.) Cabrera discussed. Notes on its cultivation are provided along with a historical insight into the collections of Née, made on the Malaspina expedition. 相似文献
8.
9.
Elsabbagh M Mercure E Hudry K Chandler S Pasco G Charman T Pickles A Baron-Cohen S Bolton P Johnson MH;BASIS Team 《Current biology : CB》2012,22(4):338-342
Autism spectrum disorders (henceforth autism) are diagnosed in around 1% of the population [1]. Familial liability confers risk for a broad spectrum of difficulties including the broader autism phenotype (BAP) [2, 3]. There are currently no reliable predictors of autism in infancy, but characteristic behaviors emerge during the second year, enabling diagnosis after this age [4, 5]. Because indicators of brain functioning may be sensitive predictors, and atypical eye contact is characteristic of the syndrome [6-9] and the BAP [10, 11], we examined whether neural sensitivity to eye gaze during infancy is associated with later autism outcomes [12, 13]. We undertook a prospective longitudinal study of infants with and without familial risk for autism. At 6-10 months, we recorded infants' event-related potentials (ERPs) in response to viewing faces with eye gaze directed toward versus away from the infant [14]. Longitudinal analyses showed that characteristics of ERP components evoked in response to dynamic eye gaze shifts during infancy were associated with autism diagnosed at 36 months. ERP responses to eye gaze may help characterize developmental processes that lead to later emerging autism. Findings also elucidate the mechanisms driving the development of the social brain in infancy. 相似文献
10.
Anthony F. Morse Viridian L. Benitez Tony Belpaeme Angelo Cangelosi Linda B. Smith 《PloS one》2015,10(3)
For infants, the first problem in learning a word is to map the word to its referent; a second problem is to remember that mapping when the word and/or referent are again encountered. Recent infant studies suggest that spatial location plays a key role in how infants solve both problems. Here we provide a new theoretical model and new empirical evidence on how the body – and its momentary posture – may be central to these processes. The present study uses a name-object mapping task in which names are either encountered in the absence of their target (experiments 1–3, 6 & 7), or when their target is present but in a location previously associated with a foil (experiments 4, 5, 8 & 9). A humanoid robot model (experiments 1–5) is used to instantiate and test the hypothesis that body-centric spatial location, and thus the bodies’ momentary posture, is used to centrally bind the multimodal features of heard names and visual objects. The robot model is shown to replicate existing infant data and then to generate novel predictions, which are tested in new infant studies (experiments 6–9). Despite spatial location being task-irrelevant in this second set of experiments, infants use body-centric spatial contingency over temporal contingency to map the name to object. Both infants and the robot remember the name-object mapping even in new spatial locations. However, the robot model shows how this memory can emerge –not from separating bodily information from the word-object mapping as proposed in previous models of the role of space in word-object mapping – but through the body’s momentary disposition in space. 相似文献