首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The localization of a sum of acoustic signals by two northern fur seals in air depending on sound parameters was investigated using the method of instrumental conditioned reflexes with food reinforcement. It was found that sound perception of northern fur seal proceeds by the binaural mechanism. The time/intensity interchange coefficient was 570 microseconds/dB for series of clicks (with amplitude maximum at 1 kHz) and 250 microseconds/dB for tonal impulses with a frequency of 1 kHz. With click amplitudes being equal, the number of approaches of the animal to the source of the first signal reached a 75% level at a delay of the second signal 0.07 ms (the minimum delay); with a delay of 6 ms (the maximum delay) and more, the fur seal, probably hears two separate signals. The minimum delay depended little on the duration of tonal impulses (with a frequency of 1 kHz) and was 0.3-0.7 ms; the maximum delay was 9-11 ms for tonal impulses with a duration of 3 ms and 37-40 ms with impulse duration 20 ms. The precedence effect became apparent at a greater delay for smooth fronts of impulses than for rectangular fronts.  相似文献   

2.
Females of the parasitoid fly Emblemasoma auditrix find their host cicada (Okanagana rimosa) by its acoustic signals. In laboratory experiments, fly phonotaxis had a mean threshold of about 66 dB SPL when tested with the cicada calling song. Flies exhibited a frequency dependent phonotaxis when testing to song models with different carrier frequencies (pulses of 6 ms duration and a repetition rate of 80 pulses s(-1)). However, the phonotactic threshold was rather broadly tuned in the range from 5 kHz to 11 kHz. Phonotaxis was also dependent on the temporal parameters of the song models: repetition rates of 60 pulses s(-1) and 80 pulses s and pulse durations of 5-7 ms resulted in the highest percentages of phonotaxis performing animals coupled with the lowest threshold values. Thus, parasitoid phonotaxis is adapted especially to the temporal parameters of the calling song of the host. Choice experiments revealed a preference of a song model with 9 kHz carrier frequency (peak energy of the host song) compared with 5 kHz carrier frequency (electrophysiologically determined best hearing frequency). However, this preference changed with the relative sound pressure level of both signals. When presented simultaneously, E. auditrix preferred 5-kHz signals, if they were 5 dB SPL louder than the 9-kHz signal.  相似文献   

3.
Signal oversampling is frequently used to prevent distortion in time-series representations. When sampling at rates just above the Nyquist critical frequency (f(c)), Shannon's reconstruction theorem provides an alternative means of circumventing this problem. The purpose of this study was to determine whether surface electromyographic (EMG) data is compromised when sampled just above f(c) and whether Shannon's reconstruction theorem can correct these deficiencies, if present. Brief isometric elbow flexion contractions were performed at 100%, 80%, 50%, 25%, 10%, 5% and 2.5% maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) followed by one trial of random cyclic isometric contractions and relaxations. The 2kHz signal was resampled at 1kHz and the 2 and 1kHz signals were reconstructed (2RS and 1RS, respectively) to a sampling rate of 20kHz. Data were full-wave rectified (FWR) and low-pass filtered (LE). Peak amplitudes of the FWR and LE signals, average EMG (AEMG) amplitudes of the FWR and LE signals, mean power frequency of the raw data, number of gaps, and mean gap time of the LE signals were calculated. Significant differences were present in peak EMG and AEMG measurements between the 1 and 2kHz, 2RS and 20kHz signals and occasionally between the 1RS and the 2kHz, 2RS and 20kHz signals. These differences, although statistically significant were quite small amounting to less than 0.5% MVC. No significant differences were found for the gaps parameters. The small differences seen, coupled with the processing time required for signal reconstruction, make oversampling as well as signal reconstruction in surface EMG measurements unnecessary.  相似文献   

4.
黑蝉(C.atrata Fabricius.)鸣声的方向性和第三气门的功能   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
黑蝉鸣声的波形结构无明显的方向性.单音节的重复周期和调幅脉冲列的间隔(I_1和I_2)分别为9.787±0.813ms、2.286±0.093ms和1.874±0.063ms.幅值特性有明显的方向性.主峰频率(MPF=5.47±0.11kHz)的幅值,头向和背向分别比尾向下降5.9dB和3.9dB,侧向和腹向分别增高1.1dB和2.3dB.两侧第三气门受阻后鸣声的波形结构和音色都产生明显变化.I_1和I_2分别为0.912±0.156ms和1.099±0.113ms,约为正常值的40—59%.有三个谱带,MPF为5775Hz,两侧谱带的峰值频率为4575Hz和7025Hz,分别下降1.5dB和3.4dB.  相似文献   

5.
The temporal integration of the A1 auditory receptor of two species of noctuid moths (Lepidoptera, Noctuidae) was investigated. Tympanal nerve spikes were recorded while stimulating the ear with broad band clicks. Thresholds were measured for single clicks, pairs of clicks with a separation of 1–20 ms, and trains of up to 8 clicks at separations of 1–2 ms. The average threshold for single clicks was 52.9 dB peSPL (SD 1.7 dB, n = 40) for Noctua pronuba and 50.1 dB peSPL (SD 4.0 dB, n = 27) for Spodoptera littoralis. The thresholds for double clicks with a 1 ms separation were lower than the thresholds for single clicks. The difference decreased as the separation between the clicks was increased. The results were fully consistent with an energy detector model (a leaky integrator with an exponential decay) with a time constant of about 4 ms.The results are compared to previously published results with pure tone intensity/duration trading. A common underlying mechanism is suggested, based on the passive electric properties of the receptor cell membrane.It is suggested, that the time constant revealed in the present study characterizes auditory receptors in general, and is related to the short time constants in vertebrate audition.Abbreviations peSPL peak equivalent sound pressure level - SD standard deviation - time constant  相似文献   

6.
False Vampires ( Megaderma lyra ) are gleaning bats which emit brief (1 ms) and faint echolocation signals consisting of four harmonics of a shallow frequency downward modulated fundamental (27–19 kHz). The complete signal spans a frequency range from 100 to 19 kHz. In sound recordings from three experimental animals we show that Megaderma lyra shifts the dominant frequency in the echolocation signals in relation to the type of prey offered and to flight style. During roaming flights the mean peak frequency was 63.2 ± 9 kHz (third harmonic). In prey catching flights, peak frequencies were shifted into the fourth harmonic. In flights towards a dish of crawling mealworms, mean peak frequency was raised to 91.2 ± 3.3 kHz. When the bats flew towards living mice the dominant frequency was further increased to 99.8 ± 5.2 kHz, and the second and third harmonic were at least 10 dB fainter or no longer recordable. The additional frequency shift when flying towards mice was not only a consequence of the dominance of the fourth harmonic but also of an additional rise of the fundamental harmonic by nearly 2 kHz. These prey-correlated frequency shifts in echolocation calls showed little variation between the three experimental animals and were reproducible over time. They occurred at or even before take-off of the bats. This is the first report of target-correlated transient adaptations in echolocation calls of any bat species.  相似文献   

7.
Dendritic shaft (Zd) and spine (Zsp) input impedances were computed numerically for sites on hippocampal neurons, using a segmental format of cable calculations. The Zsp values for a typical spine appended onto a dendritic shaft averaged less than 2% higher than the Zd values for the adjacent dendritic shaft. Spine synaptic inputs were simulated by a brief conductance transient, which possessed a time integral of 12 X 10(-10)S X ms. This input resulted in an average peak spine response of 20 mV for both dentate granule neurons and CA1 pyramidal cells. The average spine transient was attenuated less than 2% in conduction across the spine neck, considering peak voltage, waveform parameters, and charge transfer. The spine conductance transient resulted in an average somatic response of 100 microV in the dentate granule neurons, because of passive electrotonic propagation. The same input transient was also applied to proximal and distal sites on CA1 pyramidal cells. The predicted responses at the soma demonstrated a clear difference between the proximal and distal inputs, in terms of both peak voltage and waveform parameters. Thus, the main determinant of the passive propagation of transient electrical signals in these neurons appears to be dendritic branching rather than signal attenuation through the spine neck.  相似文献   

8.
1.  L3 is a prothoracic auditory interneuron which has an ascending axon projecting to the brain. It is rather broadly tuned and most sensitive to carrier frequencies around 16 kHz (mean threshold=60 dB) and at 4–5 kHz (mean threshold=70 dB, Fig. 1).
2.  During open field stimulation L3's excitatory response increases rather linearly as sound intensity is increased and is 10–15 dB more sensitive to ipsilateral stimulation (Fig. 2). With closed field stimulation L3 is 45 dB more sensitive to ipsilateral sound at 16 kHz, and at least 20 dB more sensitive at 5 kHz (Fig. 3). With closed field sound, contralateral stimulation at subthreshold intensities (5 and 16 kHz) usually results in hyperpolarization (Fig. 3).
3.  L3's excitatory response to 16 kHz on the ipsilateral side is suppressed by low frequencies on the same side and by low and high frequency sounds from the contralateral side (Fig. 4).
4.  In open and closed field conditions, the number of spikes/syllable decrements in response to successive syllables of each chirp (Fig. 5). This response is dependent on the syllable period (SP) of the song, with the greatest decrement occurring in response to SPs of 50–70 ms; longer and shorter SPs cause less decrement (Figs. 6–7). At both 5 kHz and 16 kHz the ability of L3 to encode syllables (standard SD = 23 ms) within a chirp is dependent on the SP. At short SPs L3 fires throughout the chirp, while at longer SPs (50–200 ms) L3 responds with a distinct burst of firing for each pulse. At SPs of 200 ms or more, no decrement occurs (Fig. 8).
  相似文献   

9.
ABSTRACT

We recorded echolocation and ultrasonic social signals of the bat Myotis septentrionalis. The bats foraged for insects resting on or fluttering about an outdoor screen to which they were attracted by a ‘backlight’. The bats used nearly linearly modulated echolocation signals of high frequency (117 to 49 kHz, see Tables) with a weak second harmonic. The orientational signals from patrolling bats were about 2.4 ms in duration and occurred at a repetition rate of about 18 Hz (see Figure 3). The signals used by bats as they approached the screen were of shorter duration (0.72 ms) and occurred at higher rates (33.8 Hz) (Table 2 and Figure 4). We registered one feeding ‘buzz’ (Figure 5). We recorded social signals when two bats patrolled the hunting area. The social signals were characterized by their longer durations (6 ms, see Table 1), lower frequencies (70 to 30 kHz), and curvilinear sweeps (Figures 7 and 8). We calculated the source levels of orientational and social signals using the differences in arrival times at three microphones in a linear array (Figures 1 and 2). The source levels were on average 102 dB peSPL at 10 cm (Table 1). We could not calculate source levels of the signals used by bats as they approached the screen at close range, but these signals were much weaker (about 65 dB peSPL at the microphone).  相似文献   

10.
The selectivity of female phonotactic responses to synthetic advertisement calls was tested in choice situations. Preferences based on differences in the linear rise-time of synthetic pulses depended on intensity and carrier frequency. When the carrier frequency was 1.1 kHz, simulating the low-frequency peak in the advertisement call, females preferred alternatives with slower rise-time pulses that differed by 5 ms at playback levels of 75 dB SPL and higher. A rise-time difference of 10 ms was discriminated at 65 dB SPL. When the carrier frequency was 2.2 kHz, simulating the high-frequency peak in the call, females discriminated a 5-ms difference in rise-time only at 85 dB SPL. Females showed no preference when the difference was 10 ms at lower playback levels. The difference in the thresholds (about 15–20 dB) for discriminating differences in rise-time at the two carrier frequencies was greater than the difference in behavioral thresholds for these two frequencies (about 10 dB). This result suggests that rise-time discrimination can be mediated solely by the neural channel mainly tuned to the low-frequency peak in the call. Females probably assess differences in rise-time by comparing the first few pulses of each call rather than by averaging over the entire call. Accepted: 30 March 1999  相似文献   

11.
Current neck injury criteria do not include limits for lateral bending combined with axial compression and this has been observed as a clinically relevant mechanism, particularly for rollover motor vehicle crashes. The primary objectives of this study were to evaluate the effects of lateral eccentricity (the perpendicular distance from the axial force to the centre of the spine) on peak loads, kinematics, and spinal canal occlusions of subaxial cervical spine specimens tested in dynamic axial compression (0.5 m/s). Twelve 3-vertebra human cadaver cervical spine specimens were tested in two groups: low and high eccentricity with initial eccentricities of 1 and 150% of the lateral diameter of the vertebral body. Six-axis loads inferior to the specimen, kinematics of the superior-most vertebra, and spinal canal occlusions were measured. High speed video was collected and acoustic emission (AE) sensors were used to define the time of injury. The effects of eccentricity on peak loads, kinematics, and canal occlusions were evaluated using unpaired Student t-tests. The high eccentricity group had lower peak axial forces (1544±629 vs. 4296±1693 N), inferior displacements (0.2±1.0 vs. 6.6±2.0 mm), and canal occlusions (27±5 vs. 53±15%) and higher peak ipsilateral bending moments (53±17 vs. 3±18 Nm), ipsilateral bending rotations (22±3 vs. 1±2°), and ipsilateral displacements (4.5±1.4 vs. −1.0±1.3 mm, p<0.05 for all comparisons). These results provide new insights to develop prevention, recognition, and treatment strategies for compressive cervical spine injuries with lateral eccentricities.  相似文献   

12.
We studied the sounds of narwhals ( Monodon monoceros ) foraging in the open waters in Northwest Greenland. We used a linear, vertical array of three hydrophones (depth 10 m, 30 m, 100 m) with a fourth hydrophone (depth 30 m) about 20 m from the vertical array. A smaller fifth hydrophone (depth 2 m) allowed for registering frequencies up to 125 kHz (± 2 dB) when signals were recorded at 762 mm/set on an instrumentation tape recorder. Clicks were the prevalent signals, but we heard whistles occasionally. We separated the clicks into two classes: click trains that had rates of 3-10 clicks/sec and click bursts having rates of 110-150 clicks/sec. The spectra of train clicks had maximum amplitudes at 48 ± 10 kHz and a duration of 29 ± 6 psec. The spectra of burst clicks had maximum amplitudes at 19 ± 1 kHz and a duration of 40 ± 3 psec. By analogy with other dolphin species, narwhals presumably use the clicks for echolocation during orientation and for locating prey. The narwhal click patterns resemble those of insectivorous bats. Click trains might correspond to bat searching signals and click bursts to the bat's terminal "buzz", emitted just before prey capture.  相似文献   

13.
1. The maximum acoustic gain of the external ear in Macroderma gigas was found to be 25-30 dB between 5-8 kHz and in Nyctophilus gouldi it reached 15-23 dB between 7-22 kHz. Pinna gain reached a peak of 16 dB near 4.5-6 kHz in M. gigas and 12-17 dB between 7-12 kHz in N. gouldi, with average gain of 6-10 dB up to 100 kHz. Pinna gain curves resemble that of a finite conical horn, including resonance. 2. The directional properties of the external ear in both species result from sound diffraction at the pinna face, as it approximates a circular aperture. The frequency dependent movement of the acoustic axis in azimuth and elevation is attributed to the asymmetrical structure of the pinnae. 3. Evoked potentials and neuronal responses were studied in the inferior colliculus. In M. gigas, the neural audiogram has sensitivity peaks at 10-20 kHz and 35-43 kHz, with extremely low thresholds (-18 dB SPL) in the low frequency region. In N. gouldi, the neural audiogram has sensitivity peaks at 8-14 kHz (lowest threshold 5 dB SPL) and 22-45 kHz. Removal of the contralateral pinna causes a frequency dependent loss in neural threshold sensitivity of up to 10-15 dB in both species. 4. The high frequency peak in the audiogram coincides with the sonar energy band in both species, whereas the low frequency region is used for social communication. Highly sensitive low frequency hearing is discussed in relation to hunting in bats by passive listening.  相似文献   

14.
Three commercial ultrasonic devices (A, B, and C) were tested for their ability to repel the German cockroach, Blattella germanica (L.) (Blattodea: Blattellidae), in Plexiglas enclosures. Device A generated peak frequencies at 26 kHz and 34 kHz, and produced a 95 ±1 dB sound pressure level (SPL) at 50 cm distance (0 dB = 20 log 10[20 μPa/ 20 μPa]). Device B generated peak frequencies at 27 kHz and 35 kHz, and produced a 92 ± 4 dB SPL. Device C generated a wide range of frequencies between 28-42 kHz and produced an 88 ±2 dB SPL. Ultrasound from any of the three devices did not demonstrate sufficient repelling ability against the German cockroach in the tests. The result failed to provide evidence that ultrasonic technology could be used as an effective pest management tool to repel or eliminate the German cockroach.  相似文献   

15.
2012年6月,对湖南省石门县壶瓶山国家级自然保护区神景洞短嘴金丝燕的回声定位叫声进行研究,在黑暗山洞内使用录音仪器录制其自由飞行状态的声音后使用声音软件进行分析.短嘴金丝燕捕食归巢时,快速飞入洞口,在洞内有光区域不发声,到达洞内黑暗区域后开始发出回声定位叫声,且飞行速度减慢.声音分析结果表明其回声定位叫声为双脉冲组的噪声脉冲串型(noise burst),组内脉冲间隔很短[(6.6±0.42)ms],组间脉冲间隔较长[(99.3±3.86) ms],两者差异显著(P<0.01).对比第一、第二脉冲声音参数发现,主频和脉冲时程差异不显著,第一、第二脉冲主频分别为(6.2±0.08) kHz和(6.2±0.10) kHz (P>0.05);脉冲时程分别为(2.9±0.12) ms和(3.2±0.17) ms (P>0.05);最高和最低频率差异显著,第一、第二脉冲最高频率分别为(20.1±1.10) kHz和(15.4±0.98) kHz (P<0.01),最低频率分别为(3.7±0.12) kHz和(4.0±0.09)kHz (P<0.05);第一脉冲频宽((16.5±1.17) kHz)宽于第二脉冲((11.4±1.01) kHz) (P<0.01);且第一脉冲能量[(-32.5±0.60) dB]高于第二脉冲[(-35.2±0.94) dB] (P<0.05).另外,短嘴金丝燕在黑暗山洞内的回声定位叫声还包含了部分超声波,最高频率可达33.2 kHz.  相似文献   

16.
Envelope following responses were measured in two bottlenose dolphins in response to sinusoidal amplitude modulated tones with carrier frequencies from 20 to 60 kHz and modulation rates from 100 to 5,000 Hz. One subject had elevated hearing thresholds at higher frequencies, with threshold differences between subjects varying from ±4 dB at 20 and 30 kHz to +40 dB at 50 and 60 kHz. At each carrier frequency, evoked response amplitudes and phase angles were plotted with respect to modulation frequency to construct modulation rate transfer functions. Results showed that both subjects could follow the stimulus envelope components up to at least 2,000 Hz, regardless of carrier frequency. There were no substantial differences in modulation rate transfer functions for the two subjects suggesting that reductions in hearing sensitivity did not result in reduced temporal processing ability. In contrast to earlier studies, phase data showed group delays of approximately 3.5 ms across the tested frequency range, implying generation site(s) within the brainstem rather than the periphery at modulation rates from 100 to 1,600 Hz. This discrepancy is believed to be the result of undersampling of the modulation rate during previous phase measurements.  相似文献   

17.
Auditory event-related brain potentials (ERP) in response to two different tone stimuli (1.1 kHz or 1 kHz, 80 dB, 50 ms; given by headphones at a regular interstimulus interval of 5 s with a probability distribution of 70:30) were recorded from 12 healthy male subjects (Ss) during four different conditions with two repetitions: A-60 dBA white noise (wN), no whole-body vibration (WBV); B-60 dBA wN plus sinusoidal WBV in the az-direction with a frequency of 2.01 Hz and acceleration of 2 m.s-2 root mean square; C-80 dBA wN, no WBV; D-80 dBA wN plus WBV. Each condition consisted of two runs of about 11 min interrupted by a break of 4 min. During the break with continuing exposure, but without auditory stimuli, Ss judged the difficulty of the tone-detection task and intensity of noise by means of cross-modality matching (CMM). Vibration-synchronous activity in the electrocardiogram was eliminated by a subtraction-technique. Noise caused an attenuation of the N1 and P2 amplitudes and prolongation of P3 latencies. The WBV did not cause systematic ERP effects. Condition B was associated with higher N1 and smaller P3 amplitudes. The factor "condition" had a significant effect on the peak latencies of P3 to target stimuli and the task difficulty judged by CMM. Both effects exhibited significant linear increases in the sequence of conditions A, B, C, D. For the evaluation of exposure conditions at work, it can be suggested that noise has a strong systematic effect which can be enhanced by WBV.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

18.
Recent investigations have implicated that the central nervous system has a role in the changes that occur in auditory function following acoustic trauma caused by noise exposure. These investigations indicate that the inferior colliculus may be the primary anatomical location in the ascending auditory pathway where noise-induced neuronal plasticity occurs, thereby resulting in changes in the neuronal processing of auditory information. In the present investigation, we show that the amplitudes of all peaks in the click-evoked response from the external nucleus of the inferior colliculus decrease during a 30 min exposure to a tone (104 dB sound pressure level (SPL) at 4 kHz and 8 kHz). After tone exposure, the amplitudes of two of the peaks of the response from the external nucleus of the inferior colliculus that reflect the input from more caudal structures slowly returned to baseline levels, whereas the amplitudes of the two peaks reflecting neuronal activity in the inferior colliculus increased above baseline levels and remained at the increased levels for at least 90 min following exposure to the tone.We also show that exposure to a 4 kHz tone at 104 dB SPL causes changes in the neuronal processing of tonebursts in the form of changes in the temporal integration function for one of the peaks of the response from the external nucleus of the inferior colliculus that originates in the inferior colliculus. Before tone exposure the amplitude of this peak decreased with increasing stimulus duration, but after tone exposure the amplitude of this peak was independent of the duration of the toneburst stimulus.We interpret these changes as evidence that noise exposure (tone exposure) causes changes in the excitability of the inferior colliculus that are not seen in more caudal structures, and these changes are probably a result of a change in the balance between inhibition and excitation in the inferior colliculus.  相似文献   

19.
为探讨下丘(Inferior colliculus,IC)回声定位信号主频范围内的神经元的时程选择性,在自由声场刺激条件下,我们在4 只普氏蹄蝠的IC 采用不同时程的声刺激,研究了神经元的时程选择性。通过在体细胞外记录,共获得56 个声敏感下丘神经元,其记录深度、最佳频率和最小阈值的范围分别为1547 - 3967 (2878. 9 ±629.1)μm,20 -68 (49.0 ± 11. 1)kHz 和36.5 -95. 5 (59. 8 ±13. 0)dB SPL。根据所记录到的下丘神经元对不同时程的声刺激的反应,即对不同时程的选择性(Duration selectivity),将其分为6 种类型:短通型(Short-pass,SP,n = 11/56)、带通型(Band-pass,BP,n = 1/56)、长通型(Long-pass,LP,n = 5 /56)、反带通型(Band-reject,BR,n = 3 /56)、多峰型(Multi-peak,MP,n =6 /56)和全通型(All-pass,AP,n =30 /56)或非时程选择型(Nonduration-selective,NDS)。通过比较普氏蹄蝠下丘谐波主频内和主频外神经元的时程选择性,我们发现处于回声定位信号主频范围内神经元(n =32)比主频外神经元(n = 24)具有更短的最佳时程和更高的时程选择性。结果提示,在普氏蹄蝠回声定位过程中谐波主频内神经元较谐波主频外神经元发挥了更为重要的作用。  相似文献   

20.
The detection and recognition of acoustic communication signals masked by noise was investigated in a grasshopper (Chorthippus biguttulus) whose auditory system exhibits only poor spectral resolution and therefore has to operate in the time domain. The signals of this species consist of numerous identical subunits that enable the receiver, in principle, to make repetitive measurements. We aimed at determining the maximum integration time in this species by using stimuli of different durations under increasing noise levels. As a criterion for recognition the typical phonotactic turning response of the males was evaluated, which is reliably triggered by a female song, and thus is a sensitive indicator for recognition of conspecific signals. When confronted with a long signal (1000 ms) males tolerated a 2.4 dB higher noise level as compared to a short signal (250 ms). Noise tolerance improved with increasing signal duration from 250 ms to 450 ms. Beyond this signal duration, however, no further improvement was observed, indicating an upper limit for temporal integration that corresponds to only five song subunits. The gain in noise tolerance had a slope of 2.7 dB per doubling duration, which corresponds to the expectation derived from an energy detector model (3 dB per doubling duration) rather than to the value expected from signal detection theory (1.5 dB per doubling duration).  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号