首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
The relationship between the latencies and amplitudes of the N1 and P2 components of the visual evoked potential (VEP) and the psychophysiological state of the brain immediately preceding the time of the stimulus has been investigated in 7 male subjects. Power spectral measures in the delta, theta, alpha and beta bands of the 1 sec pre-stimulus EEG were used to assess the brain state, and low intensity flashes, delivered randomly between 2 and 6 whole seconds, were used as the stimuli. Trials were ranked separately according to the relative amounts of pre-stimulus power in each EEG band and were partitioned into groups by an equal pre-stimulus spectral power criterion. Averaged EPs were computed from these groups and multiple regression analysis was used to relate pre-stimulus spectral power values to EP features. Five of the 7 subjects displayed consistent increases in N1-P2 amplitude as a function of increasing pre-stimulus relative alpha power. The between-subjects effect of pre-stimulus EEG on N1 latency was small, but was moderate for P2 latency (both significant). Both N1 and P2 latency were found to decrease with increasing amounts of pre-stimulus relative delta and theta power.  相似文献   

2.
Binaural interaction (BI) components in brain-stem auditory evoked potential (BAEP) and their changes with stimulus intensity and repetition rate were examined in human adult. Seven BI components were identified, which occurred between the latency range of 5 and 11 ms and coincided consistently with the latency range of BAEP waves IV–VII. Waves DV and DVII, occurring at the downslopes of BAEP-waves V and VII, respectively, were the two most prominent and reproducible BI components. Wave DVII existed consistently at high, moderate and, in most cases, low stimulus intensities, suggesting that this component is neurogenic although acoustic cross-talk may account for a part of its waveform at high stimulus intensities. The latencies of all BI components increased as a function of decreasing stimulus intensity, while the interpeak intervals, especially DV–DVII, were essentially constant at different intensity levels. The amplitudes of BI components decreased slightly with decreasing intensity. As click repetition rate increased, BI wave latencies and interpeak intervals increased slightly and amplitudes decreased slightly. When repetition rate increased to above 20/s, BI components became poorly differentiated. Lower repetition rates, e.g. 10/s, are therefore preferred for routine derivation of the BI. The changes in the latency and amplitude of BI components with stimulus intensity and repetition rate were associated or concomitant with those of the corresponding BAEP components in monaural and binaural potentials. In view of the concomitant relationship between BI and BAEP latency, we designate BI components in association with the corresponding BAEP components.  相似文献   

3.
Lengthening the time interval between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus increases the number of active avoidance conditioned responses in subjects that have been trained to a stable level of performance in many previous conditioning sessions. In the present research, rats chosen from a population specially selected for low rates of avoidance conditioning have been used. In addition to this characteristic, subjects were chosen for the exhibition of an apparent absence of retention from one day to another. The dependency of the number of conditioned responses on the time interval between conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus may lead to wrong evaluation of the subjects' conditioning level. In fact, the level of conditioning may be attributed to either learning or memory processes when in many cases it is determined only by the latency time of the conditioned response. The conditioned response has no possibility of manifesting itself when its latency time exceeds in length the time interval between conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus.  相似文献   

4.
M Cornella  S Leung  S Grimm  C Escera 《PloS one》2012,7(8):e43604
Auditory deviance detection in humans is indexed by the mismatch negativity (MMN), a component of the auditory evoked potential (AEP) of the electroencephalogram (EEG) occurring at a latency of 100-250 ms after stimulus onset. However, by using classic oddball paradigms, differential responses to regularity violations of simple auditory features have been found at the level of the middle latency response (MLR) of the AEP occurring within the first 50 ms after stimulus (deviation) onset. These findings suggest the existence of fast deviance detection mechanisms for simple feature changes, but it is not clear whether deviance detection among more complex acoustic regularities could be observed at such early latencies. To test this, we examined the pre-attentive processing of rare stimulus repetitions in a sequence of tones alternating in frequency in both long and middle latency ranges. Additionally, we introduced occasional changes in the interaural time difference (ITD), so that a simple-feature regularity could be examined in the same paradigm. MMN was obtained for both repetition and ITD deviants, occurring at 150 ms and 100 ms after stimulus onset respectively. At the level of the MLR, a difference was observed between standards and ITD deviants at the Na component (20-30 ms after stimulus onset), for 800 Hz tones, but not for repetition deviants. These findings suggest that detection mechanisms for deviants to simple regularities, but not to more complex regularities, are already activated in the MLR range, supporting the view that the auditory deviance detection system is organized in a hierarchical manner.  相似文献   

5.
The electrodermal response (EDR) of frogs to various acoustic stimuli was measured in the form of the skin potential response (SPR). There was no correlation between the polarity of the SPR and the intensity of the stimuli. When different frequencies were presented at the same intensity, the amplitude of the SPR to each was inversely proportional to the sound pressure at which that frequency just elicited an SPR. The amplitude of the sound-induced SPR increased monotonically with increasing sound pressure. The latency of the SPR decreased with increasing intensity of the acoustic stimulus. Acoustic stimuli repeated at intervals of 1 and 2 min elicited responses with progressively decreased amplitude and increased latency; with 4 min intervals there was little habituation. Fatigue participates to only a very slight extent in reducing the amplitude of the SPR and increasing its latency. The results are compared with the published data on frogs and mammals, including humans.  相似文献   

6.
Stimulus duration is an important feature of visual stimulation. In the present study, response properties of bullfrog ON-OFF retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) in exposure to different visual stimulus durations were studied. By using a multi-electrode recording system, spike discharges from ON-OFF RGCs were simultaneously recorded, and the cells’ ON and OFF responses were analyzed. It was found that the ON response characteristics, including response latency, spike count, as well as correlated activity and relative latency between pair-wise cells, were modulated by different light OFF intervals, while the OFF response characteristics were modulated by different light ON durations. Stimulus information carried by the ON and OFF responses was then analyzed, and it was found that information about different light ON durations was more carried by transient OFF response, whereas information about different light OFF intervals were more carried by transient ON response. Meanwhile, more than 80 % information about stimulus durations was carried by firing rate. These results suggest that ON-OFF RGCs are sensitive to different stimulus durations, and they can efficiently encode the information about visual stimulus duration by firing rate.  相似文献   

7.
Latency to bite and spontaneous rasping activity in the absenceof food were measured. On presentation of a food stimulus (sucrosecrystals) to Lymnaea, latency to bite was reduced for c. 15min (increased food arousal). There was a progressive decreasein latency to bite and subsequent interbite intervals duringa meal. However, while hungry (2-day starved) snails showedno difference in latency to bite compared with well-fed controls,the number of spontaneous rasps made in the absence of foodprior to the food stimulus increased. Spontaneous rasps wereorganised in bouts. Log survivorship curves showed these tobe controlled by 2 processes generating within- and between-outintervals. The probability of within-bout intervals occurringincreased in hungry snails. These data suggest that it is importantto consider ‘level of activation’ as well as responsivenesscriteria (latency to bite) when assessing food arousal.  相似文献   

8.
Unit responses in the lateral geniculate body of cats to photic stimuli of different contrast were investigated. The number of spikes in the initial phase of the responses (the first 30–45 msec) was found not to change at first, but then to decrease with an increase in the intensity of background illumination. The background intensity starting from which the response diminishes was shown to increase with an increase in the intensity of the test stimulus. The unit response is a linear function of the logarithm of stimulus contrast if the contrast is changed through variation of the intensity of the test stimulus. If contrast increases on account of a decrease in the intensity of background illumination the responses first increase and then remain unchanged. The range of contrasts within which the response is a linear function is narrowed if the intensity of the test stimulus is reduced. Counting the number of spikes in different time intervals of the response (t) showed that the greater the value of t (within the first 70–90 msec of the response) the steeper the curve of the number of spikes as a function of contrast. The Weber-Fechner law applies in the receptive field of the lateral geniculate body. The results are compared with those of some psychophysiological experiments.I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Leningrad. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 9, No. 3, pp. 267–274, May–June, 1977.  相似文献   

9.
Evoked potentials from the major longitudinal nerve cords of Notoplana acticola are potentiated if the preparation is decerebrated or if certain nerves in the ventral submuscular plexus are severed. Concomitant with an increase in amplitude of the response (in some preparations over fourfold) is a decrease in latency of the response and a decrease in the threshold stimulus intensity needed to evoke activity. Evoked activity can also be depressed with moderate increases in stimulus intensity and is induced some distance from the recording site. The depressant effects can be lifted if the brain is bisected, if the contralateral nerve VI is severed close to the brain, and if the commissures between the two nerves VI are cut. The depressant effects of nerves V and VI are additive. Depression and the subsequent lifting of the inhibition occur in Ringer baths containing either normal sea water or equal mixtures of sea water and isotonic MgCl2. The possibility of inhibitory synapses immune to high concentrations of magnesium ions is discussed.  相似文献   

10.
The time lapse (latent period) between a hyperpolarizing pulse and anodic break response was investigated in single myelinated (frog) and giant axons (squid). The latent period was found to first decrease when the stimulating current intensity was gradually increased beyond threshold. However, for higher stimulus intensities the latent period increased and became a direct function of stimulus intensity. These findings were shown to be consistent with the Hodgkin-Huxley axon model.The initial decrease in latency was shown to be related to the changes in potassium current. The following increase in latency was due to the passive cable properties of the fibres. The increased tendency of sensory and other fibers to fire repetitively is analysed in view of the above findings.  相似文献   

11.
There are several different strategies to control the timing of a stimulus with respect to the ongoing discharge during the recording of neuronal stimulus-response characteristics. One possible strategy consists of delivering stimuli in such a way that a constant pre-stimulus spike density is reached. Another strategy enforces spike application with a constant stimulus latency after a spontaneous discharge. In this paper the sensitivity of these different strategies for statistical verification of small excitatory response components was investigated. It was found that the difference between observed poststimulus spike distribution and expected spike distribution under the null hypothesis of no stimulus effect was larger using a constant-stimulus-latency (CSL) strategy with an appropriate value for the stimulus latency. Thus, the statistical verification of neuronal response components is clearly facilitated if a CSL strategy is used. This superiority of the CSL strategy is marked, especially for small excitations at neurons discharging slowly with low discharge variability.  相似文献   

12.
Amplitude-temporal analysis was carried out of the EP components of the visual and motor areas elicited by neutral (diffuse light) and structural (checker board pattern) stimuli in different situations, defined by instruction. Interserial comparisons showed that at any instruction, the latency of the first EP component of the motor areas is reduced; as a result it can appear here simultaneously with the EP of the visual areas. At the instruction involving the subject in the process of active change of perception, activation of the right hemisphere, including the motor area, is manifest by EP parameters, while the right occipital area is activated in response to the structural stimulus, and the left one--in response to the neutral stimulus. At complication of the stimulus or instruction, the period is prolonged when the latency of EP components of the motor area is shorter than the latency of the isopolar components of the visual area--from 120 to 150 ms in response to the neutral stimuli and the neutral with their counting; from 90 to 150 ms in response to the structural stimuli; from 80 to 210 ms in response to the neutral stimuli with mental representation of the structural one.  相似文献   

13.
Experiment 1 elicited the P1, N1, P2, and N2 components of the long latency auditory evoked potential (AEP) using a 1000 Hz tone presented at 30, 50, or 70 dB SPL and 1-, 3-, or 5-second inter-stimulus intervals to assess the relative effects of the combination of these variables on component amplitude and latency. Four blocks of 16 tone presentations each were recorded from each subject to determine if changes in the AEP would occur because of short-term habituation. Both stimulus factors interacted significantly in a systematic fashion for the amplitude measures, with increases in latency also associated with increases in intensity and inter-stimulus interval. Only minor changes across the four trial blocks for either the amplitude or latency measures were observed over the various stimulus presentation conditions. Experiment 2 employed the same tone stimulus presented at 50 dB SPL and a 3-second inter-stimulus interval. Eight blocks of 64 trials were recorded from each subject on each day for four days to investigate long-term habituation effects. No substantial changes in any of the component amplitudes or latencies were obtained across the 32 trial blocks. It was concluded that intensity and inter-stimulus interval interact to determine AEP amplitude as well as latency values and that the constituent components do not change appreciably with repeated stimulus presentations, even after several days.  相似文献   

14.
Territorial male bullfrogs ( Rana catesbeiana) display lower levels of aggression toward familiar territorial neighbors compared to unfamiliar individuals based on the perception of individual differences in vocalizations. Stimulus-specific habituation elicited by repeated exposures to a neighbor's vocalizations is believed to play a role in mediating the low levels of aggression between neighbors. The present study describes habituation in multiple components of the bullfrog territorial aggressive response and examines whether these separate response components habituate at similar or different rates. In response to repeated broadcasts of synthetic bullfrog advertisement calls in a field playback experiment, the numbers of aggressive calls and movements, the latency to the first aggressive call, and the distance approached toward the playback speaker exhibited significant response decrements that developed at nearly identical rates. All four responses exhibited recovery upon subsequent broadcasts of a novel stimulus simulating a different individual. In contrast, the number of advertisement calls did not exhibit significant decrements with repeated playbacks. The patterns of stimulus-specific response decrement were inconsistent with explanations such as sensory adaptation, effector fatigue, non-specific changes in motivation, and a simple form of auditory habituation. Possible mechanisms for the observed plasticity in aggression are discussed in the context of anuran communication.  相似文献   

15.
When uncorrelated random noise signals presented to the two ears suddenly become identical (coherent), a centrally located sound image is abruptly perceived and long latency scalp potentials are evoked. When the same signals are presented monaurally there is no perceived change and no potentials are evoked: hence the response must be purely a function of the binaural interaction.P70, N130 and P220 components were consistently recorded to both coherence and discoherence. N130 was usually largest at Fz and P220 at Cz. No potentials of shorter latency were identified, even after averaging 5000 or more sweeps. When the noise became coherent with an inter-aural time difference (δT) of ±0.5 msec (giving rise to an off-centre sound image), the responses were of slightly longer latency and showed no significant asymmetries between C3 and C4. In binaurally coherent noise, δT changes of ±0.5 or ±1.0 msec evoked similar responses which showed no significant asymmetries on the scalp. N130 was of longer latency when δT was changed from ±0.5 msec to zero, as compared with the converse change.In view of the similarity of all these responses it is considered unlikely that they were due to specific populations of binaurally responsive cortical neurones. The N130 and P220 components are thought to be non-specific potentials which are elicited by amy perceptible change in steady auditory stimulus conditions, due to a “mismatch” between the stimulus and the contents of a short-term auditory memory.  相似文献   

16.
Habituation is a generic property of the neural response to repeated stimuli. Its strength often increases as inter-stimuli relaxation periods decrease. We propose a simple, broadly applicable control structure that enables a neural mass model of the evoked EEG response to exhibit habituated behavior. A key motivation for this investigation is the ongoing effort to develop model-based reconstruction of multi-modal functional neuroimaging data. The control structure proposed here is illustrated and validated in the context of a biophysical neural mass model, developed by Riera et?al. (Hum Brain Mapp 27(11):896-914, 2006; 28(4):335-354, 2007), and of simplifications thereof, using data from rat EEG response to medial nerve stimuli presented at frequencies from 1 to 8?Hz. Performance was tested by predictions of both the response to the next stimulus based on the current one, and also of continued stimuli trains over 4-s time intervals based on the first stimulus in the interval, with similar success statistics. These tests demonstrate the ability of simple generative models to capture key features of the evoked response, including habituation.  相似文献   

17.
Small trabeculae of rabbit left atrium immersed in TKBa solution (Tyrode with 10 mM K+ and 1 mM Ba2+) were used to study frequency dependence of "membrane" slow response excitability at long cycle lengths (greater than 1 s). In TKBa, stimuli generate graded, low- amplitude (2-15 mV) subliminal responses of variable long duration (up to 450 ms). A full all-or-none slow response is generated when a subliminal response depolarizes the membrane to about--35 mV. Subliminal response amplitude and rate of rise augment with stimulus intensity-duration product. For a fixed stimulus, the subliminal response is larger and faster at higher frequencies. Sudden changes in stimulus frequency or time course induce changes in subliminal response tha take four to eight cycles to attain steady state. For a fixed stimulus, slow response latency shortens progressively during the first few cycles after a sudden increase in frequency or when a rested preparation is excited (latency adaptation phenomenon, LAP). Slow response threshold stimulus requirements decrease during LAP (excitability hysteresis). The degree of excitability hysteresis is dependent on stimulation frequency and is more pronounced at higher frequencies. Frequency sensitivity of subliminal response (which causes frequency sensitivity of slow response excitability) is explained in terms of a transient state of enhancement set up by each stimulus. The enhanced state decays between stimuli with a half-time of approximately 4 s, thus allowing cumulative effects to become evident at rates above 0.1 Hz.  相似文献   

18.
Evoked potentials from the major longitudinal nerve cords of Notoplana acticola are potentiated if the preparation is decerebrated or if certain nerves in the ventral submuscular plexus are severed. Concomitant with an increase in amplitude of the response (in some preparations over fourfold) is a decrease in latency of the response and a decrease in the threshold stimulus intensity needed to evoke activity. Evoked activity can also be depressed with moderate increases in stimulus intensity and is induced some distance from the recording site. The depressant effects can be lifted if the brain is bisected, if the contralateral nerve VI is severed close to the brain, and if the commissures between the two nerves VI are cut. The depressant effects of nerves V and VI are additive. Depression and the subsequent lifting of the inhibition occur in Ringer baths containing either normal sea water or equal mixtures of sea water and isotonic MgCl2. The possibility of inhibitory synapses immune to high concentrations of magnesium ions is discussed.  相似文献   

19.
Neurons are commonly characterized by spontaneous generation of action potentials (spikes), which appear without any apparent or controlled stimulation. When a stimulus is applied, the spontaneous firing may prevail and hamper identification of the effect of the stimulus. Therefore, for any rigorous analysis of evoked neuronal activity, the presence of spontaneous firing has to be taken into account. If the background signal is ignored, however small it is compared to the response activity, and however large is the delay, estimation of the response latency will be wrong, and the error will persist even when sample size is increasing. The first question is: what is the response latency to the stimulus? Answering this question becomes even more difficult if the latency is of a complex nature, for example composed of a physically implied deterministic part and a stochastic part. This scenario is considered here, where the response time is a sum of two components; the delay and the relative latency. Parametric estimators for the time delay and the response latency are derived. These estimators are evaluated on simulated data and their properties are discussed. Finally, we show that the mean of the response latency is always satisfactorily estimated, even assuming a wrong distribution for the response latency.  相似文献   

20.
During the foreperiod of a forewarned reaction time (RT) task reflexes in the executing limb increase to a lesser extent than those in the contralateral limb. This is possibly due to input modulation. The present study investigates the possibility of cutaneous sensory modulation during motor preparation by studying the amplitudes of somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs). Eighteen subjects performed a forewarned RT task with the same fingers as the ones which were electrically stimulated. SEPs evoked during the 4 sec preparatory period were compared to those evoked during movement execution and during the resting period after the motor response respectively. During response execution most SEP components showed smaller amplitudes, i.e., they were gated, which agrees with other studies. In the first part of the foreperiod no SEP modulation was observed. Towards the end of the foreperiod, 500 msec before the response stimulus (RS), the amplitude of the contralateral parietal N70-P100 was significantly decreased, while the P45-N70 showed a similar tendency. However, at the same time the P100-N140 was increased in amplitude. The decrease of the intermediate latency components towards the end of the foreperiod is discussed in terms of gating, while the increase in the long latency component is discussed with respect to a decrease in RT on trials where the fingers were stimulated just before the RS, pointing to the role of attentional mechanisms.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号