首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Sensory integration in citric acid/sucrose mixtures   总被引:4,自引:3,他引:1  
The scale values of perceived sweetness, sourness and totaltaste intensity of unmixed sucrose, unmixed citric acid andseveral citric acid/sucrose mixtures were assessed, using afunctional measurement approach in combination with a two-stimulusprocedure. The data showed that the scale values obtained werelinear with perceived taste intensity. It was demonstrated thatcitric acid suppresses the sweetness of sucrose and that, inversely,sucrose suppresses the sourness of citric acid. However, thissuppressive effect was not symmetrical in the range of concentrationsused. While the degree of sweetness suppression depended onlyon the citric acid level, the degree of sourness suppressiondepended on the sucrose as well as on the citric acid concentration.With regard to the perceived total taste intensity of citricacid/sucrose mixtures, it was shown that the sum of sweetnessand sourness approximately equals the total taste intensity.The implications of the present findings for the analytic—syntheticcontroversy and for taste interaction theories are discussed.  相似文献   

2.
Differences in the sweet‐blocking efficacy of 2‐(4‐methoxyphenoxy) propanoic acid (PMP) for different sweeteners (sucrose and aspartame) and for various exposure areas of the mouth were found. Twenty participants rated sweetener solutions with and without PMP for sweetness, sourness, saltiness, bitterness and umami for stimulation of anterior tongue, posterior tongue and whole‐mouth areas. For sweetness ratings, suppression was significant for all stimulation areas. In the presence of PMP, stimulation of the posterior tongue yielded significantly higher sweetness ratings than stimulation of the anterior tongue for aspartame but not for sucrose. Sourness and bitterness ratings were significantly higher for anterior tongue than posterior tongue stimulations for aspartame but not for sucrose. The increases in sourness ratings in the presence of PMP were likely because of the sour taste PMP has at the concentration used. Results imply a difference between the front and the back of the tongue in the mechanisms involved in the perception of sweetness.  相似文献   

3.
Psychophysical judgments often depend on stimulus context. For example, sugar solutions are judged sweeter when a tasteless fruity aroma has been added. Response context also matters; adding a fruity aroma to sugar increases the rated sweetness when only sweetness is considered but not when fruitiness is judged as well. The interaction between stimulus context and response context has been explored more extensively in taste-odor mixtures than in taste-taste mixtures. To address this issue, subjects in the current study rated the sourness of citric acid mixed with quinine (bitter), sodium chloride (salty), and cyclamate (sweet) (stimulus context). In one condition, subjects rated sourness alone. In another, subjects rated both sourness and the other salient quality (bitterness, saltiness, or sweetness) (response context). Sourness ratings were most sensitive to response context for sour-salty mixtures (i.e., ratings of sourness alone exceeded ratings of sourness made simultaneously with saltiness) and least sensitive to context for the sour-sweet mixtures (sourness ratings made under the 2 conditions were essentially identical). Response-context effects for the sour-bitter mixture were nominally intermediate. The magnitudes of these context effects were related to judgments of qualitative similarity between citric acid and the other stimuli, consistent with prior findings. These types of context effects are relevant to the study of taste-taste mixture interactions and should provide insight into the perceptual similarities among the taste qualities.  相似文献   

4.
This study investigated the relationship between perception of an odour when smelled and the taste of a solution to which the odour is added as a flavorant. In Experiment 1 (E1) sweetness, sourness, liking and intensity ratings were obtained for 20 odours. Taste ratings were then obtained for sucrose solutions to which the odours had been added as flavorants. Certain odours were found to enhance tasted sweetness while others suppressed it. The degree to which an odour smelled sweet was the best predictor of the taste ratings. These findings were extended in Experiment 2 (E2), which included a second tastant, citric acid, and employed four odours from E1. The most sweet smelling odour, caramel, was found to suppress the sourness of citric acid and, as in E1, to enhance the sweetness of sucrose. Again, odours with low sweetness suppressed the sweetness of tasted sucrose. The study demonstrated that the effects of odours on taste perception are not limited to sweetness enhancement and apply to sour as well as sweet tastes. The overall pattern of results is consistent with an explanation of the taste properties of odours in terms of prior flavour-taste associations.  相似文献   

5.
6.
The effect of soluble starch (acid-modified starch) on taste intensity was investigated in human subjects. Different concentrations of sucrose (Suc), six sweeteners, NaCl, quinine-HCl (QHCl) and citric acid (Cit) were dissolved in either distilled water (DW; standard) or starch solution (test solution). The solutions were presented to naive subjects and each subject was requested to taste and compare the sweetness intensity between the standard and test solutions based on a scale ranging from +3 (enhanced) to -3 (inhibited). A greater sweetness intensity occurred with Suc at different concentration (0.1-1.0 M) dissolved in soluble starch (0.125% to 4.0%) than with Suc in DW. Similarly, five other different products of soluble starch at 0.25 and 4.0% resulted in enhancement of sweetness for 0.3 and 1.0 M Suc. With the sole exception of the taste of 0.3 M Suc, sweet enhancement did not occur with 0.43 M fructose, 0.82 M glucose, 0.82 M sorbitol, 0.0037 M aspartame, 0.0042 M saccharin-Na or 0.016 M cyclamate. Neither the saltiness of NaCl (0.01-0.3 M), the bitterness of QHCl (0.00003-0.001 M) nor the sourness of Cit (0.0003-0.01 M) were affected by the soluble starch. These results suggest that the taste enhancing effects of soluble starch on Suc sweetness might depend not only on the taste transduction mechanism, but also on the molecular interaction between Suc and soluble starch.  相似文献   

7.
High intensity sweeteners were evaluated for sweetness and bitternessintensity using time-intensity scaling. Mean intensities of50:50 mixtures as well as the single sweeteners were used tocompute predicted scores which were compared to the observedscores as a means of evaluating additivity in the mixtures.Concentration-dependent effects of subadditivity, additivityand hyperadditivity were observed within some sweetener pairs,but these did not follow any consistent pattern across sweeteners.Synergy, a special case of hyperadditivity evaluated by comparingpredicted to observed scores, was seen in mixtures of aspartameand acesulfame-K at all concentrations. Aspartame/saccharinblends were synergistic only at the lowest concentration tested,despite the structural similarity between acesulfame-K and saccharin.Blends of sucrose/aspartame and acesulfame-K/saccharin did notexhibit synergy. Comparisons based on ratings of initial sweetnessrather than the whole time-intensity curve, reflected previousfindings of synergy in some sweetener pairs.  相似文献   

8.
Upon stimulation with continuously alternating (pulsatile) taste concentrations, humans report higher average taste intensities than for continuous stimulation with the same average tastant concentration. We investigated the effect of the magnitude of concentration changes (concentration contrast) and the effect of taste quality changes (quality contrast) between alternating tastants on sweet taste enhancement. The perceived sweetness intensity increased with the magnitude of the sucrose concentration contrast: The pulsatile stimulus with the highest concentration difference (average sucrose concentration: 60 g/L) was rated as the sweetest in spite of the fact that the gross sucrose concentrations were identical over stimuli. Moreover, this stimulus was rated equally sweet as a continuous reference of 70 g/L sucrose. On alternation of sucrose with the qualitatively different citric acid, sweet taste enhancement remained at the level observed for alternation with water at citric acid concentration levels up to 3 times its detection threshold. Alternation of a sucrose solution with a citric acid solution at 9 times its threshold concentration, resulted in an attenuation of the pulsation-induced enhancement effect. Upon alternation of citric acid pulses at concentrations around the threshold with water intervals only, no taste enhancement was observed compared with continuous citric acid stimuli of the same net concentration. We propose that the magnitude of pulsation-induced taste enhancement is determined by the absolute rather than relative change of tastant concentration. This explains why 1) pulsation-induced sweet taste enhancement is determined by the magnitude of the sucrose pulse-interval contrast and 2) the alteration of citric acid with water does not enhance taste intensity at detection threshold level.  相似文献   

9.
Perceived sweetness of sucrose, aspartame, D-tryptophan and thaumatin in a sour, citric acid background was analyzed in terms of the potency of these compounds relative to sucrose-water combinations. Potencies of the sweeteners were determined from (1) maximum intensity using single value and time-intensity (T-I) measurements and (2) average intensity calculated as the ratio of area under the T-I curve and total perceived time. Stevens' law was applied to sweet responses, either in static or dynamic conditions. It was found that the exponent of the concentration-response function reflected the relative capacity of a compound to sweeten a given food and stressed differences of potency among sweeteners. Aspartame, D-tryptophan and thaumatin exhibited a decrease in sweetness potency relative to sucrose as sweetness increased from 10 to 100% of the full scale of response. Across the entire sweetness range, thaumatin showed the greatest potency but its long persistence time led to differentiate this intense sweetener from the other sweeteners evaluated.  相似文献   

10.
为了解桃果实发育过程中细胞内糖酸的分布、变化规律对果实甜酸风味的影响,采用区室分析方法研究了‘白凤’桃(Prunus persica‘Hakuho’)果实不同发育时期细胞内糖酸组分、含量及其分布对甜酸风味的影响。结果表明,成熟果实中(花后100 d)可溶性糖(蔗糖、葡萄糖、果糖和山梨醇)在液泡、细胞质和细胞间隙中的含量分别为27.3、11.6、9.0 mg/g,有机酸(苹果酸、柠檬酸、奎宁酸和莽草酸)含量为2.09、0.94、0.35 mg/g;未成熟果实中(花后60 d)可溶性糖在液泡、细胞质和细胞间隙中的含量分别为0.97、2.2、2.3 mg/g,有机酸含量为0.25、0.44、0.82 mg/g。‘白凤’桃果肉细胞内不同的糖酸分布对成熟果实的甜酸风味具有显著影响,而对未成熟果实影响较小。成熟果实中糖酸在液泡、细胞质和细胞间隙三者之间的分布差异可能是导致果实甜度变化的主要原因。  相似文献   

11.
The aim of this work was to study the sourness–sweetness interactions in water, white wine and alcoholic environment to interpret sweet/sour perception in low concentrations within the range normally encountered in white wine. Nine trained assessors rated sweetness and sourness intensity in mixtures of fructose (11.1, 25.0 and 38.9 mM) and tartaric acid (pH 3.0, 3.4 and 3.8) in water and wine (experiment 1) or ethanol solutions at 2.0, 4.0 and 12.0% v/v (experiment 2). The range of quantitative responses was larger for sourness than for sweetness in the three media. The global sourness intensity perception in wine mixtures was significantly lower than in water and ethanol mixtures, indicating the effect of other wine components. The suppressive effect of tartaric acid on fructose sweetness was stronger than the suppressive effect of fructose on tartaric acid sourness.  相似文献   

12.
13.
Sowalsky  RA; Noble  AC 《Chemical senses》1998,23(3):343-349
The separate effects of concentration, pH and anion species on intensity of sourness and astringency of organic acids were evaluated. Judges rated sourness and astringency intensity of lactic, malic, tartaric and citric acid solutions at three levels of constant pH varying in normality and at three levels of constant concentration varying in pH. To assess the comparative sourness and astringency of the organic acid anions of study, binary acid solutions matched in pH and titratable acidity were also rated. As pH was decreased in equinormal solutions, both sourness and astringency increased significantly (P < 0.001). By contrast, as the normality of the equi-pH solutions was increased, only sourness demonstrated significant increases (P < 0.001) while astringency remained constant or decreased slightly. At the lowest normality tested, all solutions were more astringent than sour (P < 0.05). Although lactic acid was found to be significantly more sour than citric acid (P < 0.05), no other sourness or astringency differences among the organic acid anions were noted. This study demonstrates for the first time that astringency elicited by acids is a function of pH and not concentration or anion species, and confirms that sourness is independently influenced by concentration, pH and anion species of the acid.   相似文献   

14.
A number of commonly consumed fruits and vegetables (e.g., kiwifruit, pineapple and taro) cause oral irritation, and there is anecdotal evidence that this influences the acceptability of such products. In each of these foods, oral irritation is produced by free crystalline calcium oxalate (raphides). The psychophysics of the mechanical oral irritation caused by raphides was studied in two experiments using a model system based on kiwifruit. In the first experiment, the location of irritant sensations and perceived intensity of the irritation caused by suspensions of raphides were determined using a ten-member trained sensory panel. Stinging and numbing were found to increase significantly with increasing concentrations of raphides. Stinging occurred principally on the tongue as well as in the throat and numbing principally on the tongue. The number of oral areas irritated was shown to increase with raphide concentration and to decrease over a 60-min period. In the second experiment, sugars (fructose, sucrose, glucose and inositol), acids (citric, malic and quinic) and an enzyme (actinidin) were added to the model to examine interactions between these chemical stimuli and the mechanical action of the raphides. Addition of acids to the model was shown to enhance irritation in addition to increasing sourness and suppressing sweetness. The addition of actinidin had no significant effects on irritation or tastes. It is suggested that acidity may aggravate irritation at low raphide concentration, but beyond a critical level of mechanical irritation the presence of acids has little additional effect. These results provide the first detailed data of the psychophysics of mechanical oral irritation in food, and have important implications for producing foods with lower levels of irritation and increased acceptability.  相似文献   

15.
Detection thresholds for NaCl, KCl, sucrose, aspartame, acetic acid, citric acid, caffeine, quinine HCl, monosodium glutamate (MSG) and inosine 5'-monophosphate (IMP) were assessed in 21 young (19-33 years) and 21 elderly (60-75 years) persons by taking the average of six ascending two-alternative forced choice tests. A significant overall effect was found for age, but not for gender. However, an interaction effect of age and gender was found. The older men were less sensitive than the young men and women for acetic acid, sucrose, citric acid, sodium and potassium chloride and IMP. To detect the compound dissolved in water they needed a 1.32 (aspartame) to 5.70 times (IMP) higher concentration than the younger subjects. A significant decline in thresholds with replication was shown. The age effect found could be attributed predominantly to a generic taste loss.  相似文献   

16.
Sweetness-depressing gymnemic acid (G) and sweetness-inducingmiraculin (M) helped determine the extent to which parotid salivaryresponses match behavioral and neural gustatory responses. Parotidflow rates and tastes intensities were obtained from four subjectsfor four sweeteners (before and after G) and for citric acid(CA) before and after M and G. A mixture of 20 mM CA and 10%sucrose was also tested. Although G depressed sweetness forglucose, sucrose, fructose and aspartame, G generally failedto alter parotid responses or depress post-G ratings for bitterintensity. In fact, G markedly elevated parotid responses forthe acid–sucrose mixture. Residual sweetness after G,detected mainly from the posterior tongue, probably contributedonly partially to sustaining post–G parotid responses.We speculate that side tastes from the sweeteners and oral irritationfrom CA in the mixture contributed to elevated flow rates afterG. Sucrose- and M-induced sweetness generally elevated parotidresponses for 20 mM CA. This result agrees with human chordatympani responses after M but differs for sucrose–acidmixtures in that parotid responses approached the calculatedsum of the components. We speculate that non-gustatory inputsmay also affect sweet–sour responses and advise cautionin relating parotid reflexes only to taste judgements. 1Present address: Department of Biological Sciences, San JoseState University, San Jose, CA 95192, USA  相似文献   

17.
The Effect of Sweeteners on Perceived Viscosity   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
Two different sweeteners, sucrose and aspartame, were matchedin perceived sweetness intensity. These solutions were thickenedwith carboxymethylcellulose to six different viscosity levels.Sucrose and aspartame appeared to decrease perceived viscosityof the solutions at a specific sweetener concentration, at allviscosity levels. However, in a second similar experiment withthree viscosity levels and seven sucrose concentrations no effectof sucrose concentration on perceived viscosity was found. Reasonsfor these conflicting results are discussed. No definite conclusionsabout the effect of sweeteners on perceived viscosity can asyet be drawn. Chem. Senses 20: 441–450, 1995.  相似文献   

18.
A two-stage process of submerged citric acid fermentation with replacement of growth medium by fermentation medium has been worked out. The optimum composition of mineral nutrients and pH of the fermentation medium of the second stage of the process were determined. An addition of 0.5 g/l of NH4NO3 as nitrogen source and 0.1 g/l of MgSO4-7H2O as magnesium source ensured effective conversion of sucrose to citric acid. An addition to KH2PO4, on the other hand, was definitely unfavourable as it considerably reduced the product yield. The medium for the second stage of fermentation should be acidified to about pH 2.2, while the water used for washing the mycelium from the remains of the growth medium should have a pH of 2.5--3.5. Under these conditions, with an initial sucrose concentration of 100 g/l, after 132 hr fermentation at 26 degrees up to 90 g/l of citric acid was obtained, which corresponds to a productivity of over 16 g/l. day. The highest activity for citric acid formation was found in three- or four-day-old mycelium.  相似文献   

19.
Two-bottle preference tests have been applied to 70 (sub-) speciesof the order of Primates and, for comparison, to the tree shrew(Tupaia belangeri) to determine their responses to aspartame(APM), the first known sweet-tasting dipeptide which has toman a sweetness potency of about 200 times that of sucrose.It was found that only the Cercopithecidae, the Hylobatidaeand the Pongidae respond like man to this dipeptide and preferit to water. The other primates tested to date, show no responseto this sweetener. From a phylogenetic point of view, we notethat APM shows species specificity similar to thaumatin. Thus,a clear dichotomy exists within the order of Primates with respectto both thaumatin and APM. The results here illustrate oncemore the gustatory diversity among primates and are a compellingargument for the existence of different sweet taste receptorsor recognition sites in primate species.  相似文献   

20.
The objective of this study was to compare four palate cleansing strategies (crackers, carrots, water and nothing) for their effectiveness in evaluating the sourness of a fruit-flavored beverage. Forty panelists rated the sourness intensity of five orange drink samples that varied in citric acid content. Each subject participated in four sessions, one for each palate cleanser. As expected, sourness increased with increasing citric acid content. There were no significant differences between any of the palate cleansers for the discrimination among the sourness levels. Sourness neither increased (buildup) nor decreased (adaptation) during the session with any of the palate cleansing strategies. Using carrots or water provided similar discrimination to crackers, but the panelists rated the sourness higher after using carrots or water as a palate cleanser than after using the cracker.

PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS


Palate cleansers are widely used during sensory evaluation of foods. We suspect that part of the reason people use them is because they lower the intensity of residual flavors, leading people to believe the flavors have been removed from their mouth. Our results show that residual cleansers can produce changes in the flavor of subsequently tasted products. Because the nature and extent of these changes will logically depend on the specific product and the specific palate cleansing strategy, experiments need to be conducted to provide these measures.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号