首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Crickets produce stridulated sounds by rubbing their forewings together. The calling song of the cricket species Eneoptera guyanensis Chopard, 1931 alternates two song sections, at low and high dominant frequencies, corresponding to two distinct sections of the stridulatory file. In the present study we address the complex acoustic behavior of E. guyanensis by integrating information on the peculiar morphology of the stridulatory file, the acoustic analysis of its calling song and the forewing movements during sound production. The results show that even if E. guyanensis matches the normal cricket functioning for syllable production, the stridulation involves two different closing movements, corresponding to two types of syllables, allowing the plectrum to hit alternately each differentiated section of the file. Transition syllables combine high and low frequencies and are emitted by a complete forewing closure over the whole file. The double-teeth section of the stridulatory file may be used as a multiplier for the song frequency because of the morphological multiplication due to the double teeth, but also because of an increase of wing velocity when this file section is used. According to available phylogenetic and acoustic data, this complex stridulation may have evolved in a two-step process.  相似文献   

2.
In choosing a breeding partner, females in many animal species select between available males on the basis of several signalling traits. Some theoretical models of signalling evolution predict that multiple ornaments convey specific information on different aspects of male quality, such as current nutritional condition. We investigated the effect of nutrition on the calling song of male field crickets Gryllus campestris. This song is a multicomponent sexually selected signal. Adult males were kept on one of three feeding regimes, which resulted in significant differences in body condition between experimental groups. We found significant increases in calling rate and chirp rate and a significant decrease in interchirp duration with increasing food level. Other song characters, such as chirp duration, syllable number, chirp intensity and carrier frequency, were not affected by the food treatment. Furthermore, carrier frequency was correlated with harp area, which is an index of structural size in adult males. The calling song of the field cricket may thus serve as a multicomponent sexual signal, which contains discrete information on past growth and juvenile development as well as present nutritional condition. Copyright 2003 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.   相似文献   

3.
Although female mating preferences are a focus of current controversy,little detailed information exists on female preferences withinnatural populations. In the field cricket Gryllus integer, malecalls attract sexually receptive females, and females preferentiallymove toward male calls with longer calling bouts (periods ofcalling containing no pause greater than 0.10 s in real time).This study investigated female preferences for other variablesof the male song, including syllable period, chirp pause, andnumber of syllables per chirp. Male song was measured in thefield to determine mean values for each variable in nature.Female preferences were determined using a locomotor-compensatordevice, on which females ran in response to sequential playbacksof synthesized male song. Mean female preferences correspondedroughly to mean male song variables. Nonetheless, females variedgreatly in their responses to synthesized calls differing insyllable period, syllable number, and chirp pause. Moreover,individual females who were more selective for any one variablealso tended to be more selective for others. These results showthat females may differ from one another in their mating preferencesand degrees of selectivity, even within a single population.  相似文献   

4.
Mate finding in the phaneropterid bushcricket Ancistrura nigrovittata is achieved by a duet, where the female replies with a short sound to the male song. In experiments with artificial song models we analysed the parameters necessary for eliciting a female response. A verse of the male song consists of a group of 5–9 syllabes which after an interval of about 400 ms is followed by a final syllable. The female response was shown to depend on two processes: (i) recognition of the syllable group as belonging to a conspecific male and (ii) perception of the final syllable as a trigger. Critical parameters for the recognition process are the duration of syllables and syllable pauses, as well as the number of syllables in a group. However, even with an optimal syllable group, the response probability still depends on the interval between the syllable group and the final syllable. The female only responds when the final syllable of the male song occurs within a 250 ms long time window begining approximately 250 ms after the end of the male's syllable group. Her reply consists of a single tick, which follows the male's final syllable with a latency of only 25 ms.  相似文献   

5.
6.
ABSTRACT. In female Gryllus campestris L., three functional types of ascending auditory intemeurones have been studied by recording from them extracellularly in the split cervical connectives using suction electrodes. Type 1 neurones are characterized by an optimal sensitivity to the carrier frequency of the species calling song (4–5 kHz). They copy the syllable and pause structure of the call at all intensities. The patterned spike discharge is observable at least 8 dB above absolute threshold. With suprathreshold stimulation, the neurones exhibit maximal responses (number of spikes/chirp) around the carrier frequency. The intensity response curves are approximately linear in the range of 40–90 dB SPL. The envelope of each syllable is reflected by a corresponding change in the firing rate, and syllable periods of 24ms and longer are resolved. This type can be considered as a neural correlate for phonotactic behaviour of the female where the syllable period has been found to be the most important temporal parameter. Type 2 neurones are most sensitive in the range of 4–6 and 11–13 kHz. They copy the syllable and pause structure of the species calling song at low and moderate intensities. However, the spikes invade the intersyllable pauses, when stimulated with the calling song at higher intensities (above 85 dB). This is particularly apparent at the onset of a chirp series. The slope of the intensity—response curve mimics that of type 1 units. The neurones cannot follow syllable periods shorter than 32 ms. Type 3 neurones differ from types 1 and 2 by a rather broad-band sensitivity in the range of 3–16 kHz, and in copying the chirp as a whole. Even at low stimulus intensities, the intersyllable pauses are filled with spikes, and information about the syllable—pause structure is lost. Stimulation with suprathreshold intensities gives rise to a rather uniform, broad-band response without distinctive peaks. The intensity—response curve is characterized by a higher absolute threshold, and by the reduction in the response magnitude starting above 70–80 dB. These units are not suitable for copying the calling song temporal structure in detail, but would indicate the chirping rhythm. Their strong response in the range of the species courtship song carrier frequency make them suitable to copy the courtship song.  相似文献   

7.
The hypothesis that female sonic stimulus may evoke male pheromone release in a behavioural interaction analogous to the known male sonic stimulus of female pheromone release, was confirmed in Dendroctonus pseudotsugae, and also in D. brevicomis. In both species known male-produced substances collected from males stimulated by recorded female stridulation were identified by coupled gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. In a second test with D. pseudotsugae, male pheromone release during recorded female stridulation was evident in the change of male stridulation from the simple attractant chirp to the interrupted chirp, which is known to result from a medium concentration of 3,2-MCH. Also, the D. pseudotsugae male attractant chirp was synthesised with an electronic pulse generator and used to evoke pheromone release. It is concluded that the antiaggregative pheromone of this species is released by each sex at the sonic stimulus of the other sex.  相似文献   

8.
The phonotactic response of cricket females was investigated on a locomotion compensator to determine the temporal parameters of the male's calling song which are important for species recognition. Two sympatric species (Teleogryllus commodus, T. oceanicus) that show different syllable periods in the chirp and trill parts of their calling songs were used. By their responses T. commodus females exhibited two temporal filters for syllable periods, which were tuned to the species-specific syllable periods occurring during chirp and trill. For song recognition both filters had to be activated and for both a minimum number of three to five consecutive syllable periods was necessary. In contrast, T. oceanicus females showed only one sharply tuned filter corresponding to the chirp part of the male's calling song. This filter was sufficient for calling song recognition. Syllable periods of the trill part also influenced calling song recognition, but these played only a minor role. Carrier frequency was also important for positive phonotaxis. Calling song recognition by T. commodus females is largely based on central nervous processing, while for T. oceanicus both peripheral frequency filtering and central temporal filtering is important. Accepted: 17 January 1997  相似文献   

9.
The song of the male bushcricket Ancistrura nigrovittata consists of a sequence of verses. Each verse comprises a syllable group, plus, after about 400 ms a single syllable serving as a trigger for the female response song. The carrier frequency of the male song spectrum peaks at around 15 kHz, while the female song peaks at around 27 kHz. The thresholds of female responses to models of male songs are lowest for song frequencies between 12 and 16 kHz and therefore correspond to the male song spectrum. The threshold curve of the female response to the trigger syllable at different frequencies has the same shape as the tuning for the syllable group. Phonotactic thresholds of male Ancistrura nigrovittata to synthetic female responses at different frequencies are lowest between 24 and 28 kHz and thereby correspond to the female song spectrum and clearly differ from female response thresholds. Activity of the tympanic fibre bundle of both sexes is most sensitive between 15 and 35 kHz and therefore not specifically tuned to the partner's song. Individual behavioural thresholds have their minimum within 10 dB of the values of tympanic thresholds.  相似文献   

10.
The structure of male songs and the timing of female replies with respect to the male songs are described for four species of the palaearctic bushcricket genus Barbitistes (B. constrictus, B. ocskayi, B. serricauda, B. yersini). In a male song, 3 to 16 syllables form a chirp followed by a trigger syllable after a longer interval. The trigger syllable releases a female reply with a latency of 30 to 50 ms in all four species. In B. serricauda songs, there is no clearly separated trigger syllable. Instead, the first syllable of a chirp functions as a trigger syllable. Some B. serricauda males may produce a short female-type syllable just at the moment, when a female would reply. The possible function of such a syllable is acoustical mimicry. When comparing at least two song parameters, each species occupies a specific combination of values. According to the overlap of parameters a close phylogenetic relationship between B. constrictus and B. serricauda and between B. ocskayi and B. yersini is assumed. This interpretation is compared with a hypothesis based on morphological investigations.  相似文献   

11.
Male courtship songs have two functions in species recognition and intraspecific mate choice. Female preference might thus exert different types of selection pressure on male song traits. We used a combination of acoustic mate choice experiments and statistical analyses to examine how traits of the calling songs of male nightingale grasshoppers,Chorthippus biguttulus , are influenced by different sexual selection pressures. We recorded calling songs of males and tested their attractiveness to females in acoustic mate choice experiments. The attractiveness values were a good estimate of the potential male mating success. In experiments with a pair of males, females copulated significantly more often with the male that had the higher attractiveness value. To detect directional, stabilizing, disruptive or correlative selection acting on male song properties we used linear and nonlinear regressions between male song traits and female response behaviour. Three signal traits were revealed to be under directional selection: song loudness, pause to syllable ratio and the mean duration of gaps within syllables. A nonlinear regression testing for correlative selection showed that a fourth song trait, rhythm, in combination with mean gap duration was also important for female mate choice. With these traits and trait combinations we were able to explain 42% of the variance in attractiveness between males. Since we found no evidence for stabilizing selection, but ample evidence for directional selection, we conclude that selection on the traits examined is related to mate choice mainly in the context of intraspecific sexual selection and probably less so in species recognition. Copyright 2003 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.   相似文献   

12.
《Zoology (Jena, Germany)》2014,117(5):329-336
Many insects exhibit secondary defence mechanisms upon contact with a predator, such as defensive sound production or regurgitation of gut contents. In the tettigoniid Poecilimon ornatus, both males and females are capable of sound production and of regurgitation. However, wing stridulatory structures for intraspecific acoustic communication evolved independently in males and females, and may result in different defence sounds. Here we investigate in P. ornatus whether secondary defence behaviours, in particular defence sounds, show sex-specific differences. The male defence sound differs significantly from the male calling song in that it has a longer syllable duration and a higher number of impulses per syllable. In females, the defence sound syllables are also significantly longer than the syllables of their response song to the male calling song. In addition, the acoustic disturbance stridulation differs notably between females and males as both sexes exhibit different temporal patterns of the defence sound. Furthermore, males use defence sounds more often than females. The higher proportion of male disturbance stridulation is consistent with a male-biased predation risk during calling and phonotactic behaviour. The temporal structures of the female and male defence sounds support a deimatic function of the startling sound in both females and males, rather than an adaptation for a particular temporal pattern. Independently of the clear differences in sound defence, no difference in regurgitation of gut content occurs between the sexes.  相似文献   

13.
Male crickets (Gryllus campestris L.) mounted so that their wings and abdomen could move freely were induced to stridulate by brain lesion. During the song the activity of single neurones was recorded extracellularly in a cervical connective. Nine distinct spike patterns were observed. Patterns I and II tend to copy the chirp as a whole rather than the onset of the syllables (the recorded potentials of the wing-opener muscle M99 marked the syllable onset). The other patterns reflect the syllabic structure. Each, in its own way, marks the various syllables with different numbers of spikes. The delay of the spike response is different for each pattern. Some patterns, but not others, also reflect the beginning or end of the song, or the abdominal expiratory activity. One neurone also responds in correlation with muscle discharges typical of the courtship song. In some of the patterns it is evident that there is a stronger correlation with the closer muscle (M90) discharge than with the opener muscle discharge. Activation by auditory self-stimulation by way of the tympanal organs can be ruled out for all patterns. It is possible that patterns I–V are induced by afferent activity coupled to the wing movement. Patterns VI–VIII are probably copies of motor signals ascending from the thoracic song-pattern generators to the head ganglia. It is evident that the head ganglia have detailed information as to the motor output for stridulation and abdominal expiration.  相似文献   

14.
15.
Male files homozygous for the gene ebony11 are partially blind, and at a disadvantage in competitive mating. The courtship of the mutant males is deficient in wing vibration stimulation, which is characterized by a low proportion of sine song and a high intra-pulse frequency. Males heterozygous for ebony have normal vision, but show an increase in courtship song, and are superior in competitive mating to wild type males. The auditory characteristics of courtship song produced by heterozygous males are indistinguishable from those of wild type, and their superiority in competitive mating success is due to overdominance involving this specific element of male courtship behaviour.  相似文献   

16.
Six female song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) and six female swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) were treated with oestradiol, and their response to song measured by frequency and intensity of copulation-solicitation display. Test stimuli were synthetic songs composed of either song sparrow or swamp sparrow syllables assembled in either song-sparrow-like or swamp-sparrow-like temporal patterns. Female song sparrows responded preferentially to songs containing their own species' syllables and to songs containing their own species' temporal patterns. Swamp sparrows were also sensitive to both syllable type and temporal, pattern, in contrast to male swamp sparrows, which show no preference for swamp sparrow temporal patterns.  相似文献   

17.
Summary Temperature effects on calling song production and recognition were investigated in the North American field cricket, Gryllus firmus. Temporal parameters of field-recorded G. firmus calling song are strongly affected by temperature. Chirp rate and syllable rate increase, by factors of 4 and 2, respectively, as linear functions of temperature over the range in which these animals sing in the field (12°–30 °C). Temperature affects syllable duration to a lesser extent, and does not influence calling song carrier frequency. Female phonotactic preference, measured on a spherical treadmill in the laboratory, also changes with temperature such that warmer females prefer songs with faster chirp and syllable rates. Best phonotaxis, measured as accuracy of orientation to the sound source, and highest walking velocity, occur in response to temperature-matched songs at 15°, 21°, and 30 °C. Experiments under semi-natural conditions in an outdoor arena revealed that females perform phonotaxis at temperatures as low as 13 °C. Taken together, the song and phonotaxis data demonstrate that this communication system is temperature coupled. A strategy is outlined by which temperature coupling may be exploited to test hypotheses about the organization of neural networks subserving song recognition.Abbreviations CP chirp period - SP syllable period - SD syllable duration  相似文献   

18.
This research focused on how adult female brown‐headed cowbirds, Molothrus ater, regulate social feedback on a group level to shape the development of male song. Specifically, females produce rapid wing movements in response to male song, termed ‘wing strokes,’ which have been shown to shape male song and predict song quality. These effects have been documented in captive dyads and triads, but not in more naturalistic flocks, where song development actually occurs. Here, we studied wing stroking in small seminatural flocks of differing female‐to‐male ratios. Despite differences in the number of females and their social selectivity, the same pattern of female feedback emerged in seven of eight flocks: One female produced the majority of wing strokes to male song, making her the primary wing stroker in her flock. Previous studies on large flocks have demonstrated females to facilitate male song improvisation and development if they exhibited higher social selectivity by approaching immature males less. Here, we found that primary wing strokers were indeed more socially selective than non‐primary wing strokers. This research is the first to document social stimulation being facilitated at the group level to ensure that more highly selective females deliver the most feedback.  相似文献   

19.
In katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigonioidea) of the subfamily Phaneropterinae females ready to mate initiate a duet, announcing her position to the male singer, but also potentially to eavesdropping rivals. In many species the male seems to defend the communication by adding self-produced imitations of a female response. If these signals occur within the male sensory time-window after the female song, they can disturb the orientation of rivals. In two species of the genus Gonatoxia, males and females use short, relatively narrow-banded sounds (width 2–7 kHz 10 dB below peak). Male song and female response, however, differ considerably in peak frequency. In G. maculata, the peak frequency of the last part of the male song (13 kHz) is between that of the first part (15 kHz) and the female response (9 kHz), in G. helleri the last part (9 kHz; assumed imitation) and the female song are identical in peak frequency and by a factor two lower than the first part (19 kHz). The male stridulatory file of this species is correspondingly modified and differs from all other members of the genus. The imitation of spectral properties of the female response is not known from any other katydid.  相似文献   

20.
To examine the concept of female courtship summation in Drosophila melanogaster, two experiments previously reported in the literature, the first involving repeated matings of females and the second progressive removal of the males' wings, were repeated. The present results do not convincingly support the concept of female summation of stimuli provided by the males' courtship. The results of the first experiment also refute the idea that low male courtship intensity leads to long courtships. These results also fail to support an earlier suggestion that females summate the sine rather than pulse song component of the males' wing vibration. Instead, the variation in courtship duration appeared to result from the inverse hyperbolic relationship between the male latency to courtship and the subsequent courtship duration. Thus short male latencies led to longer courtship durations. This is interpreted as resulting from a female latency period during which the female is too agitated to receive the male's courtship, and after which she mates upon recognizing the male as conspecific. In addition, very long courtships largely resulted from additional agitation of the female by the male's courtship. Long courtships therefore appear to be an artifact of the experimental situation and the established concept of female courtship summation, which is supposed to explain them, is unnecessary. The implications of this conclusion are discussed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号