首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
    
Abstract  1. Large male seaweed flies (Diptera: Coelopidae) are more likely to mate than smaller males. This is due to sexual conflict over mating, by which females physically resist male attempts to copulate. In some species, large males are simply more efficient at overpowering female resistance.
2. Female reluctance to mate is likely to have evolved due to the costs of mating to females. In many dipterans, males manipulate female behaviour through seminal proteins that have evolved through sperm competition. This behavioural manipulation can be costly to females, for example forcing females to oviposit in sub-optimal conditions and increasing their mortality.
3. Previous work has failed to identify any ubiquitous costs of mating to female coelopids. The work reported here was designed to investigate the effects of exposure to oviposition sites ( Fucus algae) on the reproductive behaviour of four species of coelopid. Algae deposition in nature is stochastic and females mate with multiple males in and around oviposition sites. Spermatogenesis is restricted to the pupal stage and there is last-male sperm precedence. It was predicted that males would avoid wasting sperm and would be more willing to mate, and to remain paired with females for longer, when exposed to oviposition material compared with control males. Females were predicted to incur longevity costs of mating if mating increased their rate of oviposition, especially in the presence of algae.
4. The behaviour of males of all four species concurred with the predictions; however mating did not affect female receptivity, oviposition behaviour, or longevity. Exposure to algae induced oviposition and increased female mortality in all species independently of mating and egg production. The evolutionary ecology of potential costs of mating to female coelopids are discussed in the light of these findings.  相似文献   

2.
Mate sampling and the sexual conflict over mating in seaweed flies   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:2  
The order in which females encounter, or sample, males in apopulation may have important consequences for mate choice,with the information gathered about males influencing boththe preference function and degree of choosiness of females.Sexual selection may be affected as a result. Sampling of particularsubsets of males may be a crucial component of individual variation in mate preferences within populations. However, the sequencein which males are sampled may also be important in specieswithout traditional, active mate choice, such as when sexualselection involves sexual conflict over mating. This wouldoccur if the likelihood of a female mating with a male of acertain phenotype changes as a result of previous encounters.We examined the effects of encountering males differing inbody size, a sexually selected phenotype, in the seaweed flyCoelopa frigida. Sexual selection occurs in this species asa result of a sexual conflict over mating. We show that theoutcome of the sexual conflict is independent of the orderin which males are encountered by female seaweed flies, withthe overall mating advantage to large males being unaffected.In addition, we explored female preference functions and evaluatethe heterogeneity in female willingness to mate. We suggestthat consideration of mate sampling theory is valuable whenexamining mate choice in species in which sexual selectionis driven by sexual conflict.  相似文献   

3.
    
Various characteristics of a long‐distance acoustic signal have been shown to vary to different degrees. It has been suggested that female preferences based on stable song parameters are stabilising or weakly directional, and preferences based on variable parameters are strongly directional. We tested this hypothesis based on a short‐distance signal (courtship song) produced by the field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. We studied the degree of variability of different courtship song parameters and the behavioural importance of several parameters using synthesised song models in playback experiments. We found that most of the courtship song elements of G. bimaculatus were quite variable (coefficient of variation, CV, in the range of 20–53%). The most variable parameter of the courtship song was the relative amplitude of two elements: high‐amplitude ticks and low‐amplitude pulses. Because songs containing only ticks (of rare occurrence) appeared to be more effective than songs with both ticks and pulses (of frequent occurrence), we consider female preferences to be directional. Alteration of less variable traits, such as the carrier frequency and duration of ticks (CV = 20–25%), had a different effect on female responsiveness. The synthesised songs with different carrier frequencies of ticks were as attractive to females as the positive control (courtship of muted males accompanied by playback of the recorded song). Altering the duration of ticks had a crucial effect on the female response rate, decreasing female responsiveness to the level observed in the negative control (courtship of muted males). Thus, we did not find a strong relationship between the variability of individual song parameters and their potential importance in song recognition and the evaluation of male quality. The partial inconsistency of our results with the data of other authors may be due to different patterns of past and current selection on long‐distance and short‐distance acoustic signals.  相似文献   

4.
5.
The evolutionary outcome of interspecific hybridization, i.e. collapse of species into a hybrid swarm, persistence or even divergence with reinforcement, depends on the balance between gene flow and selection against hybrids. If female mating preferences are open-ended but sign-inversed between species, they can theoretically be a source of such selection. Cichlid fish in African lakes have sustained high rates of speciation despite evidence for widespread hybridization, and sexual selection by female choice has been proposed as important in the origin and maintenance of species boundaries. However, it had never been tested whether hybridizing species have open-ended preference rules. Here we report the first experimental test using Pundamilia pundamilia, Pundamilia nyererei and their hybrids in three-way choice experiments. Hybrid males are phenotypically intermediate. Wild-caught females of both species have strong preferences for conspecific over heterospecific males. Their responses to F1 hybrid males are intermediate, but more similar to responses to conspecifics in one species and more similar to responses to heterospecifics in the other. We suggest that their mate choice mechanism may predispose haplochromine cichlids to maintain and perhaps undergo phenotypic diversification despite hybridization, and that species differences in female preference functions may predict the potential for adaptive trait transfer between hybridizing species.  相似文献   

6.
    
In spite of abundant evidence that intra- and inter-specific competition occurs in natural communities, there is surprisingly little to suggest it is a major force promoting genetic change. This report assesses the genetic effects of competition in two species of seaweed fly, Coelopa frigida and C. pilipes. In laboratory cultures of C. frigida the relative survival of heterozygotes at the Adh locus, which was being used as a marker for the large αβ chromosomal inversion, was greater than that of homozygotes. In monocultures of C. frigida this competitive superiority was dependent on larval density. At low densities facilitation was seen, whereas at high larval densities there was competition. In mixed cultures of the two species, interspecific competition contributed to the differential mortality of C. frigida , and observations of natural populations suggested that competition may have similar effects to those described in laboratory culture. A possible mechanism involving the supply of nutritive microorganisms is proposed to underly both intra- and inter-specific competition. In seaweed flies, competition and the consequent differential mortality appear to be forces maintaining rather than reducing genetic variation.  相似文献   

7.
8.
    
Mating rate optima often differ between the sexes: males may increase their fitness by multiple mating, but for females multiple mating confers little benefit and can often be costly (especially in taxa without nuptial gifts or mala parental care). Sexually antagonistic evolution is thus expected in traits related to mating rates under sexual selection. This prediction has been tested by multiple studies that applied experimental evolution technique, which is a powerful tool to directly examine the evolutionary consequences of selection. Yet, the results so far only partly support the prediction. Here, we provide another example of experimental evolution of sexual selection, by applying it for the first time to the mating behaviour of a seed beetle Callsorobruchus chinensis. We found a lower remating rate in polygamy-line females than in monogamy-line (i.e. no sexual selection) females after 21 generations of selection. Polygamy-line females also showed a longer duration of first mating than monogamy-line females. We found no effect of male evolutionary lines on the remating rate or first mating duration. Though not consistent with the original prediction, the current and previous studies collectively suggest that the observed female-limited responses may be a norm, which is also consistent with the conceptual advances in the last two decades of the advantages and limitations of experimental evolution technique.  相似文献   

9.
10.
In this paper, Coelopa frigida (Fabricius, 1805) of the family Coelopidae is reported for the first time in Korea. The domestic localities and some illustrations are given.  相似文献   

11.
12.
Multiple mating by females is widely thought to encourage post-mating sexual selection and enhance female fitness. We show that whether polyandrous mating has these effects depends on two conditions. Condition 1 is the pattern of sperm utilization by females; specifically, whether, among females, male mating number, m (i.e. the number of times a male mates with one or more females) covaries with male offspring number, o. Polyandrous mating enhances sexual selection only when males who are successful at multiple mating also sire most or all of each of their mates'' offspring, i.e. only when Cov(m,o), is positive. Condition 2 is the pattern of female reproductive life-history; specifically, whether female mating number, m, covaries with female offspring number, o. Only semelparity does not erode sexual selection, whereas iteroparity (i.e. when Cov(m,o), is positive) always increases the variance in offspring numbers among females, which always decreases the intensity of sexual selection on males. To document the covariance between mating number and offspring number for each sex, it is necessary to assign progeny to all parents, as well as identify mating and non-mating individuals. To document significant fitness gains by females through iteroparity, it is necessary to determine the relative magnitudes of male as well as female contributions to the total variance in relative fitness. We show how such data can be collected, how often they are collected, and we explain the circumstances in which selection favouring multiple mating by females can be strong or weak.  相似文献   

13.
We review the evidence for genetic variation in female and male mate preferences. Genetic differences between species and partially isolated races show that preferences can evolve and were genetically variable in the past. Within populations there is good evidence of genetic variation, both of discrete genetic effects (8 cases) and quantitative genetic effects (17 cases), from a diverse range of taxa. We also review evidence for the presence of genetic covariance between mate preferences and sexual traits in the other sex. The 11 studies go a long way to validating the theoretical prediction of positive genetic covariance. The few negative results are best explained by a lack of appropriate experimental design. One unresolved question is whether genetic covariance is due to linkage disequilibrium between unlinked genes or physical linkage. Some evidence points to linkage disequilibrium but this is not yet conclusive.  相似文献   

14.
    
We investigated the role of maintenance and origin of female mating preferences in three species of fish. Poecilia latipinna and P. mexicana are sexual species that hybridized to form the gynogenetic clonal P. formosa, which reproduces asexually but requires sperm to initiate embryogenesis. We demonstrate that all three species display almost identical and statistically indistinguishable preferences for large males. Although processes of good genes, runaway sexual selection, and direct selection could maintain preferences in the sexual species, good genes and runaway sexual selection are unlikely to operate in the asexual species. Furthermore, we found that the most likely direct selection benefit, an increase in fecundity, can also be excluded in the gynogens. We conclude that the most parsimonious explanation for this P. formosa preference is that it was inherited from the parental species and is maintained without forces generated by good genes, runaway selection, or direct selection for increased fecundity. This preference may be maintained because of pleiotropic effects (e.g., sensory bias) or mate searching costs.  相似文献   

15.
Examining the distribution of female preference and attractive male secondary sexual characters in closely related species can help evaluate the various models for the evolution of female preference. We examined whether orange coloration in males was attractive to females of Poecilia picta, a species in the same subgenus as the guppy, P. reticulata. Females did not respond differently to males with or without a conspicuous orange stripe on the dorsum of the tail, a characteristic ornamentation of the male of this species. This lack of preference in a taxon that possesses a conspicuous, secondary sexual character directly contradicts the predictions of the sensory exploitation hypothesis for the evolution of female preference, but may support a model of antagonistic sexual selection proposed by Rice (1996).  相似文献   

16.
    
The time a female spends in front of a male is a commonly assumed but rarely tested measure of female mate preference in mate choice experiments. I investigated whether the mate preference in zebra finch females Taeniopygia guttata castanotis measured as the number of solicitations directed towards a male was related to the time females spent with that male. In a binary choice situation within an aviary, females were allowed to fly freely and visit two individually caged males over a whole day. I measured the females' solicitation displays as the number of tail quivering and/or presentations of nesting material. I also measured the time the females spent in front of each male. Females that solicited did so exclusively to one of the two males and when they did they spent almost twice as much time with that male than with the other. The time these females spent in front of a male correlated positively with the number of solicitation displays directed to that male. The study shows the first experimental link between mate preference measured by courtship displays and preference measured as the time a female spends with a male. Hence, time spent with a male, the most common measure of female mate preference in zebra finches choice experiments is a good indicator of female mate preference.  相似文献   

17.
18.
Finding, assessing, rejecting, and copulating with a mate isassumed to carry fitness costs, particularly for females, thathave to be traded off against fitness benefits of mating suchas increased fecundity, fertility, longevity, or better qualityoffspring. Female dung flies, Sepsis cynipsea (Diptera: Sepsidae),typically attempt to dislodge mounted males harassing themby vigorous shaking. Shaking duration has been shown to reflect both direct and indirect female choice in this species. Thelatter is an expression of the females' general reluctanceto mate due to presumed costs of mating. We investigated thecosts of copulation in the laboratory. Females were randomlyassigned to one of three treatment groups and allowed to copulateeither not at all, once, or twice. The males' armored genitalia injured females internally during copula. Injuries were visibleas sclerotized scars in the female ovipositor, and their occurrenceincreased with mating frequency. Presumably due to these injuries,mated females showed higher mortality. This effect was statisticallyindependent from additional costs of reproduction related tooviposition, as copulation also increased lifetime egg productionand tended to augment oviposition rate (eggs per day), while fertility (proportion of offspring emerged) was unaffected.We thus found high mortality costs of copulating, indicatingsubstantial sexual conflict, which helps explain female reluctanceto mate in this species.  相似文献   

19.
    
The expected strong directional selection for traits that increase a male's mating ability conflicts with the frequent observation that within species, males may show extreme variation in sexual traits. These male reproductive polymorphisms are usually attributed to direct male–male competition. It is currently unclear, however, how directional selection for sexually selected traits may convert into disruptive selection, and if female preference for elaborate traits may be an alternative mechanism driving the evolution of male polymorphism. Here, we explore this mechanism using the polyandric dwarf spider Oedothorax gibbosus as a model. We first show that males characterized by conspicuous cephalic structures serving as a nuptial feeding device (“gibbosus males”) significantly outperform other males in siring offspring of previously fertilized females. However, significant costs in terms of development time of gibbosus males open a mating niche for an alternative male type lacking expensive secondary sexual traits. These “tuberosus males” obtain virtually all fertilizations early in the breeding season. Individual‐based simulations demonstrate a hitherto unknown general principle, by which males selected for high investment to attract females suffer constrained mating opportunities. This creates a vacant mating niche of unmated females for noninvesting males and, consequently, disruptive selection on male secondary sexual traits.  相似文献   

20.
    
In promiscuous species with sperm storage, males are expected to show a preference for mating with virgin and young females to reduce the risk of sperm competition. In the simultaneous hermaphrodite land snail Arianta arbustorum, sperm production precedes egg production by 2–4 weeks, resulting in a short period of protandric hermaphroditism before shell growth is completed. In natural populations, copulating pairs involving individuals which have not yet completed shell growth (virgins) have been observed. We ran a series of mate-choice experiments to examine whether virgin and nonvirgin (experienced) individuals of A. arbustorum discriminate between virgin and nonvirgin mating partners. We also assessed the number of sperm delivered to partners with different mating status. Neither virgin nor nonvirgin snails showed any preference for mating with a virgin partner. In all test situations mating was random and the number of sperm delivered was not adjusted to the mating status of the partner. Mating success was mainly determined by the activity of the individual. The random mating pattern does not imply random fertilization of eggs because the presence of a sperm-digesting organ and the morphology of the sperm storage organ allow a selective storage and use of sperm in A. arbustorum.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号