首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
The interactions between illusory and real contours have been investigated under monocular, binocular and dichoptic conditions. Results show that under all three presentation conditions, periodic alternations, generally called rivalry, occur during the perception of cognitive (or illusory) triangles, while earlier research had failed to find such rivalry (Bradley and Dumais 1975). With line triangles, rivalry is experienced only under dichoptic conditions. A model is proposed to account for the observed phenomena.  相似文献   

3.
Investigation of perceptual rivalry between conflicting stimuli presented one to each eye can further understanding of the neural underpinnings of conscious visual perception. During rivalry, visual awareness fluctuates between perceptions of the two stimuli. Here, we demonstrate that high-level perceptual grouping can promote rivalry between stimulus pairs that would otherwise be perceived as nonrivalrous. Perceptual grouping was generated with point-light walker stimuli that simulate human motion, visible only as lights placed on the joints. Although such walking figures are unrecognizable when stationary, recognition judgments as complex as gender and identity can accurately be made from animated displays, demonstrating the efficiency with which our visual system can group dynamic local signals into a globally coherent walking figure. We find that point-light walker stimuli presented one to each eye and in different colors and configurations results in strong rivalry. However, rivalry is minimal when the two walkers are split between the eyes or both presented to one eye. This pattern of results suggests that processing animated walker figures promotes rivalry between signals from the two eyes rather than between higher-level representations of the walkers. This leads us to hypothesize that awareness during binocular rivalry involves the integrated activity of high-level perceptual mechanisms in conjunction with lower-level ocular suppression modulated via cortical feedback.  相似文献   

4.
L Wang  X Weng  S He 《PloS one》2012,7(6):e40106
Much information could be processed unconsciously. However, there is no direct evidence on whether perceptual grouping could occur without awareness. To answer this question, we investigated whether a Kanizsa triangle (an example of perceptual grouping) is processed differently from stimuli with the same local components but are ungrouped or weakly grouped. Specifically, using a suppression time paradigm we tested whether a Kanizsa triangle would emerge from interocular continuous flash suppression sooner than control stimuli. Results show a significant advantage of the Kanizsa triangle: the Kanizsa triangle emerged from suppression noise significantly faster than the control stimulus with the local Pacmen randomly rotated (t(9)?=?-2.78, p?=?0.02); and also faster than the control stimulus with all Pacmen rotated 180° (t(11)?=?-3.20, p<0.01). Additional results demonstrated that the advantage of the grouped Kanizsa triangle could not be accounted for by the faster detection speed at the conscious level for the Kanizsa figures on a dynamic noise background. Our results indicate that certain properties supporting perceptual grouping could be processed in the absence of awareness.  相似文献   

5.
A visual model for object detection is proposed. In order to make the detection ability comparable with existing technical methods for object detection, an evolution equation of neurons in the model is derived from the computational principle of active contours. The hierarchical structure of the model emerges naturally from the evolution equation. One drawback involved with initial values of active contours is alleviated by introducing and formulating convexity, which is a visual property. Numerical experiments show that the proposed model detects objects with complex topologies and that it is tolerant of noise. A visual attention model is introduced into the proposed model. Other simulations show that the visual properties of the model are consistent with the results of psychological experiments that disclose the relation between figure–ground reversal and visual attention. We also demonstrate that the model tends to perceive smaller regions as figures, which is a characteristic observed in human visual perception.This work was partially supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (#14780254) from Japan Society of Promotion of Science.  相似文献   

6.
I report spontaneous spatial object grouping in five chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)ranging from 1 to 4 years of age. I recorded subjects' spontaneous interactions with groups of objects, noting the spatial relations in the groupings and the constructive processes that the subjects adopted. Though one subject developed vertical stacks, none of them realized horizontal alignments or spatial correspondences between groups. All subjects showed consistent trends in their constructive processes toward manipulating objects in relation to the body: they increasingly manipulated objects globally, that is to say, they moved or held objects together with the same part of the body. They also increasingly placed objects on the body or in correspondence to their symmetrical body parts. The increasing importance assigned by chimpanzees to body- object relations over object- object relations contrasts with human developmental trends in the domain of manipulative space.  相似文献   

7.
All-round researches of a human being's eye movements in norm and in pathology have been carried out (1967-2006). An analysis of generating of rapid eye movements, those are saccades, has been done. A concept of automaticity of saccades has been formulated. According to the concept a saccade is generated by rhythmo-genesis type, without any external and internal stimuli in their own rhythm. The role of automaticity of saccades in the process of visual perception, and data of impairments of automaticity of saccades in pathology and in uncomfortable visual environment were show.  相似文献   

8.
Coordinating the movements of different body parts is a challenging process for the central nervous system because of several problems. Four of these main difficulties are: first, moving one part can move others; second, the parts can have different dynamics; third, some parts can have different motor goals; and fourth, some parts may be perturbed by outside forces. Here, we propose a novel approach for the control of linked systems with feedback loops for each part. The proximal parts have separate goals, but critically the most distal part has only the common goal. We apply this new control policy to eye-head coordination in two-dimensions, specifically head-unrestrained gaze saccades. Paradoxically, the hierarchical structure has controllers for the gaze and the head, but not for the eye (the most distal part). Our simulations demonstrate that the proposed control structure reproduces much of the published empirical data about gaze movements, e.g., it compensates for perturbations, accurately reaches goals for gaze and head from arbitrary initial positions, simulates the nine relationships of the head-unrestrained main sequence, and reproduces observations from lesion and single-unit recording experiments. We conclude by showing how our model can be easily extended to control structures with more linked segments, such as the control of coordinated eye on head on trunk movements.  相似文献   

9.
Summary An enzymatic tripartition of bacteria is suggested, on the basis of their glycolytic and proteolytic metabolisms, drawing the attention about its advantages, for what concerns both their scientific study and their functional employment in the zymotechnical and medical fields after up-to-date directions.  相似文献   

10.
11.
The prefrontal cortex has been implicated in the suppression of unwanted behavior, based upon observations of humans and monkeys with prefrontal lesions. Despite this, there has been little direct neurophysiological evidence for a mechanism that suppresses specific behavior. In this study, we used an oculomotor delayed match/nonmatch-to-sample task in which monkeys had to remember a stimulus location either as a marker of where to look or as a marker of where not to look. We found a group of neurons in both the frontal eye field and the caudal prefrontal cortex that carried signals selective for the forbidden stimulus. The activity of these "don't look" neurons correlated with the monkeys' success or failure on the task. These results demonstrate a frontal signal that is related to the active suppression of one action while the subject performs another.  相似文献   

12.
As you read this text, your eyes make saccades that guide your fovea from one word to the next. Accuracy of these movements require the brain to monitor and learn from visual errors. A current model suggests that learning is supported by two different adaptive processes, one fast (high error sensitivity, low retention), and the other slow (low error sensitivity, high retention). Here, we searched for signatures of these hypothesized processes and found that following experience of a visual error, there was an adaptive change in the motor commands of the subsequent saccade. Surprisingly, this adaptation was not uniformly expressed throughout the movement. Rather, after experience of a single error, the adaptive response in the subsequent trial was limited to the deceleration period. After repeated exposure to the same error, the acceleration period commands also adapted, and exhibited resistance to forgetting during set-breaks. In contrast, the deceleration period commands adapted more rapidly, but suffered from poor retention during these same breaks. State-space models suggested that acceleration and deceleration periods were supported by a shared adaptive state which re-aimed the saccade, as well as two separate processes which resembled a two-state model: one that learned slowly and contributed primarily via acceleration period commands, and another that learned rapidly but contributed primarily via deceleration period commands.  相似文献   

13.
On the detection of salient contours   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
Braun J 《Spatial Vision》1999,12(2):211-225
When visual space is densely populated by elements of random orientation, elements which happen to be aligned may form a perceptually salient 'contour' (Field et al., 1993; Kovacs and Julesz, 1993). Here we further characterize human performance for detecting this type of Gestalt grouping. We find that detectability of salient contours reaches a plateau when they comprise at least 10 elements and are presented for at least 200 ms. It has been suggested that the detection of salient contours is mediated by the intrinsic connectivity of striate cortex and several computational models have been formulated on this basis. The present data provide a benchmark against which such models can be evaluated.  相似文献   

14.
15.
16.
The goal of this study was to explore the development of the gas transport systems in chick embryos after half of the shell surface is closed on incubation day 11. By the end of day 3 of the experiment (incubation day 14), the vascular reduction in the chorioallantois under the covered zone is fully compensated by the vessel dilatation and growth in the intact half. In parallel, the oxygen capacity of blood elevates and hematopoiesis increases: the indices of hematocrit, hemoglobin, and erythrocyte count increase by half. The development of the left ventricular myocardium accelerates through the myocyte hyperplasia and their more mature ultrastructure. The obtained data indicate that hypoxia accelerates the embryonic development and leads to earlier and faster differentiation of the gas transport systems. However, the efficiency of antihypoxic responses is limited by the upper bound of capillary density in the chorionic respiratory network. After 1 week of the experiment (incubation day 18), the total vascular volume in the chorioallantois is halved relative to control, while the arterial walls substantially thicken in the open part, which increases the peripheral resistance. Coronary sclerosis and dystrophy of cardiomyocytes developed in the left ventricle. These general hemodynamic abnormalities are accompanied by a notable embryonic growth inhibition. Thus, the structural compensation of the gas transport systems in the experiment becomes exhausted and cannot provide for increasing metabolic demands of the growing embryo.  相似文献   

17.
Investigation on illusory contours is important for understanding the mechanisms underlying the object recognition of human visual system. Numerous researches have shown that illusory contours formed in motion and stereopsis are generated by the unmatched features. Here we conduct three psychophysical experiments to test if Kanizsa illusory contours are also caused by unmatched information. Different types of motion (including horizontal translation, radial expanding and shrinking) are utilized in the experiments. The results show that no matter under what kind of motion, when figures or background move separately illusory contours are perceived stronger, and there is no significant difference between the perceived strength in these two types of motion. However, no such enhancement of perceived strength is found when figures and background move together. It is found that the strengthened unmatched features generate the enhancement effect of illusory contour perception in motion. Thus the results suggest that the process of unmatched information in visual system is a critical step in the formation of illusory contours.  相似文献   

18.
错觉轮廓或主观边框是指人们知觉到的刺激图案中实际上并不存在的轮廓、线、边或面。在人们的视觉系统将视网膜平面上的二维图像还原成三维的过程中,错觉轮廓笑觉起着一定的作用。本文综述了近年来在生理学领域对视觉皮层ICs反应细胞的研究和由此提出的ICs知觉的可能神经机制,并简述了心理学上对ICs相关研究。  相似文献   

19.
Object perception is one of the most important components of visual perception of human beings and mammalian animals. It is a most confusing problem on object perception that how we separate object from background and obtain the picture of the whole object. In many cases one object partly occludes the other one in natural world. When the brightness of the occluding object is the same as or similar to that of the background, though there is no difference between visual stimuli, we can still ret…  相似文献   

20.
It is proposed that there is a low-level automatic component of visual scanning in which saccades are triggered probabilistically in time by nonfoveal stimulus features. By ignoring cognitive factors and memory, a first-order Markov approach is taken, which is tractable for spatially homogeneous stimuli. The predicted distributions of saccade magnitude, fixation duration, fixation position, and how they are related to stimulus size are in good agreement with empirical observations by Bahill et al. (1975), Enoch (1959), and Harris et al. (1988). Ways in which this low-level process could be modulated by cognitive factors are discussed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号