共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The intensity of stone collection and stone theft by breeding Chinstrap penguins was measured, and estimations made of the number of stones per nest in large (> 400 nests) and small subcolonies (< 50 nests) in the large Vapour Col colony on Deception Island, South Shetland Islands. Stone availability was significantly higher both inside and outside small subcolonies. Penguins carried stones to the nest at the same rate in large and small subcolonies, but stole more intensively in large subcolonies. Stones obtained by theft were significantly larger than those collected elsewhere. When stone availability was increased experimentally, individuals of large subcolonies collected more intensively than control individuals in large and small subcolonies, and stole significantly less than control individuals in large subcolonies, and as much as individuals in small subcolonies. The greater theft pressure in large subcolonies was accompanied by more aggressive defence by nest owners and by reduced succession stealing. However, the reduced availability of stones on the ground near large subcolonies led to a significantly lower number of stones per nest than in small subcolonies. These results are interpreted in the light of the geometric effects of breeding group size (perimeter to surface ratio) on stone accessibility. 相似文献
2.
Every year, shortly after the emancipation of chicks at our study colony (Deception Island, South Shetlands), hundreds of carcasses of presumably starved Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica chicks are washed on the shore. In 1997 we measured the flippers of fresh carcasses and compared their lengths with those of live chicks about to become independent. There was a highly significant difference of 6.5 mm between both distributions, which suggests strong directional phenotypic selection on skeletal size operating through its association with body reserves at independence. Given that heritabilities of flipper length and body weight measured on 36 families are 0.73(± 0.32) and 0.075(± 0.081), and that both characters show a genetic correlation of 0.44(± 0.14), we can expect an evolutionary response to this selection episode. Assuming that the target of selection is weight at emancipation (heavier chicks carry proportionally larger reserves), and that flipper length changes as a consequence of its genetic correlation with weight, we can predict a response of 1.32–2.87 mm or 0.23–0.51 standard deviation units for flipper length. This substantial evolutionary response may be countered by other selective pressures affecting other life stages of these birds. Selection on reserve storage capacity at independence may affect morphological traits also in other species. 相似文献
3.
We examined if laying intervals and hatching asynchronies are related in a chinstrap penguin (Pygoscelis antarctica) population in the South Orkneys. The lack of association between the two variables, as well as data on brood patch development, indicated that incubation did not begin immediately after the first egg was laid. This suggests that longer laying intervals may be compensated by longer delays in the onset of full incubation. Hatching asynchrony increased with within-clutch egg size asymmetry, decreased with breeding date, and was related to the laying order of the eggs according to size. However, only egg-size asymmetry remained significant when controlling for the other variables. We conclude that more asymmetric clutches were more asynchronous, although a large part of the variation in hatching asynchrony remains unexplained. 相似文献
4.
Stenning MJ 《Trends in ecology & evolution》1996,11(6):243-246
Hatching asynchrony (extended hatching period) is apparently ubiquitous among altricial birds, and may represent a striking example of adaptive family planning. Research has focused on evaluating various benefits to resulting partial brood loss. Current conclusions fall into three major categories: that hatching asynchrony is (1) an adaptation to food availability, (2) a means of saving time, ultimately to increase lifetime reproductive success, or (3) a maladaptation. Almost every study develops a new explanation or qualifies an old one. Either most of them are wrong, or hatching asynchrony is an example of convergent evolution resulting in a behavioural trait serving many functions. 相似文献
5.
We analysed genetic relatedness through DNA-fingerprinting in 38 two-chick families of Chinstrap Penguins Pygoscelis antarctica in the Vapour Col colony on Deception Island, Antarctica. Band-sharing coefficients between males and chicks in their nests, females and chicks in their nests and brood mates were very close to 0.50 and were not significantly different. Presumably unrelated pair mates showed much lower band-sharing coefficients (0.12), which differed significantly from those between putative parents and their chicks and between presumed siblings. No case of extra-pair paternity was detected according to the presence of novel bands or to band-sharing coefficients. Monogamy in the study population is not only social but also genetic. 相似文献
6.
Yoshihisa Mori 《Journal of Ethology》1997,15(1):9-15
Diving behavior of 2 breeding Chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) was studied focusing first and primarily on dive bouts rather than dives themselves. Analysis of dive bout organization
revealed (1) though there are differences between solitary dives and dive bouts in dive duration and dive depth, the first
dives of dive bouts do not differ from solitary dives in the dive parameters, (2) mean dive duration during bout correlates
positively to both mean dive depth during bout and mean surface interval during bout, while number of dives during bout negatively
correlates to both cost (consumed energy) and duration of a dive cycle during bout. These findings suggest the following possibilities
on foraging behavior of penguins: (1) their decision to repeat diving depends on the result of the first dive at a site, and
the first dives of bouts would tend to be searching or evaluating dives though they would be also successful foraging dives,
(2) they repeat diving at a foraging patch until foraging efficiency decrease to a threshold of diminishing returns. 相似文献
7.
Breeding time, health and immune response in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Health status and immunocompetence have been proposed as important factors affecting individual variation in the attainment
of breeding condition in birds. We studied individual variation in serological variables indicating health status (blood sedimentation
rate, haematocrit, `buffy coat' layer, proportions of different types of leucocytes) in two groups of breeding chinstrap penguins
Pygoscelis antarctica with breeding dates 9 days apart. We sampled these individuals shortly after hatching of their young and at the end of the
chick-raising period. A group of failed breeders was also sampled. Birds of both sexes were included. We also measured the
T-cell-mediated immune response as indicated by an in vivo hypersensitivity response to an intradermal injection of a mitogen
(phytohaemagglutinin) in early and late breeders. Sex had no significant effect on most variables. Late breeders had poorer
health (more leucocytes, especially heterophils and lymphocytes) and a lower T-cell-mediated immune response than early breeders.
Failed breeders were more similar to late than to early breeders. Early breeders suffered a decline in health status throughout
the chick-raising period. The impact of pathogens on variation in life history traits in avian populations may be important
even in extreme Antarctic environments.
Received: 18 December 1997 / Accepted: 1 March 1998 相似文献
8.
9.
The onset of incubation before the end of laying imposes asynchrony at hatching and, therefore, a size hierarchy in the brood. It has been argued that hatching asynchrony might be a strategy to improve reproductive output in terms of quality or quantity of offspring. However, little is known about the mediating effect of hatching asynchrony on offspring quality when brood reduction occurs. Here, we investigate the relationship between phenotypic quality and hatching asynchrony in Common Kestrel Falco tinnunculus nestlings in Spain. Hatching asynchrony did not increase breeding success or nestling quality. Furthermore, hatching asynchrony and brood reduction had different effects on nestlings’ phytohaematogglutinin (PHA)‐mediated immune response and nestling growth. In asynchronous and reduced broods (in which at least one nestling died), nestlings showed a stronger PHA‐mediated immune response and tended to have a smaller body size compared with nestlings raised in synchronous and reduced broods. When brood reduction occurred in broods hatched synchronously, there was no effect on nestling size, but nestlings had a relatively poor PHA‐mediated immune response compared with nestlings raised in asynchronous and reduced broods. We suggest that resources for growth can be directed to immune function only in asynchronously hatched broods, resulting in improved nestling quality, as suggested by their immune response. We also found that males produced a greater PHA‐mediated immune response than females only in brood‐reduced nests without any effect on nestling size or condition, suggesting that females may trade off immune activities and body condition, size or weight. Overall, our results suggest that hatching pattern and brood reduction may mediate resource allocation to different fitness traits. They also highlight that the resolution of immune‐related trade‐offs when brood reduction occurs may differ between male and female nestlings. 相似文献
10.
We report an observational study of aggressive behaviour by adults during the post-guard phase in the chinstrap penguin. The study was carried out in one subcolony where all the individuals (breeders, chicks and failed breeders) were banded, as well as in nine other subcolonies where individuals were not identifiable. Breeding adults were more aggressive towards unrelated chicks when they were feeding their own chicks than in other contexts. Chicks were also attacked at high rates by adults that did not belong to their own subcolonies. The evidence suggests that some aggressions to chicks by adults may function to avoid interference from unrelated chicks during food transfers, but our data also show that aggressive interactions are common in contexts other than chick feeding. However, aggression by adults did not show any clear spatial directionality or purpose of shepherding the chicks, and these tended not to move in any particular direction when attacked. 相似文献
11.
Robert A. Aldredge Raoul K. Boughton Michelle A. Rensel Stephan J. Schoech Reed Bowman 《Oecologia》2014,174(1):77-85
In birds, hatching failure is pervasive and incurs an energetic and reproductive cost to breeding individuals. The egg viability hypothesis posits that exposure to warm temperatures prior to incubation decreases viability of early laid eggs and predicts that females in warm environments minimize hatching failure by beginning incubation earlier in the laying period, laying smaller clutches, or both. However, beginning incubation prior to clutch completion may incur a cost by increasing hatching asynchrony and possibly brood reduction. We examined whether Florida scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) began incubation earlier relative to clutch completion when laying larger clutches or when ambient temperatures increased, and whether variation in incubation onset influenced subsequent patterns of hatching asynchrony and brood reduction. We compared these patterns between a suburban and wildland site because site-specific differences in hatching failure match a priori predictions of the egg viability hypothesis. Females at both sites began incubation earlier relative to clutch completion when laying larger clutches and as ambient temperatures increased. Incubation onset was correlated with patterns of hatching asynchrony at both sites; however, brood reduction increased only in the suburbs, where nestling food is limiting, and only during the late nestling period. Hatching asynchrony may be an unintended consequence of beginning incubation early to minimize hatching failure of early laid eggs. Food limitation in the suburbs appears to result in increased brood reduction in large clutches that hatch asynchronously. Therefore, site-specific rates of brood reduction may be a consequence of asynchronous hatching patterns that result from parental effort to minimize hatching failure in first-laid eggs. This illustrates how anthropogenic change, such as urbanization, can lead to loss of fitness when animals use behavioral strategies intended to maximize fitness in natural landscapes. 相似文献
12.
All parental hosts of heterospecific brood parasites must paythe cost of rearing non-kin. Previous research on nest parasitismby brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) concluded that competitivesuperiority of the typically more intensively begging and largercowbird chick leads to preferential feeding by foster parentsand causes a reduction in the hosts' own brood. The larger sizeof cowbird nestlings can be the result of at least two causes:(1) cowbirds preferentially parasitize species with smallernestlings and lower growth rates; and/or (2) cowbirds hatchearlier than hosts. I estimated the cost of cowbird parasitismfor each of 29 species by calculating the difference betweenhosts' published brood sizes in nonparasitized and parasitizednests and using clutch size to standardize values. In this analysis,greater incubation length and lower adult mass, surrogate measuresof the hatching asynchrony and size difference between parasiteand hosts, were both related to greater costs of cowbird parasitismwithout bias owing to phylogeny. To establish causality, I manipulatedclutch contents of eastern phoebes (Sayornis phoebe) and examinedwhether earlier hatching by a single cowbird or phoebe egg reducesthe size of the rest of the original host brood. As predicted,greater hatching asynchrony increased the proportion of theoriginal phoebe brood that was lost. This measure of the costof parasitism was partially owing to increased hatching failureof the original eggs in asynchronous broods but was not at allrelated to the size differences of older and younger conspecificnestmates. However, proportional brood loss owing to an earlierhatching conspecific was consistently smaller than brood lossowing to asynchronous cowbirds in both naturally and experimentallyparasitized phoebe nests. These results imply that althoughhatching asynchrony is an important cause of the reduction ofhost broods in parasitized clutches, competitive features ofcowbird nestlings remain necessary to explain the full extentof hosts' reproductive costs caused by interspecific brood parasitism. 相似文献
13.
Although the majority of chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) inhabit the Scotia Arc, a minuscule population of ten breeding pairs has been reported several times from Chinstrap Islet, in the Balleny Islands, 5,000 km distant on the opposite side of Antarctica. An aerial photographic census (December 2000) reveals that the overall penguin population of the Balleny Islands has declined by 8% since the last census 16 years ago, while the combined populations of the two largest colonies, Chinstrap Islet and Sabrina Islet, have increased by 11%. A ground visit confirmed for the first time that chinstrap penguins also breed on Sabrina Islet, occupying 20-24 nests on the margins of the Adélie colony (3,790 pairs in all). The small size and anomalous location of the Balleny Islands chinstrap population clearly warrant further study and the strongest protective measures. 相似文献
14.
No haematoza were detected in 40 adult and 58 nestling wild chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) from Antarctica examined by blood smear.
Received: 27 November 1996 / Accepted: 24 March 1997 相似文献
15.
16.
17.
Meal size of chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) was estimated through weighing adult breeders before and after food transfer during the crèche phase. Mean meal size delivered
by each parent was 630.2 ± 178.3 g and was independent of breeding date, number of chicks and sex. Only body size as estimated
by flipper length had a significant effect on meal size, suggesting an advantage of large body size which allows maximization
of food carried per visit.
Received: 22 September 1997 / Accepted: 16 December 1997 相似文献
18.
J. Vinuela J. Moreno L. M. Carrascal J. J. Sanz J. A. Amat # M. Ferrer # J. Belliure J. Cuervo # 《Journal of Zoology》1996,240(1):51-58
We studied the effect of hatching date on breeding performance (chick growth and mortality) and phenology (creching and fledging ages) of the chinstrap penguin during three years. The year affected every variable considered, probably due to pack-ice persistence and food availability differences between years. Hatching date had slight or no effect on mortality and early growth, but was negatively correlated with creching age, which, in turn, was positively related to final size. The decision to leave the chicks unguarded does not seem to be based on the condition of the chicks, but on that of adults. Fledging age was negatively correlated with hatching date, and this effect was more marked in the year with poor growth performance. Given the short time available for breeding in Antarctica, there must be conflicting pressures between investing in feeding chicks and advancing the period of premoult resource storage, this explaining the strong relationship between hatching dates and subsequent phenological events (creching and fledging). In this kind of study, it may be important to remove the effect of inter-year variation before assessing the possible effects of other variables. 相似文献
19.
Egg temperature and initial brood patch area determine hatching asynchrony in Magellanic penguin Spheniscus magellanicus 下载免费PDF全文
In birds, the adaptive significance of hatching asynchrony has been under debate for many years and the parental effects on hatching asynchrony have been largely assumed but not often tested. Some authors suggest that hatching asynchrony depends on the incubation onset and many factors have been shown to influence hatching asynchrony in different species. Our objective was to analyze the exact timing of the onset of incubation and if this affects hatching asynchrony; and, in addition, which other factors (brood patch development, incubation position, adult body condition, intra‐clutch egg dimorphism, laying date and year) affect hatching asynchrony in Magellanic penguins Spheniscus magellanicus. We first estimated the eggshell temperature at which embryo development starts, with a non‐destructive and novel method. We then recorded individual egg temperatures in 61 nests during incubation, and related them, and other breeding parameters, to hatching asynchrony. We also observed incubation positions in 307 nests. We found a significant positive relationship between hatching asynchrony and the temperature that the first‐laid egg experienced during egg laying and between hatching asynchrony and the initial brood patch area. We also found a negative relationship between hatching asynchrony and the difference in temperature between second and first‐laid eggs within a clutch, measured after the egg‐laying period was finished. We ruled out position of the eggs during incubation, adult body condition, egg volume, laying date, and study year as factors influencing hatching asynchrony. The egg temperature during laying and the difference in temperature between eggs of a clutch are determinants of hatching asynchrony in Magellanic penguins. 相似文献
20.
G. S. LlSHMAN 《Ibis》1985,127(1):84-99
Adélie Penguins and Chinstrap Penguins breed in abundance at Signy Island, South Orkney Islands, which is near the northern breeding limit of the former but in the centre of the latter's breeding range. During the 1980–81 and 1981–82 breeding seasons daily checks were made to record laying date and interval between laying, egg weight and volume, incubation period and shifts by each parent, hatching date and interval between hatching, the date on which the young first creched and their age at the time, and fledging date and age. This was done for the eggs and chicks from 60 marked nests of each species. Adelies started breeding about one month before Chinstraps and showed other adaptations which may relate to early breeding and probably to the presence of ice around the breeding site. These adaptations included longer incubation shifts and a heavier and larger first egg which may promote more rapid brood reduction. Chinstraps had shorter incubation shifts and both eggs had similar weights and volumes. Although the later breeding of Chinstraps meant that chick rearing coincided with the availability of a better food source than that utilized by Adelies, Chinstraps were unable to cope with persistent heavy ice conditions in 1980–81 and were no more successful than Adelies in 1981–82. All previous breeding data for both species are reviewed and it is suggested that early breeding by Adelies may be a consequence of competition for food with Chinstraps but, by retaining adaptations to harsh environmental conditions, Adelies are well able to breed successfully. Conversely, Chinstrap Penguins appear mainly adapted to milder environmental conditions and it is unlikely that they could compete successfully with Adelie Penguins at breeding sites on the Antarctic Continent. 相似文献