共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Philipp J. Kraemer 《Behavioural processes》1993,30(3):291-297
Pigeons were trained on a matching-to-sample task in which they had to respond to a different choice stimulus following the same durations (2 or 10 s) of two different signals. The duration signals consisted of a white light presented from the ceiling and a red light presented from the front wall. Subsequent test performance indicated that matching accuracy declined (1) when the set of choice stimuli following a duration signal differed from the set presented during training, and (2) when the color or location of the duration signal was changed from values used during training. These results are discussed in terms of attention to uninformative features of a visual stimulus. 相似文献
2.
Ansgar D. Endress Donal Cahill Stefanie Block Jeffrey Watumull Marc D. Hauser 《Biology letters》2009,5(6):749-751
Human language, and grammatical competence in particular, relies on a set of computational operations that, in its entirety, is not observed in other animals. Such uniqueness leaves open the possibility that components of our linguistic competence are shared with other animals, having evolved for non-linguistic functions. Here, we explore this problem from a comparative perspective, asking whether cotton-top tamarin monkeys (Saguinus oedipus) can spontaneously (no training) acquire an affixation rule that shares important properties with our inflectional morphology (e.g. the rule that adds –ed to create the past tense, as in the transformation of walk into walk-ed). Using playback experiments, we show that tamarins discriminate between bisyllabic items that start with a specific ‘prefix’ syllable and those that end with the same syllable as a ‘suffix’. These results suggest that some of the computational mechanisms subserving affixation in a diversity of languages are shared with other animals, relying on basic perceptual or memory primitives that evolved for non-linguistic functions. 相似文献
3.
Oren Kolodny Shimon Edelman Arnon Lotem 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2015,282(1811)
The skills required for the learning and use of language are the focus of extensive research, and their evolutionary origins are widely debated. Using agent-based simulations in a range of virtual environments, we demonstrate that challenges of foraging for food can select for cognitive mechanisms supporting complex, hierarchical, sequential learning, the need for which arises in language acquisition. Building on previous work, where we explored the conditions under which reinforcement learning is out-competed by seldom-reinforced continuous learning that constructs a network model of the environment, we now show that realistic features of the foraging environment can select for two critical advances: (i) chunking of meaningful sequences found in the data, leading to representations composed of units that better fit the prevalent statistical patterns in the environment; and (ii) generalization across units based on their contextual similarity. Importantly, these learning processes, which in our framework evolved for making better foraging decisions, had been earlier shown to reproduce a range of findings in language learning in humans. Thus, our results suggest a possible evolutionary trajectory that may have led from basic learning mechanisms to complex hierarchical sequential learning that can support advanced cognitive abilities of the kind needed for language acquisition. 相似文献
4.
A large body of studies has investigated the capacity of non-human primates, dogs, birds and lower vertebrates to estimate different quantities of objects or events. Little attention, however, has been devoted to felines, and no study has specifically concentrated on cats’ numerical cognition. The present study aims to investigate the capacity of domestic cats to distinguish between two and three dots in order to obtain food; results showed that cats can be trained to discriminate between the two quantities. Furthermore our research suggests that cats do not spontaneously use numerical information, but rather seem to make use of visual cues that co-vary with numerosity in order to solve the task. 相似文献
5.
Johan J. Bolhuis Stephen Crain Ian Roberts 《Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society》2023,98(3):931-941
Around the middle of the last century, the prevailing psychological paradigm of behaviourism was challenged by what is now known as the cognitive revolution. Behaviourists viewed learning as changes in patterns of behaviour through reinforcement. By contrast, advocates of the cognitive approach argued that such behavioural changes were outward manifestations of computational operations on mental representations. Here we consider the current state of the cognitive revolution, focusing on the two most contentious issues in the debate: language and learning. The cognitive approach has proved to be extremely fruitful in both fields. Although contemporary learning theory has almost completely embraced the cognitive approach, the study of language has witnessed a clear empiricist trend to revert back to a kind of neo-behaviourism. Many contemporary authors contend that language is a means of communication that is learned solely through the observation of external events, and culturally transmitted to successive generations. Here, we argue that learning and language can only be properly understood from a cognitive perspective, where the mind is conceived of as a biologically underpinned computational system. As is the case in learning theory, there is abundant evidence showing that language is subserved by an autonomous cognitive system in the mind. We conclude that the cognitive revolution has fundamentally changed our understanding of the mind. 相似文献
6.
Ida E. Bailey Kate V. Morgan Marion Bertin Simone L. Meddle Susan D. Healy 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2014,281(1784)
It is generally assumed that birds’ choice of structurally suitable materials
for nest building is genetically predetermined. Here, we tested that assumption by
investigating whether experience affected male zebra finches’
(Taeniopygia guttata) choice of nest material. After a short
period of building with relatively flexible string, birds preferred to build with
stiffer string while those that had experienced a stiffer string were indifferent to
string type. After building a complete nest with either string type, however, all
birds increased their preference for stiff string. The stiffer string appeared to be
the more effective building material as birds required fewer pieces of stiffer than
flexible string to build a roofed nest. For birds that raised chicks successfully,
there was no association between the material they used to build their nest and the
type they subsequently preferred. Birds’ material preference reflected neither
the preference of their father nor of their siblings but juvenile experience of
either string type increased their preference for stiffer string. Our results
represent two important advances: (i) birds choose nest material based on the
structural properties of the material; (ii) nest material preference is not entirely
genetically predetermined as both the type and amount of experience influences
birds’ choices. 相似文献
7.
H. Hultsch D. Todt 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1996,179(3):291-299
Studies on the song learning in birds revealed a puzzling property of the acquisition system: Stimulus memorization becomes effective after remarkably few exposures, but nevertheless shows a relationship to the frequency of exposure to learning stimuli. This raises questions on the amount of learning that will occur during a given exposure to song. To examine this issue, we tutored handraised fledgling nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos) with song strings, in which the serial succession of species-typical master songs was altered upon subsequent exposures. The sequencing of imitations obtained from the birds' adult singing revealed the following results: (1) A single exposure was sufficient for subjects to acquire serial information on song-type sequencing. (2) The first exposure to a master string played a key role for this accomplishment. (3) Nevertheless, the acquisition of serial information improved with increasing exposure frequency of master strings. (4) The acquisition of song patterns was not impaired by a non-regular presentation of master song-types. With respect to the particular salience of the first exposure for sequence memorization, we termed the phenomenon primer effect. The findings suggest that stimulus acquisition during perceptual song learning is mediated by a discontinuous process. Once acquired, information is then consolidated gradually, i.e. through an incremental process. 相似文献
8.
E. W. Menzel Jr. 《Primates; journal of primatology》1991,32(4):497-508
Fifteen adult chimpanzees were tested on a series of tasks that differed from standard two-choice object discrimination learning
problems in one detail: a third choice was sometimes offered, and it consisted of clearly visible and readily accessible food.
Even under conditions where they would have to score 100% on the discrimination learning tasks to get as much food as they
could get by taking the “free” food, many of the chimpanzees worked on the problems. Individual differences were large and
reliable. Frequency of response to a given problem also varied according to how accurately the animals were performing and
increased markedly if the hidden food was made a few grams larger than the free food. The chimpanzees did not rely strictly
on a “bird-in-hand” strategy or necessarily always work to get the maximum amount of food with the minimum amount of energy
expenditure. Whether this is bad economics or good economics depends on the time scale on which one views adaptation. 相似文献
9.
In recent years, it has been shown that animals can localize the geometric center of an area by reference to the shape of the environment. We trained a group of mice (experimental group) to search for a pellet hidden under sand in the center of a square-shaped dry maze. Three weeks later, they were tested in a triangular enclosure half the size of the training area and a circular enclosure double the size of the training area to see transfer to these enclosures. We compared their searching behavior with that of subjects that had received no training. The results show that the experimental group searched the geometric center of each enclosure in both transfer tests, while the untrained control group walked along the walls. This indicates that the experimental group localized the center not by reference to the absolute distance from the corners but by equal distances from all walls (geometric center). 相似文献
10.
The photosynthetic pathway composition (C3:C4 mixture) of an ecosystem is an important controller of carbon exchanges and surface energy flux partitioning, and therefore represents a fundamental ecophysiological distinction. To assess photosynthetic mixtures at a tallgrass prairie pasture in Oklahoma, we collected nighttime above-canopy air samples along concentration and isotopic gradients throughout the 1999 and 2000 growing seasons. We analyzed these samples for their CO2 concentration and carbon isotopic composition and calculated C3:C4 proportions with a two-source mixing model. In 1999, the C4 percentage increased from 38% in spring (late April) to 86% in early fall (mid-September). The C4 percentages inferred from ecosystem respiration measurements in 2000 indicate a smaller shift, from 67% in spring (early May) to 77% in mid-summer (late July). We also sampled daytime CO2 concentration and carbon isotope gradients above the canopy to determine ecosystem discrimination against 13CO2 during net uptake. These discrimination values were always lower than corresponding nighttime ecosystem respiration isotopic signatures would suggest. After accounting for the isotopic disequilibria between respiration and photosynthesis resulting from seasonal variations in the C3:C4 mixture, we estimated canopy photosynthetic discrimination. The C4 percentage calculated from this approach agrees with the percentage determined from nighttime respiration for sampling periods in both growing seasons. Isotopic imbalances between photosynthesis and respiration are likely to be common in mixed C3:C4 ecosystems and must be considered when using daytime isotopic measurements to constrain ecosystem physiology. Given the global extent of such ecosystems, isotopic imbalances likely contribute to global variations in the carbon isotopic composition of atmospheric CO2. 相似文献
11.
12.
S. Yoshida A. Yasuda H. Kawazato K. Sakai T. Shimada †M. Takeshita ‡S. Yuasa ‡T. Kobayashi ‡S. Watanabe ‡H. Okuyama 《Journal of neurochemistry》1997,68(3):1261-1268
Abstract: Rats fed either a safflower oil (α-linolenate-deficient) or a perilla oil (α-linolenate-sufficient) diet through two generations (F1) showed significant differences in the brightness-discrimination learning task. In this task, correct responses were lever-pressing responses, which were reinforced with dietary pellets, and incorrect responses were those with no reinforcement. The inferior learning performance in the safflower oil group was caused mainly by the inferior ability to rectify the incorrect responses through the learning sessions. In the safflower oil group after the learning task, the average densities of synaptic vesicles in the terminals of the hippocampus CA1 region were decreased by nearly 30% as compared with those in the perilla oil group, and it is notable that this difference was not detected without the learning task. These results suggest that dietary oil-induced morphological changes in synapses in the hippocampus of rats are related to the differential learning performance and that the turnover rate of synaptic vesicles in the hippocampus may be an important factor affecting learning performance. 相似文献
13.
Cartmill EA Beilock S Goldin-Meadow S 《Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences》2012,367(1585):129-143
The movements we make with our hands both reflect our mental processes and help to shape them. Our actions and gestures can affect our mental representations of actions and objects. In this paper, we explore the relationship between action, gesture and thought in both humans and non-human primates and discuss its role in the evolution of language. Human gesture (specifically representational gesture) may provide a unique link between action and mental representation. It is kinaesthetically close to action and is, at the same time, symbolic. Non-human primates use gesture frequently to communicate, and do so flexibly. However, their gestures mainly resemble incomplete actions and lack the representational elements that characterize much of human gesture. Differences in the mirror neuron system provide a potential explanation for non-human primates' lack of representational gestures; the monkey mirror system does not respond to representational gestures, while the human system does. In humans, gesture grounds mental representation in action, but there is no evidence for this link in other primates. We argue that gesture played an important role in the transition to symbolic thought and language in human evolution, following a cognitive leap that allowed gesture to incorporate representational elements. 相似文献
14.
David L. Hull 《Biology & philosophy》1994,9(3):375-386
Mayr has made both conceptual and professional contributions to the establishment of the history and philosophy of biology. His conceptual contributions include, among many others, the notion of population thinking. He has also played an important role in the establishment of history and philosophy of biology as viable professional disciplines. 相似文献
15.
16.
Doris S. Warriner 《Anthropology & education quarterly》2007,38(4):343-359
In this article, I explore the complicated relationship between ideologies of language and language learning, discourses of immigration and belonging, and the actual lived experiences of individual language learners. The analysis demonstrates how questions of educational access, economic stability, and social membership are all influenced by a range of social, political, and historical factors, particularly for recently arrived immigrants and refugees from war-torn African contexts. 相似文献
17.
18.
Alfred Mansour Larry P. Taylor Jeffrey L. Fine Robert C. Thompson Mary T. Hoversten Henry I. Mosberg Stanley J. Watson Huda Akil 《Journal of neurochemistry》1997,68(1):344-353
Abstract: Structural elements of the rat μ-opioid receptor important in ligand receptor binding and selectivity were examined using a site-directed mutagenesis approach. Five single amino acid mutations were made, three that altered conserved residues in the μ, δ, and κ receptors (Asn150 to Ala, His297 to Ala, and Tyr326 to Phe) and two designed to test for μ/δ selectivity (Ile198 to Val and Val202 to Ile). Mutation of His297 in transmembrane domain 6 (TM6) resulted in no detectable binding with [3 H]DAMGO (3 H-labeled d -Ala2 , N -Me-Phe4 ,Gly-ol5 -enkephalin), [3 H]bremazocine, or [3 H]ethylketocyclazocine. Mutation of Asn150 in TM3 produces a three- to 20-fold increase in affinity for the opioid agonists morphine, DAMGO, fentanyl, β-endorphin1–31 , JOM-13, deltorphin II, dynorphin1–13 , and U50,488, with no change in the binding of antagonists such as naloxone, naltrexone, naltrindole, and nor-binaltorphamine. In contrast, the Tyr326 mutation in TM7 resulted in a decreased affinity for a wide spectrum of μ, δ, and κ agonists and antagonists. Altering Val202 to Ile in TM4 produced no change on ligand affinity, but Ile198 to Val resulted in a four- to fivefold decreased affinity for the μ agonists morphine and DAMGO, with no change in the binding affinities of κ and δ ligands. 相似文献
19.
Detection thresholds for an increment in duration of a 10-kHz pure tone were determined in the NMRI mouse using a Go/NoGo-procedure and the method of constant stimuli. Thresholds for reference durations of 50, 100 and 200 ms were obtained presenting the signals at a fixed level or at a level varying by ±3 dB. Thresholds were determined using signal-detection theory (d=1.0 or d=1.8) and the criterion of 50% correct responses. For a fixed level, the average Weber fraction T/T (criterion of d=1.8) significantly decreased from 1.18 or 1.23 at reference durations of 50 or 100 ms, respectively, to 0.97 at a reference duration of 200 ms. Thresholds were on average reduced by 46.8 or 55.4% for the threshold criteria d=1 or 50% correct responses, respectively. There was no effect of randomizing the level on the discrimination threshold. Duration discrimination in the NMRI mouse does not follow Webers law. The results are consistent with a mechanism summing up neural impulses over the duration of the stimulus. The psychoacoustic data are compared with results obtained by Brand et al. (J Acoust Soc Am 51:1291–1223, 2000) on the representation of acoustic signal duration in the mouse inferior colliculus. 相似文献