共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Corticosterone excretion patterns and affiliative behavior over development in ravens (Corvus corax) 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Stöwe M Bugnyar T Schloegl C Heinrich B Kotrschal K Möstl E 《Hormones and behavior》2008,53(1):208-216
Averse effects of social stress may be buffered by the presence of social allies, which mainly has been demonstrated in mammals and recently also in birds. However, effects of socio-positive behavior prior to fledging in relation to corticosterone excretion in altricial birds have not been investigated yet. We here monitored corticosterone excretion patterns in three groups of hand raised juvenile ravens (n=5, 6 and 11) in the nest, post-fledging (May-July) and when ravens would be independent from their parents (September-November). We related these corticosterone excretion patterns to socio-positive behavior. Behavioral data were collected via focal sampling in each developmental period considered. We analyzed amounts of excreted immunoreactive corticosterone metabolites (CM) using enzyme immuno assays. We collected fecal samples in each developmental period considered and evaluated the most appropriate assay via an isolation stress experiment. Basal CM was significantly higher during the nestling period than post-fledging or when birds were independent. The time nestlings spent allopreening correlated negatively with mean CM. Post-fledging, individuals with higher CM levels sat close to (distance <50 cm) conspecifics more frequently and tended to preen them longer. When birds were independent and a stable rank hierarchy was established, dominant individuals were preened significantly longer than subordinates. These patterns observed in ravens parallel those described for primates, which could indicate that animal species living in a complex social environment may deal with social problems in a similar way that is not restricted to mammals or primates. 相似文献
3.
Individuals foraging in groups should develop behavioural tactics to optimise their gain. In novel feeding situations, predation risk and pressure of kleptoparasites may be particularly high and hence may constrain optimal foraging. To create a novel feeding situation, we offered common ravens (Corvus corax) equal numbers of either small (40 g) or large (160 g) pieces of meat on successive days, always in combination with the same novel object. During the first weeks, when ravens were still neophobic, small pieces were taken in larger numbers than large pieces. Intraspecific kleptoparasitism was more likely to occur when ravens carried large food items. It seems that initiating foragers were mainly innovative subdominants. Preference for small items might have decreased with increasing habituation because more dominants were then feeding directly at the source and hence were less likely to resort to kleptoparasitism as an alternative foraging tactic. Electronic Publication 相似文献
4.
Vagrant non-breeding ravens frequently attract conspecifics to rich ephemeral food sources. There, grouping may allow them to overcome the defence of territorial breeders. Here, we focus on ravens making use of regular food supplies in a game park, where they divert food from the provision of park animals. We investigated if ravens foraging in the Cumberland game park (Grünau, Austria) are attentive towards one another when they experience some unpredictability in food provisioning. We confronted a group of 30–50 ravens with two different treatments. Ten minutes ahead of the feeding of either wolves or wild boars we showed buckets containing pieces of meat to the ravens flying overhead. In the reliable cue treatment (RCT), the meat was placed next to one of the two enclosures, whereas in the unreliable cue treatment (UCT), the buckets were placed simultaneously in front of both enclosures though only in one of the enclosures were the animals fed 10 min later. Thus, during RCT but not during UCT, ravens could predict where food would become available. Only during UCT, ravens moved in large groups between the two feeding sites. Many ravens moving at the same time in the same direction may indicate some co-ordination in space and time, which is most likely achieved by social attraction among individuals. Furthermore, the number of ravens approaching and leaving, respectively, a feeding site cross-correlated with a temporary increase in the rate of a food-associated call, the yell. This suggests that in addition to watching each other, calling may have contributed to group formation. Possible benefits of group formation during food inspection are discussed. Received: 9 February 2000 / Received in revised form: 30 May 2000 / Accepted: 1 June 2000 相似文献
5.
6.
Ole Jakobsen 《Zoology in the Middle East.》2013,59(1):32-33
An adult male of Maynard's Longnose Sand Snake (Lytorhynchus maynardi) was collected on 24 July 2009 from the east of Sistan and Baluchistan Province in south-eastern Iran. The new locality is the westernmost record of this species in western Asia, and the first record for 42 years from Iran. Information on the geographic distribution of the genus Lytorhynchus Peters, 1863 in Iran, comparative morphology and some ecological data are presented here. 相似文献
7.
J. T. Pearson H. Tazawa 《Journal of comparative physiology. B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology》1999,169(4-5):256-262
The developmental patterns of mean heart rate (MHR) and instantaneous heart rate (IHR) were investigated in embryos and chicks
of altricial Corvuscorone and Corvus macrorhynchos. The MHR of embryos increased linearly with time from 250 beats · min−1 at mid-incubation to 290 beats · min−1 in hatchlings. MHR during the pipping period was maximal, but only marginally higher than in hatchlings. MHR was stable at
about 290–300 beats · min−1 during the 1st week after hatching. Spontaneous heart rate (HR) decelerations and accelerations were found in embryos and
chicks, disturbing the baseline HR with increasing frequency during development. However, the IHR accelerations developed
later and were less frequent than in precocial species. IHR and body temperature decreased during mild cold exposure (23–25 °C)
and IHR accelerations were reduced in nestlings during the 1st week. We suggest that the development of parasympathetic control
of HR in crows occurs at 60% of incubation, similar to precocial embryos, but sympathetic control may be delayed and suppressed
in contrast to precocial embryos.
Accepted: 3 March 1999 相似文献
8.
9.
Staffan Tamm 《Behavioural processes》1977,2(3):293-299
Social hierarchy was studied in two flocks (14 and 10 individuals, respectively) of captive jackdaws. Parent birds acquired high-ranking positions during the nesting period. Outside the breeding season the hierarchy seemed to be stable. Males generally dominated females. When the top-ranking male arrived at the feeding place it tended to supplant one of the feeding birds rather than choose an unoccupied site. 相似文献
10.
Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus) 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Seed AM Clayton NS Emery NJ 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2008,275(1641):1421-1429
Recent work has shown that captive rooks, like chimpanzees and other primates, develop cooperative alliances with their conspecifics. Furthermore, the pressures hypothesized to have favoured social intelligence in primates also apply to corvids. We tested cooperative problem-solving in rooks to compare their performance and cognition with primates. Without training, eight rooks quickly solved a problem in which two individuals had to pull both ends of a string simultaneously in order to pull in a food platform. Similar to chimpanzees and capuchin monkeys, performance was better when within-dyad tolerance levels were higher. In contrast to chimpanzees, rooks did not delay acting on the apparatus while their partner gained access to the test room. Furthermore, given a choice between an apparatus that could be operated individually over one that required the action of two individuals, four out of six individuals showed no preference. These results may indicate that cooperation in chimpanzees is underpinned by more complex cognitive processes than that in rooks. Such a difference may arise from the fact that while both chimpanzees and rooks form cooperative alliances, chimpanzees, but not rooks, live in a variable social network made up of competitive and cooperative relationships. 相似文献
11.
12.
In this study,both long PCR and conserved primers walking sequencing methods were used to determine the complete sequence of the of Pyrgilauda ruficollis mitochondrial genome(KC836121).The results showed that the complete mitochondrial genome of P.ruficollis is 16909 bp in length with 55.0%A+T content,harboring the typical 37 genes.The mitogenome had the same gene order with that of Podoces hendersoni.All protein coding genes started with ATG codon,except ND3 with GTG.For the stop codon usage,most genes terminate with codons TAA or TAG,but ND5 terminated with AGA,while ND1 and COI genes with AGG,and both the genes COIII and ND4 have an incomplete termination codon(T).The secondary structures of 22 tRNA genes were also predicted,showing that all tRNAs can form typical clover-leaf secondary structures,except for the tRNASer(AGN)which loses the DHU arm,while tRNAPhe harbor an extra nucleotide inserted in the TψC arm.The predicted secondary structures of 12S rRNA and16S rRNA exhibit 47 helices in 4 domains and 60 helices in 6 domains respectively.The control region of P.ruficollis with the length of 1 305 bp was located between tRNAGlu and tRNAPhe,and typical domains of which could be found as other bird groups.Using the data from 13 mitochondrial protein-coding genes,results of a final phylogenetic analysis strongly supports the traditional view that P.ruficollis is closely related with Passeridae and Fringillidae. 相似文献
13.
It has been suggested that affiliated social relations may facilitate information transfer between individuals. We here tested this rarely examined hypothesis with juvenile and adult jackdaws (Corvus monedula) in three stimulus enhancement tasks, both in a non-food context (experiment 1) and in a food context (experiments 2 and 3). We first show that siblings and pair partners maintain stronger bonded social relations than do non-siblings and non-pair partners. We therefore tested individuals in sibling and non-sibling dyads and, later in ontogeny, in pair and non-pair dyads. Jackdaws either did not learn from any other conspecific (experiment 1), or they learned from non-affiliated individuals (non-siblings, non-pair partners in experiments 2 and 3). This may be related to two main characteristics of jackdaws' affiliated relationships. First, affiliates share food at a high rate and may rely on their knowledgeable partners to secure food rather than learning from them. Second, affiliates spend most time in close spatial proximity to each other which increases the probability that they simultaneously experience occurrences in their environment. Hence, spatially more distant individuals, which are more likely to be non-affiliated, face different foraging situations and may therefore provide more relevant information which may lead to selective social learning. 相似文献
14.
C. N. JOHNSON 《Journal of Zoology》1989,217(2):267-280
Male red–necked wallabies established dominance relationships by fighting, and dominance rankings among males were positively correlated with body size. Females became outstandingly attractive to males during the week preceding mating, and males, who normally ranged widely and solitarily in search of mates, gathered in groups around such females. It is suggested that females, by prolonging their advertisement of oestrus, and thereby inciting sustained competitive interactions among the males who were attracted to them, contributed to a bias in the distribution of male mating success in favour of very large dominant individuals. High–ranking males mated within discrete but undefended areas of absolute dominance ('dominions'). Direct interactions between dominion–holding males were rare, but males just below dominion–holding status often fought with one another, and were frequently harassed by dominional males. 相似文献
15.
16.
We describe cooperative hunting by Brown-necked Raven (Corvus ruficollis) on Egyptian Mastigure (Uromastyx aegyptius) in the Arava Valley, Israel. At first, in all nine observed hunts, the ravens were observed to be in the vicinity and were
seen simultaneously. The Mastigure was almost always at a distance from the burrow entrance and either foraging on the bushes
or lying flattened on a bush sunning itself. The attack started when a circling pair of ravens flew in at high speed and landed
on the entrance of the burrow, cutting off the escape route of the lizard. Following this maneuver, the other ravens attacked
the lizard. The ravens pecked randomly at the most exposed part of the lizard, eventually causing its death. Only when the
lizard was evidently dead did the two individuals that blocked the escape route join in the feeding ravens. 相似文献
17.
Melhania steudneri (Sterculiaceae) new record for Egypt,and a synopsis of the Sterculiaceae in Egypt
The cultivated and indigenous taxa of Sterculiaceae in Egypt are systematically revised. This revealed the presence of 15 species, belonging to 10 genera, accepted as cultivated taxa and 6 indigenous species, belonging to 4 genera, restricted to the southeastern corner of Egypt; of which Melhania steudneri Schweinf. is a new record to the flora of Egypt. For the investigated taxa valid name, synonyms, types, general distribution and representative specimens are given. Identification keys for the determination of all genera and species are also provided. 相似文献
18.
19.
Colpocephalum holzenthali, a new species of chewing louse, is described from a series of lice collected in Peru from Micrastur ruficollis zonothorax, the barred forest-falcon. Colpocephalum holzenthali is most similar to members of the Colpocephalum polybori species group. 相似文献
20.
We investigated nucleotide sequences of the mitochondrial DNA control region to describe natural genetic variations and to assess the relationships between subpopulations of the brown bear Ursus arctos on Hokkaido Island, Japan. Using the polymerase chain reaction product-direct sequencing technique, partial sequences (about 930 bases) of the control region were determined for 56 brown bears sampled throughout Hokkaido Island. A sequence alignment revealed that the brown bear control region included a variable sequence on the 5' side and a repetitive region on the 3' side. Phylogenetic trees reconstructed from the 5' variable region (696-702 bases) exhibited 17 haplotypes, which were clustered into three groups (Clusters A, B, and C). The distribution of each group did not overlap with those of the others, and the three different areas were located in separate mountainous forests of Hokkaido Island. Furthermore, most of the phylogenetically close haplotypes within each group were distributed geographically close to each other. In addition, the 3' repetitive region (arrays of 10 bases) exhibited a much faster mutation rate than the 5' variable region, resulting in heteroplasmy. Such mitochondrial DNA divergence in each group could have occurred after the brown bears migrated from the continent to Hokkaido and became fixed in the different areas. 相似文献