首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 310 毫秒
1.
ABSTRACT. The zigzagging behaviour of male Plodia interpunctella flying up a plume of sex pheromone was investigated in a horizontal wind tunnel by detailed analysis of the moths' ground tracks, groundspeeds, orientations and airspeeds. The moths ‘homed in’ on the source of the pheromone plume by progressively reducing airspeed and turning more into wind, thereby reducing groundspeed and the distance between track reversals and so narrowing down their zigzags (Fig. 16). Track angles and times between reversals were unaffected. Removing the wind-borne pheromone plume while a moth was flying along it confirmed that zigzagging can be an anemotactic response to losing the scent rather than a chemotactic response to the plume. For the first 1–2 s after the moth entered pheromone-free air the zigzagging was indistinguishable from that shown when the plume remained; thereafter it widened progressively until the moths were flying to and fro at c. 90° to the wind. The after-effect of odour stimulation persisted for many zigs and zags and many seconds (Figs. 4 and 5). Moths flying along pheromone plumes compensated efficiently for differences of wind speed, showing similar distributions of track angles to wind, and of ground-speeds, in winds of 0.1, 0.2 and 0.3 ms-1 (Figs. 12 and 13). Groundspeed varied with track angle to wind and this relationship was also similar in the three wind speeds (Fig. 14). This constancy of track angles and groundspeeds was due to the moths both increasing their airspeeds and turning more into wind at the higher wind speeds (Fig. 17). Thus the direction of the apparent movement of the ground pattern beneath the moths varied with wind speed. It is inferred that the moths, although unable to sense the wind directly, are able to compensate for changes in wind speed by integrating the wind-dependent optomotor input with information about their own airspeed, or with information about their own turning movements. Maintaining some ‘preferred’ relationship between these inputs by adjustments of orientation and airspeed, would then serve to maintain a given combination of track angle and groundspeed independently of wind speed. The preferred relationship is repeatedly re-set by the changing olfactory input from the pheromone plume, which also controls the switching between left and right of the upwind direction.  相似文献   

2.
The pheromone-modulated upwind flight ofLymantria dispar males responding to different pheromone plume structures and visual stimuli designed to mimic trees was video recorded in a forest. Males flying upwind along pheromone plumes of similar structure generated tracks that were similar in appearance and quantitatively similar in almost all parameters measured, regardless of the experimentally manipulated visual stimuli associated with the pheromone source. Net velocities, ground speeds, and airspeeds of males flying in point-source plumes were slower than those of males flying in the wider, more diffuse plumes issuing from a cylindrical baffle. The mean track angle of males flying in plumes issuing from a point source was greater (oriented more across the wind) than that of males flying in plumes issuing from a transparent cylindrical baffle. Males flying in point-source plumes also turned more frequently and had narrower tracks overall than males responding to plumes from a cylindrical baffle. These data suggest thatL. dispar males orienting to pheromone sources (i.e., calling females) associated with visible vertical cylinders (i.e., trees) use predominantly olfactory cues to locate the source and that the structure of the pheromone plume markedly affects the flight orientation and the resultant track.  相似文献   

3.
Odor-modulated upwind flight of the sphinx moth,Manduca sexta L.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
1. Male and female Manduca sexta flew upwind in response to the odor of female sex-pheromone gland extract or fresh tobacco leaf respectively, and generated very similar zigzagging tracks along the odor plume. 2. After loss of odor during flight, males and females alike: (1) first flew slower and steered their flight more across the wind, then (2) stopped moving upwind, and finally (3) regressed downwind. 3. Males flying upwind in a pheromone plume in wind of different velocities maintained their ground speed near a relatively constant 'preferred' value by increasing their air speed as the velocity of the wind increased, and also maintained the average angle of their resultant flight tracks with respect to the wind at a preferred value by steering a course more precisely due upwind. 4. The inter-turn duration and turn rate, two measures of the temporal aspects of the flight track, were maintained, on average, with remarkable consistency across all wind velocities and in both sexes. The inter-turn durations also decreased significantly as moths approached the odor source, suggesting modulation of the temporal pattern of turning by some feature of the odor plume. This temporal regularity of turning appears to be one of the most stereotyped features of odor-modulated flight in M. sexta.  相似文献   

4.
The interception of a pheromone filament induces flying moths to surge briefly nearly straight upwind; in the absence of pheromone moths cease upwind progress and zigzag crosswind. We tested males of the almond moth, Cadra cautella (Lepidoptera, Pyralidae), in a low-turbulence wind tunnel in wind velocities of 20, 40 and 80 cm s−1. A mechanical pulse generator was set to produce plumes either with same pheromone pulse frequency (pulse generation frequency of 2.9 Hz, interpulse distances from 7 cm to 28 cm) or plumes with same interpulse distance across the three wind velocities (interpulse distance of 14 ± 2 cm, pulse generation frequency of 1.7–5.0 Hz). In plumes of similar pulse frequency, the faster the speed of the wind the slower the ground speed of flight. However, in plumes of similar interpulse distance, ground speed remained relatively constant independent of the wind speed. A `realized' frequency of pulse interception for males flying along the various combinations of pulse frequencies and wind velocities was calculated using the males' average airspeed and the spatial distribution of pheromone pulses in the plume. Realized frequency of pulse interception ranged from 1.3- to 3.0-fold higher than the frequency of pulse generation. The flight tracks of males reflected the regime of realized pulse interception. These results suggest that upwind flight orientation of male C. cautella to pheromone in different wind velocities is determined by the flux of filament encounter. Accepted: 3 September 1997  相似文献   

5.
Several mechanisms have been proposed to underlie the characteristic `zigzag' tracks produced by moths flying up an odor plume. To test which, if any, of these are capable of reproducing the animals' behavior, we constructed behavioral-level simulations. The simulations are as tightly constrained as possible by the known biology, and incorporate realistic physical effects such as air turbulence, and delays due to sensory transduction and axonal condition, to mimic the dynamics of sensory information encountered by real moths. Formulated as schemas, the models all share a common set of sensory and motor systems, but differ in the interposed control systems. We analyzed the behavior of the models with the same methods we use for real moths. Even the simplest of the models was capable of successful orientation some of the time, and of producing flight tracks similar to those of moths. Individuals which succeeded in tracking the odor plume produced average behavior not significantly different from that of real moths. As a population, however, none of the models was as successful as the moths. The best of the models had a success rate in tracking the plume of about 30%, compared to the average of 70% seen in the insects. Accepted: 25 March 1998  相似文献   

6.
Abstract Airborne pheromone plumes in wind comprise filaments of odour interspersed with gaps of clean air. When flying moths intercept a filament, they have a tendency to surge upwind momentarily, and then fly crosswind until another filament is intercepted. Thus, the moment-to-moment contact with pheromone mediates the shape of a flight track along the plume. Within some range of favourable interception rates, flight tracks become straighter and are headed more due upwind. However, as the rate of interception increases, there comes a point at which the moth should not be able to discern discreet filaments but, rather, should perceive a 'fused signal'. At the extreme, homogeneous clouds of pheromone inhibit upwind progress by representative tortricids. In a wind tunnel, Cadra cautella (Walker) (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae) were presented with 10 ms pulses of pheromone at a repetition rate of 5, 10, 17 and 25/s and a continuous, internally turbulent plume. Pulse size and concentrations were verified with a miniature photoionization detector sampling surrogate odour, propylene, at 100 Hz. Male moths maintain upwind progress even at plumes of 25 filaments/s. Furthermore, moths exhibited greater velocities and headings more due upwind at 17 and 25 Hz than at the lower frequencies or with the continuous plume. It is hypothesized that either C. cautella possesses a versatile sensory system that allows the resolution of these rapidly pulsed pheromone plumes, or that this species does not require a 'flickering' signal to fly upwind.  相似文献   

7.
Abstract.  Two-day-old male cowpea weevils, Callosobruchus maculatus, fly upwind to a point source of female sex pheromone at three wind speeds. All beetles initiating flight along the pheromone plume make contact with the pheromone source. Analysis of digitized flight tracks indicates that C. maculatus males respond similarly to moths tested at several wind speeds. Beetles' mean net upwind speeds and speeds along their track are similar ( P  > 0.05) across wind speeds, whereas airspeeds increase ( P <  0.01) with increasing wind speed. Beetles adjust their course angles to fly more directly upwind in higher wind speeds, whereas track angles are almost identical at each wind speed. The zigzag flight paths are generally narrow compared with most moth flight tracks and interturn distances are similar ( P  > 0.05) at the wind speeds employed. The frequency of these counterturns across the wind line is almost constant regardless of wind speed, and there is little variation between individuals. The upwind flight tracks are more directly upwind than those typically seen for male moths flying upwind toward sex pheromone sources. Male moths typically produce a bimodal distribution of track angles to the left and right of the windline, whereas C. maculatus males' track angles are centred about 0°. Preliminary examination of two other beetle species indicates that they fly upwind in a similar fashion.  相似文献   

8.
9.
While walking upwind to a pheromone source, male oriental fruit moths, Grapholita molesta (Busck) exhibit none of the temporally regular course reversals (counterturns) and resultant zigzag tracks which characterise the tracks of males flying upwind to pheromone. Rather, males walk in a nearly straight line, steering directly upwind, in contrast to flying males, which steer a course alternating back and forth about 15–20° to either side of the windline. These results support the idea that counterturning in males flying to pheromone sources is a mechanism which enhances their visual perception of wind-induced drift. Counterturning would be unnecessary in walking males, which can perceive wind velocity and direction via mechanoreceptors.  相似文献   

10.
  • 1 Catches of male pea moths in six designs of trap, each containing (E,E)-8,10-dodecadienyl acetate as an attractant, were compared; triangular-shaped traps caught most moths.
  • 2 The vertical distribution of moths within a pea crop and the size of catches at different levels varied in different wind speeds. Three-quarter crop height is probably the optimum height for monitoring.
  • 3 Five trap designs (including the triangular type) captured only 20–30% of individuals landing on them; water traps were slightly more efficient.
  • 4 There was an eight-fold range in the number of moths caught depending on the sticky material used. ‘Bird-Tanglefoot’ was most effective.
  • 5 Smoke plumes, emitted from different trap designs in the field to simulate plumes of attractant, were photographed and measured. The effect of trap design on plume shape was confirmed in wind tunnel tests.
  • 6 Field tests showed that the more elongated the mean plume emitted from a trap the greater the catch.
  • 7 The tracks of individual moths flying to traps are discussed in relation to current views on orientation to odour sources.
  相似文献   

11.
ABSTRACT. Male oriental fruit moths, Grapholitha molesta (Busck) (Tortricidae), continue to zigzag along a pheromone plume to the source in zero wind, if they have started flight with wind on. If the pheromone source is removed and the plume is hence truncated, moths flying in zero wind out of the end of the plume into clean air increase the width of their reversals and the angles of the straight legs of the tracks so they are more directly across the former wind line. Such moths reach the source less often than do those flying along a continuous plume. The males continue to zigzag up a plume in zero wind, apparently by a combination of sequential sampling of concentration along their path and the performance of an internal, self-steered programme of track reversals (zigzags) whose frequency increases with concentration. Visual feedback may aid in the still-air performance of the zigzags. We propose that both the sequential sampling (longitudinal klinotaxis) and self-steered counter-turning programme also are used in wind as well; anemotaxis apparently polarizes the direction of the zigzags to result in upwind displacement, and the narrow zigzags caused by the higher concentration in the plume keep the male 'locked on' to the odour.  相似文献   

12.
Many animals have the ability to search for odor sources by tracking their plumes. Some of the key features of this search behavior have been successfully transferred to robot platforms, although the capabilities of animals are still beyond the current level of sensor technologies. The examples described in this paper are (1) incorporating into a wheeled robot the upwind surges and casting used by moths in tracking pheromone plumes, (2) extracting useful information from the response patterns of a chemical sensor array patterned after the spatially distributed chemoreceptors of some animals, and (3) mimicking the fanning behavior of silkworm moths to enhance the reception of chemical signals by drawing molecules from one direction. The achievements so far and current efforts are reviewed to illustrate the steps to be taken toward future development of this technology.  相似文献   

13.
The upwind zigzag flights of male gypsy moths (Lymantria dispar L.; Lepidoptera: Lymantriidae) along narrow, ribbon‐like and wide, turbulent plumes of pheromone were examined in a wind tunnel at light levels of 450 and 4 lux. Under all conditions tested males flew upwind zigzag paths. In 450 lux, males flying along turbulent plumes had the highest ground speeds and the widest crosswind excursions between counterturns, compared to slow flight and a narrow zigzag of males along a ribbon plume. In a turbulent plume, males flew more slowly and had narrower zigzags in 4 than in 450 lux. Across most treatments of plume structure and light level, the rate of transverse image flow and the frequency of counterturning remained relatively constant. The effects of light levels on orientation are not readily reconcilable with a model in which moths in low light levels would head more towards crosswind, thereby enhancing the rate of transverse image flow and the perception of wind‐induced drift.  相似文献   

14.
Abstract. Mature female Brachymeria intermedia (Hymenoptera: Chalcididae) were conditioned to fly towards vanilla odour in a wind tunnel. We analysed the tracks of wasps flying along turbulent plumes of either host odour (pupae of the gypsy moth, Lymantria dispar) or vanilla odour, along either a ribbon plume or a turbulent plume of vanilla odour, and before and after plume removal. Wasps flew in similar shallow zigzagging tracks along the turbulent plume of host and vanilla odours. When the plume was removed while wasps were flying upwind along a turbulent plume of vanilla odour, wasps either maintained an upwind course or drifted sideways, alternating upwind and downwind courses before turning around and flying downwind. No wasp casted upon loss of the plume.  相似文献   

15.
Mechanisms of animal navigation in odor plumes   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Chemical signals mediate many of life's processes. For organisms that use these signals to orient and navigate in their environment, where and when these cues are encountered is crucial in determining behavioral responses. In air and water, fluid mechanics impinge directly upon the distribution of odorous molecules in time and space. Animals frequently employ behavioral mechanisms that allow them to take advantage of both chemical and fluid dynamic information in order to move toward the source. In turbulent plumes, where odor is patchily distributed, animals are exposed to a highly intermittent signal. The most detailed studies that have attempted to measure fluid dynamic conditions, odor plume structure, and resultant orientation behavior have involved moths, crabs, and lobsters. The behavioral mechanisms employed by these organisms are different but generally integrate some form of chemically modulated orientation (chemotaxis) with a visual or mechanical assessment of flow conditions in order to steer up-current or upwind (rheo- or anemo-taxis, respectively). Across-stream turns are another conspicuous feature of odor-modulated tracks of a variety of organisms in different fluid conditions. In some cases, turning is initiated by detection of the lateral edges of a well-defined plume (crabs), whereas in other animals turning appears to be steered according to an internally generated program modulated by odor contacts (moth counterturning). Other organisms such as birds and fish may use similar mechanisms, but the experimental data for these organisms is not yet as convincing. The behavioral strategies employed by a variety of animals result in orientation responses that are appropriate for the dispersed, intermittent plumes dictated by the fluid-mechanical conditions in the environments that these different macroscopic organisms inhabit.  相似文献   

16.
Abstract. The effects of pheromone concentration and ambient temperature on male gypsy moth, Lymantria dispar (L.) (Lepidoptera), flight responses to pheromone were investigated in a wind tunnel. As the pheromone dose increased from 10 ng to 1000 ng, males flew at progressively slower airspeeds and ground speeds, and reduced their wingbeat frequencies. Furthermore, the moths steered significantly smaller course angles as the pheromone concentration increased, indicating that they were adopting a more upwind heading. The overall width of the flight tracks also decreased when males flew in more concentrated pheromone plumes. Estimation of plume dimensions using a male wing-fanning assay showed that as pheromone dosage increased, the resultant active spaces became wider, indicating that an inverse relationship existed between the dimensions of the time-averaged plume and the width of track reversals and that most turns were initiated within the plume. When males were flown at cool (20°C) and warm (26°C) ambient temperatures but to equivalent pheromone emission rates, they exhibited higher airspeeds and ground speeds at the higher temperature but steered larger course angles. Track widths, and length of track legs were, however, similar at the two temperatures. The mean turning frequency was nearly the same (c. 4 turns/s) across all the concentrations and temperatures tested even though the moths' thoracic temperature differed by 5°C when the ambient temperature was varied.  相似文献   

17.
Male Cadra cautella were presented with five heterogeneous pheromone clouds (created from source doses of 0, 0.01, 1, 100, and 10 000 ng) with and without superimposed plumes of either clean air or sex pheromone in a wind tunnel. Moths provided with the lowest doses of background clouds without a superimposed plume did not fly upwind. Moths provided with higher doses of background clouds, with or without superimposed air plumes, increased their track, course, and drift angles (i.e., their zigzags headed more towards crosswind) with increased dose, but slowed their velocity. No differences in flight track parameters were observed for moths provided with a superimposed pheromone plume, regardless of the background cloud dose. Moreover, moths were able to locate the source of superimposed air plumes in the highest background dose, and of superimposed pheromone plumes in any background dose. The significance of these results is discussed in the context of mating disruption.  相似文献   

18.
Abstract. The effects of plume intermittency and volume on behavioural and flight responses to pheromone of male Cadra cautella (Walker) (Lepidoptera: Phycitinae) were investigated in a pulling wind tunnel. The fine-scale structure of turbulent pheromone plumes was mimicked and manipulated using a pulser device that generated continuous ribbon plumes or intermittent plumes with defined pulse frequency and volume. As pulse frequency increased from 0.6 to 5 Hz and injected volume increased from 0.5 to 5 mls-1, males flew progressively higher air and ground speeds, turned less frequently, and steered smaller course angles, resulting in straighter flight tracks. The faster the frequency of pulses and the greater the volume of the plume, the higher the proportion of males responding, the shorter their latencies, and the less time spent in the behaviour. Flight tracks of male C.cautella to point sources of pheromone depend on the frequency of filaments encountered.  相似文献   

19.
Insects flying in a horizontal pheromone plume must attend to visual cues to ensure that they make upwind progress. Moreover, it is suggested that flying insects will also modulate their flight speed to maintain a constant retinal angular velocity of terrestrial contrast elements. Evidence from flies and honeybees supports such a hypothesis, although tests with male moths and beetles flying in pheromone plumes are not conclusive. These insects typically fly faster at higher elevations above a high‐contrast ground pattern, as predicted by the hypothesis, although the increase in speed is not sufficient to demonstrate quantitatively that they maintain constant visual angular velocity of the ground pattern. To test this hypothesis more rigorously, the flight speed of male oriental fruit moths (OFM) Grapholita molesta Busck (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) flying in a sex pheromone plume in a laboratory wind tunnel is measured at various heights (5–40 cm) above patterns of different spatial wavelength (1.8–90°) in the direction of flight. The OFM modulate their flight speed three‐fold over different patterns. They fly fastest when there is no pattern in the tunnel or the contrast elements are too narrow to resolve. When the spatial wavelength of the pattern is sufficiently wide to resolve, moths fly at a speed that tends to maintain a visual contrast frequency of 3.5 ± 3.2 Hz rather than a constant angular velocity, which varies from 57 to 611° s?1. In addition, for the first time, it is also demonstrated that the width of a contrast pattern perpendicular to the flight direction modulates flight speed.  相似文献   

20.
Abstract .Unilaterally antennectomized Heliothis virescens (F.) males flying close to the central axis of a plume of sex pheromone display no significant differences in behaviour compared to sham-operated males in course angles, track angles, airspeed and groundspeed. This demonstrates that right/left antennal information is not necessary for normal orientation movements in response to pheromone, but rather that it is 'blended' within the moth's central nervous system before pheromone-mediated manoeuvres are made. However, some unilaterally antennectomized moths (36%) make repetitive, asymmetrical, saw-tooth-shaped tracks during pheromone-mediated upwind progress, whereas control moths never make such tracks. Unilaterally antennectomized moths made such tracks on the side of the plume contralateral to the missing antenna. We hypothesize that these occasional asymmetrical tracks in unilaterally ablated males are the result of reiterative asymmetrical pheromone stimulation of a higher probability on track legs going toward rather than away from the long axis of the plume on males with a single antenna remaining on the 'away from axis' side. Combined with a greater propensity for treated moths to lock onto the plume away from the central axis on one side rather than the other, repetitive successive asymmetrical track legs (resulting in a saw-tooth-shaped track) are commonly observed in these moths. Control moths do also make asymmetric successive track legs but they rarely are repeated and thus are not readily observed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号