共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The intensity of stone collection and stone theft by breeding Chinstrap penguins was measured, and estimations made of the number of stones per nest in large (> 400 nests) and small subcolonies (< 50 nests) in the large Vapour Col colony on Deception Island, South Shetland Islands. Stone availability was significantly higher both inside and outside small subcolonies. Penguins carried stones to the nest at the same rate in large and small subcolonies, but stole more intensively in large subcolonies. Stones obtained by theft were significantly larger than those collected elsewhere. When stone availability was increased experimentally, individuals of large subcolonies collected more intensively than control individuals in large and small subcolonies, and stole significantly less than control individuals in large subcolonies, and as much as individuals in small subcolonies. The greater theft pressure in large subcolonies was accompanied by more aggressive defence by nest owners and by reduced succession stealing. However, the reduced availability of stones on the ground near large subcolonies led to a significantly lower number of stones per nest than in small subcolonies. These results are interpreted in the light of the geometric effects of breeding group size (perimeter to surface ratio) on stone accessibility. 相似文献
2.
Every year, shortly after the emancipation of chicks at our study colony (Deception Island, South Shetlands), hundreds of carcasses of presumably starved Chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica chicks are washed on the shore. In 1997 we measured the flippers of fresh carcasses and compared their lengths with those of live chicks about to become independent. There was a highly significant difference of 6.5 mm between both distributions, which suggests strong directional phenotypic selection on skeletal size operating through its association with body reserves at independence. Given that heritabilities of flipper length and body weight measured on 36 families are 0.73(± 0.32) and 0.075(± 0.081), and that both characters show a genetic correlation of 0.44(± 0.14), we can expect an evolutionary response to this selection episode. Assuming that the target of selection is weight at emancipation (heavier chicks carry proportionally larger reserves), and that flipper length changes as a consequence of its genetic correlation with weight, we can predict a response of 1.32–2.87 mm or 0.23–0.51 standard deviation units for flipper length. This substantial evolutionary response may be countered by other selective pressures affecting other life stages of these birds. Selection on reserve storage capacity at independence may affect morphological traits also in other species. 相似文献
3.
We examined if laying intervals and hatching asynchronies are related in a chinstrap penguin (Pygoscelis antarctica) population in the South Orkneys. The lack of association between the two variables, as well as data on brood patch development, indicated that incubation did not begin immediately after the first egg was laid. This suggests that longer laying intervals may be compensated by longer delays in the onset of full incubation. Hatching asynchrony increased with within-clutch egg size asymmetry, decreased with breeding date, and was related to the laying order of the eggs according to size. However, only egg-size asymmetry remained significant when controlling for the other variables. We conclude that more asymmetric clutches were more asynchronous, although a large part of the variation in hatching asynchrony remains unexplained. 相似文献
4.
Stenning MJ 《Trends in ecology & evolution》1996,11(6):243-246
Hatching asynchrony (extended hatching period) is apparently ubiquitous among altricial birds, and may represent a striking example of adaptive family planning. Research has focused on evaluating various benefits to resulting partial brood loss. Current conclusions fall into three major categories: that hatching asynchrony is (1) an adaptation to food availability, (2) a means of saving time, ultimately to increase lifetime reproductive success, or (3) a maladaptation. Almost every study develops a new explanation or qualifies an old one. Either most of them are wrong, or hatching asynchrony is an example of convergent evolution resulting in a behavioural trait serving many functions. 相似文献
5.
We analysed genetic relatedness through DNA-fingerprinting in 38 two-chick families of Chinstrap Penguins Pygoscelis antarctica in the Vapour Col colony on Deception Island, Antarctica. Band-sharing coefficients between males and chicks in their nests, females and chicks in their nests and brood mates were very close to 0.50 and were not significantly different. Presumably unrelated pair mates showed much lower band-sharing coefficients (0.12), which differed significantly from those between putative parents and their chicks and between presumed siblings. No case of extra-pair paternity was detected according to the presence of novel bands or to band-sharing coefficients. Monogamy in the study population is not only social but also genetic. 相似文献
6.
Yoshihisa Mori 《Journal of Ethology》1997,15(1):9-15
Diving behavior of 2 breeding Chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) was studied focusing first and primarily on dive bouts rather than dives themselves. Analysis of dive bout organization
revealed (1) though there are differences between solitary dives and dive bouts in dive duration and dive depth, the first
dives of dive bouts do not differ from solitary dives in the dive parameters, (2) mean dive duration during bout correlates
positively to both mean dive depth during bout and mean surface interval during bout, while number of dives during bout negatively
correlates to both cost (consumed energy) and duration of a dive cycle during bout. These findings suggest the following possibilities
on foraging behavior of penguins: (1) their decision to repeat diving depends on the result of the first dive at a site, and
the first dives of bouts would tend to be searching or evaluating dives though they would be also successful foraging dives,
(2) they repeat diving at a foraging patch until foraging efficiency decrease to a threshold of diminishing returns. 相似文献
7.
Breeding time, health and immune response in the chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Health status and immunocompetence have been proposed as important factors affecting individual variation in the attainment
of breeding condition in birds. We studied individual variation in serological variables indicating health status (blood sedimentation
rate, haematocrit, `buffy coat' layer, proportions of different types of leucocytes) in two groups of breeding chinstrap penguins
Pygoscelis antarctica with breeding dates 9 days apart. We sampled these individuals shortly after hatching of their young and at the end of the
chick-raising period. A group of failed breeders was also sampled. Birds of both sexes were included. We also measured the
T-cell-mediated immune response as indicated by an in vivo hypersensitivity response to an intradermal injection of a mitogen
(phytohaemagglutinin) in early and late breeders. Sex had no significant effect on most variables. Late breeders had poorer
health (more leucocytes, especially heterophils and lymphocytes) and a lower T-cell-mediated immune response than early breeders.
Failed breeders were more similar to late than to early breeders. Early breeders suffered a decline in health status throughout
the chick-raising period. The impact of pathogens on variation in life history traits in avian populations may be important
even in extreme Antarctic environments.
Received: 18 December 1997 / Accepted: 1 March 1998 相似文献
8.
9.
We report an observational study of aggressive behaviour by adults during the post-guard phase in the chinstrap penguin. The study was carried out in one subcolony where all the individuals (breeders, chicks and failed breeders) were banded, as well as in nine other subcolonies where individuals were not identifiable. Breeding adults were more aggressive towards unrelated chicks when they were feeding their own chicks than in other contexts. Chicks were also attacked at high rates by adults that did not belong to their own subcolonies. The evidence suggests that some aggressions to chicks by adults may function to avoid interference from unrelated chicks during food transfers, but our data also show that aggressive interactions are common in contexts other than chick feeding. However, aggression by adults did not show any clear spatial directionality or purpose of shepherding the chicks, and these tended not to move in any particular direction when attacked. 相似文献
10.
All parental hosts of heterospecific brood parasites must paythe cost of rearing non-kin. Previous research on nest parasitismby brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) concluded that competitivesuperiority of the typically more intensively begging and largercowbird chick leads to preferential feeding by foster parentsand causes a reduction in the hosts' own brood. The larger sizeof cowbird nestlings can be the result of at least two causes:(1) cowbirds preferentially parasitize species with smallernestlings and lower growth rates; and/or (2) cowbirds hatchearlier than hosts. I estimated the cost of cowbird parasitismfor each of 29 species by calculating the difference betweenhosts' published brood sizes in nonparasitized and parasitizednests and using clutch size to standardize values. In this analysis,greater incubation length and lower adult mass, surrogate measuresof the hatching asynchrony and size difference between parasiteand hosts, were both related to greater costs of cowbird parasitismwithout bias owing to phylogeny. To establish causality, I manipulatedclutch contents of eastern phoebes (Sayornis phoebe) and examinedwhether earlier hatching by a single cowbird or phoebe egg reducesthe size of the rest of the original host brood. As predicted,greater hatching asynchrony increased the proportion of theoriginal phoebe brood that was lost. This measure of the costof parasitism was partially owing to increased hatching failureof the original eggs in asynchronous broods but was not at allrelated to the size differences of older and younger conspecificnestmates. However, proportional brood loss owing to an earlierhatching conspecific was consistently smaller than brood lossowing to asynchronous cowbirds in both naturally and experimentallyparasitized phoebe nests. These results imply that althoughhatching asynchrony is an important cause of the reduction ofhost broods in parasitized clutches, competitive features ofcowbird nestlings remain necessary to explain the full extentof hosts' reproductive costs caused by interspecific brood parasitism. 相似文献
11.
No haematoza were detected in 40 adult and 58 nestling wild chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) from Antarctica examined by blood smear.
Received: 27 November 1996 / Accepted: 24 March 1997 相似文献
12.
Although the majority of chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) inhabit the Scotia Arc, a minuscule population of ten breeding pairs has been reported several times from Chinstrap Islet, in the Balleny Islands, 5,000 km distant on the opposite side of Antarctica. An aerial photographic census (December 2000) reveals that the overall penguin population of the Balleny Islands has declined by 8% since the last census 16 years ago, while the combined populations of the two largest colonies, Chinstrap Islet and Sabrina Islet, have increased by 11%. A ground visit confirmed for the first time that chinstrap penguins also breed on Sabrina Islet, occupying 20-24 nests on the margins of the Adélie colony (3,790 pairs in all). The small size and anomalous location of the Balleny Islands chinstrap population clearly warrant further study and the strongest protective measures. 相似文献
13.
14.
Meal size of chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) was estimated through weighing adult breeders before and after food transfer during the crèche phase. Mean meal size delivered by each parent was 630.2 ± 178.3 g and was independent of breeding date, number of chicks and sex. Only body size as estimated by flipper length had a significant effect on meal size, suggesting an advantage of large body size which allows maximization of food carried per visit. Received: 22 September 1997 / Accepted: 16 December 1997 相似文献
15.
J. Vinuela J. Moreno L. M. Carrascal J. J. Sanz J. A. Amat # M. Ferrer # J. Belliure J. Cuervo # 《Journal of Zoology》1996,240(1):51-58
We studied the effect of hatching date on breeding performance (chick growth and mortality) and phenology (creching and fledging ages) of the chinstrap penguin during three years. The year affected every variable considered, probably due to pack-ice persistence and food availability differences between years. Hatching date had slight or no effect on mortality and early growth, but was negatively correlated with creching age, which, in turn, was positively related to final size. The decision to leave the chicks unguarded does not seem to be based on the condition of the chicks, but on that of adults. Fledging age was negatively correlated with hatching date, and this effect was more marked in the year with poor growth performance. Given the short time available for breeding in Antarctica, there must be conflicting pressures between investing in feeding chicks and advancing the period of premoult resource storage, this explaining the strong relationship between hatching dates and subsequent phenological events (creching and fledging). In this kind of study, it may be important to remove the effect of inter-year variation before assessing the possible effects of other variables. 相似文献
16.
17.
18.
G. S. Lishman 《Journal of Zoology》1985,205(2):245-263
Adélie Pygoscelis adeliae and Chinstrap P. antarctica penguins are important consumers of Southern Ocean marine resources. The stomach contents of adult penguins at Signy Island, South Orkney Islands, were analysed quantitatively throughout the chick-rearing period. They consisted almost exclusively of Antarctic krill Euphausia superba , Adélies eating 35–63% by number and 23–28% by weight of juvenile krill and Chinstraps 72–87% by number and 90–95% by weight of mature krill, as well as small amounts offish and amphipods. Interspecific dietary differences may partly be attributable to Adélies starting breeding one month before Chinstraps but, as they persist when both are simultaneously rearing chicks, the two species may also forage in somewhat different areas.
Krill data from net hauls indicate a substantial overlap in the size of krill taken by scientific, and probably also commercial, operations and by Adélie and Chinstrap penguins.
Chicks were fed c. 300 g of food 0–5-0-8 times per day, Chinstrap chicks without a sibling being fed most frequently. Chicks of both species were most often fed in the late afternoon, and from estimates of swimming speed and feeding frequency adults may feed quite extensively at night. 相似文献
Krill data from net hauls indicate a substantial overlap in the size of krill taken by scientific, and probably also commercial, operations and by Adélie and Chinstrap penguins.
Chicks were fed c. 300 g of food 0–5-0-8 times per day, Chinstrap chicks without a sibling being fed most frequently. Chicks of both species were most often fed in the late afternoon, and from estimates of swimming speed and feeding frequency adults may feed quite extensively at night. 相似文献
19.
Inter-annual variability in the diet of Chinstrap penguins (Pygoscelis antarctica) at Laurie Island (South Orkney Islands) and 25 de Mayo/King George and Nelson Islands (South Shetland) was examined based
on stomach contents of adults during the 2002/2003–2006/2007 and 2002/2003–2004/2005 breeding seasons, respectively. Krill
(Euphausia superba) dominated the diet as frequency of occurrence (in 100% of samples), number (>99%), and percentage contribution in weight
(>94.8%). Other prey items were minor and varied between years. The weight of stomach contents was significantly different.
The percentage in weight of whole krill was used to compare the feeding conditions across seasons. It differed significantly
at the three sites studied. Distribution of krill size varied among years and localities, showing different krill availability
for penguins. 相似文献
20.
T. D. Williams † D. R. Briggs J. P. Croxall ‡ Y. Naito A. Kato 《Journal of Zoology》1992,227(2):211-230
We present data on diving pattern and performance (dive depth, duration, frequency and organization during the foraging trip) in gentoo penguins Pygoscelis papua , obtained using time-depth recorders ( n = 9 birds, 99 foraging trips). These data are used to estimate various parameters of foraging activity, e.g. foraging range, prey capture rates, and are compared in relation to breeding chronology. Foraging trip duration was 6 h and 10 h, and trip frequency 1.0/day and 0.96/day, during the brooding and creche periods, respectively. Birds spent on average 52%of each foraging trip diving. Dive depth and duration were highly bimodal: shallow dives (< 21 m) averaged 4 m and 0.23 min, and deep dives (> 30 m) 80 m and 2.5 min, respectively. Birds spent on average 71%and 25%of total diving time in deep and shallow dives, respectively. For deep dives, dive duration exceeded the subsequent surface interval, but shallow dives were followed by surface intervals 2–3 times dive duration. We suggest that most shallow dives are searching/exploratory dives and most deep dives are feeding dives. Deep dives showed clear diel patterns averaging 40 m at dawn and dusk and 80–90 m at midday. Estimated foraging ranges were 2.3 km and 4.1 km during the brood and creche period, respectively. Foraging trip duration increased by 4 h between the brood and creche periods but total time spent in deep dives (i.e. time spent feeding) was the same (3 h). Of 99 foraging trips, 56%consisted of only one dive bout and 44%of 2–4 bouts delimited by extended surface intervals > 10 min. We suggest that this pattern of diving activity reflects variation in spatial distribution of prey rather than the effect of physiological constraints on diving ability. 相似文献