共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
《Anthrozo?s》2013,26(2-3):84-89
ABSTRACTTherapy using animals is now a widespread phenomenon, but some authorities have argued that there is insufficient evidence either to justify or to guide animal-assisted therapy. This reflects a paucity of adequate quantitative methodology for investigations in this area. The little research that has been carried out suggests that animal presence may increase general social behavior. Our study aimed to develop a methodology to investigate the effects of a dog on the behavior of children with severe learning disabilities. A repeated measures design was used to observe eight children with Down's syndrome in controlled sessions, under the direction of an adult. Each session consisted of seven minutes of interaction with a real dog, and seven minutes with an imitation dog. Differences in visual attending, and verbal and non-verbal initiation and response behaviors indicated that the real dog provided a more sustained focus for positive and co-operative interactions with itself and the adult, than did the imitation dog. 相似文献
4.
5.
6.
Takeo Fujiwara Junko Yagi Hiroaki Homma Hirobumi Mashiko Keizo Nagao Makiko Okuyama for the Great East Japan Earthquake Follow-up for Children Study Team 《PloS one》2014,9(10)
Background
On March 11, 2011, a massive undersea earthquake and tsunami struck East Japan. Few studies have investigated the impact of exposure to a natural disaster on preschool children. We investigated the association of trauma experiences during the Great East Japan Earthquake on clinically significant behavior problems among preschool children 2 years after the earthquake.Method
Participants were children who were exposed to the 2011 disaster at preschool age (affected area, n = 178; unaffected area, n = 82). Data were collected from September 2012 to June 2013 (around 2 years after the earthquake), thus participants were aged 5 to 8 years when assessed. Severe trauma exposures related to the earthquake (e.g., loss of family members) were assessed by interview, and trauma events in the physical environment related to the earthquake (e.g. housing damage), and other trauma exposure before the earthquake, were assessed by questionnaire. Behavior problems were assessed by caregivers using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), which encompasses internalizing, externalizing, and total problems. Children who exceeded clinical cut-off of the CBCL were defined as having clinically significant behavior problems.Results
Rates of internalizing, externalizing, and total problems in the affected area were 27.7%, 21.2%, and 25.9%, respectively. The rate ratio suggests that children who lost distant relatives or friends were 2.36 times more likely to have internalizing behavior problems (47.6% vs. 20.2%, 95% CI: 1.10–5.07). Other trauma experiences before the earthquake also showed significant positive association with internalizing, externalizing, and total behavior problems, which were not observed in the unaffected area.Conclusions
One in four children still had behavior problems even 2 years after the Great East Japan Earthquake. Children who had other trauma experiences before the earthquake were more likely to have behavior problems. These data will be useful for developing future interventions in child mental health after a natural disaster. 相似文献7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
Essi Viding Thomas S. Price Sara R. Jaffee Maciej Trzaskowski Oliver S. P. Davis Emma L. Meaburn Claire M. A. Haworth Robert Plomin 《PloS one》2013,8(7)
Callous-unemotional behavior (CU) is currently under consideration as a subtyping index for conduct disorder diagnosis. Twin studies routinely estimate the heritability of CU as greater than 50%. It is now possible to estimate genetic influence using DNA alone from samples of unrelated individuals, not relying on the assumptions of the twin method. Here we use this new DNA method (implemented in a software package called Genome-wide Complex Trait Analysis, GCTA) for the first time to estimate genetic influence on CU. We also report the first genome-wide association (GWA) study of CU as a quantitative trait. We compare these DNA results to those from twin analyses using the same measure and the same community sample of 2,930 children rated by their teachers at ages 7, 9 and 12. GCTA estimates of heritability were near zero, even though twin analysis of CU in this sample confirmed the high heritability of CU reported in the literature, and even though GCTA estimates of heritability were substantial for cognitive and anthropological traits in this sample. No significant associations were found in GWA analysis, which, like GCTA, only detects additive effects of common DNA variants. The phrase ‘missing heritability’ was coined to refer to the gap between variance associated with DNA variants identified in GWA studies versus twin study heritability. However, GCTA heritability, not twin study heritability, is the ceiling for GWA studies because both GCTA and GWA are limited to the overall additive effects of common DNA variants, whereas twin studies are not. This GCTA ceiling is very low for CU in our study, despite its high twin study heritability estimate. The gap between GCTA and twin study heritabilities will make it challenging to identify genes responsible for the heritability of CU. 相似文献
13.
EEG spectral characteristics were studied in two age groups (7–8.5 and 8.5–10 years) of mentally healthy children and children with learning problems at rest and during performance of a Raven test. It was shown that slow frequencies are more pronounced in the EEG of 7- to 8.5-year-old children with learning problems than in EEG of healthy children of the same age group. An immature form of EEG activation, i.e., an increase not only in the but also in the frequencies during activity, was characteristic of these children. The reaction of the activation of the definitive type develops between the 8.5–10 years of age. This reaction is correlated with an increase in the efficiency of the sensory perceptive and sensorimotor activity. The distinctive feature of children with learning problems between 8.5–10 years of age is a greater expression of slow frequencies in the baseline EEG of the frontal (in particular, left frontal) areas of the cortex. The obtained results are considered as a reflection of a retardation of the functional maturation of the brain structures responsible for the deficit of involuntary and voluntary attention and the disorder of a systemic organization of perception and analytical–synthetic brain activity as compared to the normal age characteristics. Possible neurophysiological mechanisms responsible for learning problems in junior schoolchildren are discussed on the basis of the obtained results and evidence from the literature. 相似文献
14.
We often learn and recall long sequences in smaller segments, such as a phone number 858 534 22 30 memorized as four segments. Behavioral experiments suggest that humans and some animals employ this strategy of breaking down cognitive or behavioral sequences into chunks in a wide variety of tasks, but the dynamical principles of how this is achieved remains unknown. Here, we study the temporal dynamics of chunking for learning cognitive sequences in a chunking representation using a dynamical model of competing modes arranged to evoke hierarchical Winnerless Competition (WLC) dynamics. Sequential memory is represented as trajectories along a chain of metastable fixed points at each level of the hierarchy, and bistable Hebbian dynamics enables the learning of such trajectories in an unsupervised fashion. Using computer simulations, we demonstrate the learning of a chunking representation of sequences and their robust recall. During learning, the dynamics associates a set of modes to each information-carrying item in the sequence and encodes their relative order. During recall, hierarchical WLC guarantees the robustness of the sequence order when the sequence is not too long. The resulting patterns of activities share several features observed in behavioral experiments, such as the pauses between boundaries of chunks, their size and their duration. Failures in learning chunking sequences provide new insights into the dynamical causes of neurological disorders such as Parkinson’s disease and Schizophrenia. 相似文献
15.
16.
Background
The success of social insects can be in part attributed to their division of labor, which has been explained by a response threshold model. This model posits that individuals differ in their response thresholds to task-associated stimuli, so that individuals with lower thresholds specialize in this task. This model is at odds with findings on honeybee behavior as nectar and pollen foragers exhibit different responsiveness to sucrose, with nectar foragers having higher response thresholds to sucrose concentration. Moreover, it has been suggested that sucrose responsiveness correlates with responsiveness to most if not all other stimuli. If this is the case, explaining task specialization and the origins of division of labor on the basis of differences in response thresholds is difficult.Methodology
To compare responsiveness to stimuli presenting clear-cut differences in hedonic value and behavioral contexts, we measured appetitive and aversive responsiveness in the same bees in the laboratory. We quantified proboscis extension responses to increasing sucrose concentrations and sting extension responses to electric shocks of increasing voltage. We analyzed the relationship between aversive responsiveness and aversive olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex, and determined how this relationship relates to division of labor.Principal Findings
Sucrose and shock responsiveness measured in the same bees did not correlate, thus suggesting that they correspond to independent behavioral syndromes, a foraging and a defensive one. Bees which were more responsive to shock learned and memorized better aversive associations. Finally, guards were less responsive than nectar foragers to electric shocks, exhibiting higher tolerance to low voltage shocks. Consequently, foragers, which are more sensitive, were the ones learning and memorizing better in aversive conditioning.Conclusions
Our results constitute the first integrative study on how aversive responsiveness affects learning, memory and social organization in honeybees. We suggest that parallel behavioral modules (e.g. appetitive, aversive) coexist within each individual bee and determine its tendency to adopt a given task. This conclusion, which is at odds with a simple threshold model, should open new opportunities for exploring the division of labor in social insects. 相似文献17.
《Journal of Russian & East European Psychology》2013,51(4):65-116
In the literature on the psychology of the preschool-age child one finds it frequently stated that the preschool period is when the first signs of the development of volition [or the will] appear (Ushinsky [19], Sikorskii [17], Kornilov [6], and others). Empirical studies on the development of specific processes in the child, e.g., perception, memory, speech, and movement, have shown that the principal change these processes undergo in the three-to-seven-year-old child is that they become voluntary and subject to regulation.* Empirical studies have also been made, of course, on the specific problem of the development of voluntary behavior in the child (Ivanov [4], Gurevich [3], Gorbacheva [2]). 相似文献
18.
《Journal of Russian & East European Psychology》2013,51(3):92-96
On 6-9 May 1985, an international conference on "Children in the information age," organized by the State Committee on Science and Technical Progress of the People's Republic of Bulgaria, was held in Varna, Bulgaria. Among the participants were [representatives of] the International Institute of Applied Systems Analysis (Austria), UNESCO, the International Fund of Ludmila Zhivkova (People's Republic of Bulgaria), and the World Health Organization. The purpose of the conference was to exchange experience on current problems in the computerization of learning, computer literacy, and information sciences in schools. About two hundred scientists from 45 different countries took part in the conference. The results of studies of methodological, psychological, pedagogical, physiological, and technical aspects of the problem of computerization of learning were discussed at plenary and workshop sessions. In addition, a special exhibit acquainted people with modern computer teaching devices, microprocessor teaching technology, dialogue teaching systems, logical and algorithmic games, programs, etc. 相似文献
19.
Sebastian M. Frank Susanne Bründl Ulrike I. Frank Yuka Sasaki Mark W. Greenlee Takeo Watanabe 《Current biology : CB》2021,31(2):427-432.e5
- Download : Download high-res image (157KB)
- Download : Download full-size image
20.
周海霞 《现代生物医学进展》2007,7(11):1738-1740
学习障碍是学龄儿童较常见的问题之一。近年来,西方学者在学习障碍领域开始引人弹性发展理论,取得了大量的研究成果。本文就弹性发展理论的内容、影响学习障碍儿童弹性发展的保护性因素和危险性因素、弹性发展的作用机制以及学习障碍儿童的弹性干预作了简要的介绍,以期对我国的学习障碍研究和干预有所启发。 相似文献