首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The sequential deployment of gaze to regions of interest is an integral part of human visual function. Owing to its central importance, decades of research have focused on predicting gaze locations, but there has been relatively little formal attempt to predict the temporal aspects of gaze deployment in natural multi-tasking situations. We approach this problem by decomposing complex visual behaviour into individual task modules that require independent sources of visual information for control, in order to model human gaze deployment on different task-relevant objects. We introduce a softmax barrier model for gaze selection that uses two key elements: a priority parameter that represents task importance per module, and noise estimates that allow modules to represent uncertainty about the state of task-relevant visual information. Comparisons with human gaze data gathered in a virtual driving environment show that the model closely approximates human performance.  相似文献   

2.
A F Khizhun 《Biofizika》1989,34(5):909-910
The problem is discussed concerning possible loss of dynamic stability of the source of man's alpha-rhythm, as well as one of psychoneurological aspects of this phenomenon. With the aim of modeling man's EEG on animals an experimental criterion is suggested which makes it possible to state that definite spectral peak of animal EKoG cannot serve as a model for studying the properties of man's alpha-rhythm.  相似文献   

3.
Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum   总被引:25,自引:0,他引:25  
McClure SM  Berns GS  Montague PR 《Neuron》2003,38(2):339-346
Functional MRI experiments in human subjects strongly suggest that the striatum participates in processing information about the predictability of rewarding stimuli. However, stimuli can be unpredictable in character (what stimulus arrives next), unpredictable in time (when the stimulus arrives), and unpredictable in amount (how much arrives). These variables have not been dissociated in previous imaging work in humans, thus conflating possible interpretations of the kinds of expectation errors driving the measured brain responses. Using a passive conditioning task and fMRI in human subjects, we show that positive and negative prediction errors in reward delivery time correlate with BOLD changes in human striatum, with the strongest activation lateralized to the left putamen. For the negative prediction error, the brain response was elicited by expectations only and not by stimuli presented directly; that is, we measured the brain response to nothing delivered (juice expected but not delivered) contrasted with nothing delivered (nothing expected).  相似文献   

4.
5.
 The effect of spatial frequency discrimination learning on spatial frequency detection tuning curves, obtained by a summation to threshold paradigm, has been investigated. Three human observers were exposed to a grating discrimination task for longer than two weeks, and their detection thresholds for compound Gabor gratings were measured before and after this time interval. Discrimination thresholds decreased continuously and substantially during the course of learning, while the spatial frequency detection tuning curves show significant broadening in the posttest. Calculating the discrimination resolution of an ensemble of sensory coding units shows that larger bandwidths lead to better spatial frequency discrimination performance if pattern discrimination rests on multidimensional comparison or one-dimensional scaling of the spatial frequency parameter. Further, it is shown that a multiple-mechanism nonlinear pooling model is capable of explaining the results if plasticity of coding unit bandwidth or adaptive weights of the coding unit responses at the stage of response integration is assumed. The alternative sources of plasticity and the consequences of the findings for psychophysical modeling are discussed. Received: 8 September 1999 / Accepted in revised form: 16 October 2000  相似文献   

6.
Tests of self-awareness in nonhuman primates have to date beenconcerned almost entirely with the recognition of an animal'sreflection in a mirror. By contrast, we know much less aboutnon-human primates' perception of their place within a socialnetwork, or of their understanding of themselves as individualswith unique sets of social relationships. Here we review evidencethat monkeys who fail the mirror test may nonetheless behaveas if they recognize themselves as distinct individuals, eachof whom occupies a unique place in society and has a specificset of relations with others. A free-ranging vervet monkey,baboon, or macaque recognizes other members of his group asindividuals. He also recognizes matrilineal kin groups, lineardominance rank orders, and behaves as if he recognizes his ownunique place within them. This sense of "social self" in monkeys,however, is markedly different from self-awareness in humans.Although monkeys may behave in ways that accurately place themselveswithin a social network, they are unaware of the knowledge thatallows them to do so: they do not know what they know, cannotreflect on what they know, and cannot become the object of theirown attention.  相似文献   

7.
Stochastic models are proposed for sleep and for the sleep related electroencephalogram (EEG), electrooculogram (EOG), and electromyogram (EMG). The evolution of sleep through its various stages is described as a Markov chain. The EEG is modelled using Wiener processes. The EOG and EMG are modelled as combinations of Poisson point processes and Gaussian processes, respectively. The EEG models contain a feedback structure that is based on physiological data. The maximum likelihood sleep stage monitor, that uses the sleep-related observations, has been derived and implemented. The agreement between automatic and human stage classifications of six sleep recordings was 70.6%, which was 4.5% worse than the average agreement between six human classifiers. Monitoring of simulated sleep suggests that the difficulty in separating wakefulness from stage 1 is due to poor modelling. If one ignores this difference, which, from a diagnostic point of view is fairly unimportant, the above mentioned agreement reaches 81.8%, which is 0.5% better than the corresponding average human vs human agreement.  相似文献   

8.
Cell kinetics to monitor radioresponsivity in human epidermoid carcinoma   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The clinical relevance of proliferative activity as an indicator of radioresponsivity was investigated in advanced epidermoid carcinomas of the oral cavity. Proliferative activity, determined in vitro as 3H-thymidine labeling index, was assessed before starting radiotherapy and after 10 Gy. In a series of 35 patients, pretreatment proliferative activity was not indicative of response to radiotherapy. Conversely, in the same series of patients, an association was observed between an early variation of proliferative activity induced by 10 Gy and the response to the full course of radiotherapy. The effect on proliferative activity was not related to tumor volume reduction but to long-term clinical response. An inhibition of more or less than 70% was significantly correlated with long-term clinical outcome at 36 months in terms of the probability of local recurrence (29% vs 90%; p = 0.002) and overall survival (55% vs 16%; p = 0.006) at 36 months.  相似文献   

9.
目的建立食蟹猴的肾移植模型。方法通过显微外科手术对食蟹猴进行同种异体移植,将供体肾动脉、肾静脉分别端侧吻合到受体腹主动脉、下腔静脉,供体输尿管端端吻合到受体输尿管。结果154例肾移植食蟹猴无一只在手术过程中因意外死亡;无一只因手术感染死亡;无一只因手术并发症死亡;肾热缺血时间(30±4.5)min。结论应用食蟹猴建立肾移植模型,技术成熟,方法可靠。应用此模型评价新型免疫抑制药物,进行免疫耐受、异种移植研究可为临床试验提供更为准确可靠的依据。  相似文献   

10.
Skorupa  Joseph P. 《Oecologia》1983,57(3):391-396
Oecologia - A recently proposed model of competition was claimed to have predicted accurately the independently estimated densities of four sympatric primates in Kibale Forest, Uganda. The validity...  相似文献   

11.
12.
13.
14.
In group‐living animals, social relationships are the result of the interactions between two individuals over time and can provide fitness benefits to both the participants. Recently, components of social relationships were identified in Old World primates and ravens through the use of principal component analysis (PCA). We employed PCA to identify components that define the social relationships in two communities of wild spider monkeys in the Yucatán Peninsula, Mexico and investigated whether characteristics of the dyads, such as age combination, sex combination and kinship, had an effect on the components obtained. We found two components. Component 1 had high positive loadings of proximity, grooming and subgroup index, which may reflect value or compatibility. Embrace and aggression rates loaded highly on Component 2, which was labelled risk. Component 1 was affected by kinship: kin had higher scores than non‐kin. Both the components were affected by sex combination of the dyad. Male–male dyads had higher scores than female–male and female–female dyads. The results are in accordance with what is currently known about spider monkeys’ social relationships, but provide an additional perspective with the novel component of risk. In this respect, the two components are effective in capturing the ambiguous nature of male–male relationships in spider monkeys. Overall, this study offers further insight into the social patterns underlying the relationship quality in group‐living animals and the usefulness of PCA in providing an objective assessment that reflects the animals′ perspective of their social interactions.  相似文献   

15.
Muscles expend energy to perform active work during locomotion, but they may also expend significant energy to produce force, for example when tendons perform much of the work passively. The relative contributions of work and force to overall energy expenditure are unknown. We therefore measured the mechanics and energetics of a cyclical bouncing task, designed to control for work and force. We hypothesized that near bouncing resonance, little work would be performed actively by muscle, but the cyclical production of force would cost substantial metabolic energy. Human subjects (n = 9) bounced vertically about the ankles at inversely proportional frequencies (1-4 Hz) and amplitudes (15-4 mm), such that the overall rate of work performed on the body remained approximately constant (0.30 ± 0.06 W/kg), but the forces varied considerably. We used parameter identification to estimate series elasticity of the triceps surae tendon, as well as the work performed actively by muscle and passively by tendon. Net metabolic energy expenditure for bouncing at 1 Hz was 1.15 ± 0.31 W/kg, attributable mainly to active muscle work with an efficiency of 24 ± 3%. But at 3 Hz (near resonance), most of the work was performed passively, so that active muscle work could account for only 40% of the net metabolic rate of 0.76 ± 0.28 W/kg. Near resonance, a cost for cyclical force that increased with both amplitude and frequency of force accounted for at least as much of the total energy expenditure as a cost for work. Series elasticity reduces the need for active work, but energy must still be expended for force production.  相似文献   

16.
17.
18.
Images of two fragments of regular geometrical figures (square, triangle etc.) have been presented to 58 healthy persons successively with intervals of 20, 80, 120 and 380 ms. The subject must compare these fragments mentally, decide whether they form the standard figure and press a button by the right or left hand according to the instruction. At presentation of both fragments in one visual field, left or right, the number of correct responses is greater when they form the figure. The greater the interstimulus intervals, the greater the number of correct responses to stimuli forming and not forming the standard figure. At presentation of fragments in different visual fields, the number of correct decisions is the same, independently from forming the standard figure. The reaction time is shorter when exposing fragments forming the figure, independently from the way of their presentation; with prolongation of interstimulus intervals the reaction time decreases in all cases. The number of correct decisions is greater and the reaction time is shorter when the stimuli are presented in different visual fields.  相似文献   

19.
An experiment is described which compares the performance of a neural network to human performance on a visual task which consists of detecting a target in a background image of correlated noise. A three-layer, feed-forward, multi-layer perceptron is trained to indicate the presence or absence of a target in images also presented to human observers. The basis for the comparison between the network and the human observers is the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. Network performance is comparable to human performance for this particular task.  相似文献   

20.
Liu Y  Shen H  Zhou Z  Hu D 《PloS one》2011,6(8):e23839
In this work, we investigated the sustained negative blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response (sNBR) using functional magnetic resonance imaging during a finger tapping task. We observed that the sNBR for this task was more extensive than has previously been reported. The cortical regions involved in sNBR are divided into the following three groups: frontal, somatosensory and occipital. By investigating the spatial structure, area, amplitude, and dynamics of the sNBR in comparison with those of its positive BOLD response (PBR) counterpart, we made the following observations. First, among the three groups, the somatosensory group contained the greatest number of activated voxels and the fewest deactivated voxels. In addition, the amplitude of the sNBR in this group was the smallest among the three groups. Second, the onset and peak time of the sNBR are both larger than those of the PBR, whereas the falling edge time of the sNBR is less than that of the PBR. Third, the long distance between most sNBR foci and their corresponding PBR foci makes it unlikely that they share the same blood supply artery. Fourth, the couplings between the sNBR and its PBR counterpart are distinct among different regions and thus should be investigated separately. These findings imply that the origin of most sNBR foci in the finger-tapping task is much more likely to be neuronal activity suppression rather than "blood steal."  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号