共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Substrate choice is one of the most important decisions thatsit-and-wait predators must make. Not only may it dictate theprey available but also the cover for the predator which mayconceal it from prey or its own predators. However, while ona particular substrate the behavior and use of that substratemay vary widely. When naïve, newly emerged crab spiderlingsMisumena vatia (Thomisidae) occupied flowering goldenrod Solidagocanadensis, their behavior differed markedly on inflorescenceswith relatively sparse and densely packed flower heads as wellas on experimentally thinned and unthinned inflorescences. Initially,the spiderlings most often hunted at the thinned sites and hidamong the dense flower heads at the unthinned sites, a differencethat disappeared in all broods tested after 23 h, possiblybecause of the growing hunger of the initially concealed individuals.Prey capture (dance flies) in the thinned sites initially significantlyexceeded that in unthinned sites but subsequently did not differ.However, spiderlings encountered their principal predator, thejumping spider Pelegrina insignis, significantly more oftenon unthinned than thinned inflorescences. Even though usagepatterns initially differed strikingly, spiderlings did notdiffer in their rates of quitting the two types of sites. Theseresults suggest a trade-off between foraging and predator avoidancethat changes in response to increasing hunger over time. 相似文献
2.
In males that compete aggressively for females, size and agemay determine which males obtain access to these females. Inthe present study, we use the crab spider, Misumena vatia, aspecies with males that do not grow after becoming sexuallymature adults, to test the hypothesis that age affects the successof males competing for access to females. M. vatia is an excellentspecies to test this hypothesis because it is possible to disentangleage from size, characters that typically vary together in thespecies usually tested. We staged encounters between similar-sizedolder and younger adult male M. vatia in the presence of a femaleto determine the role of age in male access to females. Encountersbetween the males occurred during 63.3% of these pairings. Youngermales won significantly more (70.2%) of the encounters thandid older ones, but did not initiate significantly more encountersthan did older ones (62.5%). Although older males won only 29.8%of these encounters, they initiated significantly more (76.5%)of them than predicted by chance. This design also allowed usto test Parker's hypothesis that older individuals should exhibita higher level of aggression than younger ones. However, attacksby younger males were most likely to include extensive bodilycontact, whereas attacks by older males involved significantlyless contact. These results counter the frequent assertion thatolder individuals usually prevail over younger ones in contestsfor access to females, and that older males are more likelyto engage in highly overt aggression than are younger ones.Aging may decrease reproductive opportunities and success ratesof male M. vatia, affecting as many as nearly one-fourth oftheir encounters. 相似文献
3.
The low population density of the crab spider Misumena vatiaand the high percentage of gravid adult females begs the question,how do adult males find reproductive females? We explored oneof the potential mate-locating tactics of adult males, theirline-following behavior. M. vatia do not build webs; however,they do lay down silken lines during their movements. In boththe field and the laboratory, adult males (but not juvenilemales) regularly followed lines of other individuals, as wellas their own, sometimes multiple times. However, they did notdistinguish the direction in which lines were laid, and theyeven followed lines of other species. Males responded mechanicallyto these lines, but not chemically. They followed lines ofpenultimate and adult virgin females, as well as new and old
lines, with similar high frequency. Neither washing the lineswith water nor washing them with acetone (to remove possiblewater-soluble or organically soluble pheromones) affected theirchoice of lines. Due to the low activity of females, theirlines may be several days old, over which time any possible
chemical information would likely dissipate. Therefore, pheromonesseem unlikely to aid males searching for unmated adult females.Nevertheless, even searching males that follow lines largelyindiscriminately should derive a benefit because concentrationsof lines could denote the presence of females and thereby maximizenumbers of females encountered. 相似文献
4.
Sit‐and‐wait predators use relatively simple rules for their decisions to choose and leave a patch, such as using the direct presence of prey to select a hunting site. However, the direct presence of prey can only be used when there is a highly visited patch in the proximity of the predator. Therefore, it is plausible that sit‐and‐wait predators also exploit indirect cues of prey presence and, consequently, use associative learning to select a hunting site. The present study tests for the role of associative learning in a sit‐and‐wait predator species for which the ecology is well understood: Misumena vatia Clerck crab spiders. An ecologically relevant scenario is used by selecting flower colour as the conditioned stimulus and prey presence as the unconditioned stimulus. The results provide no evidence that M. vatia crab spiders use the association between flower colour and food presence for selecting a hunting site. After a training phase of being exposed to a colourful artificial flower highly visited by bees, spiders select a hunting site independently of its colour during the testing phase. Investigations of similar scope and ecological relevance are required with other sit‐and‐wait predators to identify the conditions promoting the use of associative learning for foraging site selection when animals face an unpredictable food supply. 相似文献
5.
Adult female crab spiders (Misumena valid) released in a meadowmoved to milkweed stems more often than to other sites and alsomoved to flowering milkweed stems more often than to equallyavailable nonflowering ones. Preferences of the spiders as apopulation were similar during initial and second trials, butgiven individuals did not repeat their initial choices moreoften than predicted by chance. Thus, spiders probably respondeddirectly to cues present, rather than to information retainedfrom earlier experience. Spiders placed at the base of floweringmilkweed stems did not choose these sites more often than predictedby chance or more often than did spiders released in the grass,suggesting that this increase in localization provided no additionalinformation for choosing a hunting site. However, spiders placedat the bases of nonflowering stems selected them less oftenthan predicted. Second trials with these spiders also resembledinitial trials, but, as with the spiders released in the grass,given individuals did not repeat their initial performance moreoften than predicted. Thus, spiders released in the grass probablyresponded directly to cues present, as did spiders releasedat the base of stems. The choices of these spiders, combinedwith flexible giving-up times, caused the spiders to be concentratedon flowering stems. Although this strategy results in a better-than-randompattern of selecting hunting sites, the spiders lose considerableforaging time in the process. 相似文献
6.
R. R. Borisov A. B. Epelbaum N. V. Kryakhova A. G. Tertitskaya N. P. Kovatcheva 《Russian Journal of Marine Biology》2007,33(4):227-231
The cannibalistic behavior during all life history stages of the red king crab Paralithodes camtschaticus (Tilesius, 1815) raised under laboratory conditions was studied. All feeding stages were found to be cannibalistic. The major factors affecting the level of cannibalism were identified, and methods were proposed to reduce cannibalism intensity under laboratory conditions. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
Cannibalism may cause considerable mortality on juvenile fish and it has been hypothesised that it may exercise selection on offspring size in that larger offspring may enjoy a size refuge. For this to be evolutionarily advantageous the survival of individual offspring must compensate for the reduced fecundity implied by larger offspring size. We develop a model which combines standard assumptions of size‐dependent mortality with adult cannibalism to investigate the potential for cannibalism to act as selective force on offspring size. We find that for this potential to be realised, the mortality due to cannibalism must exceed a threshold value that is a decreasing function of non‐cannibalistic predation intensity, cannibalized size range width and the average cannibalized size. If cannibalism exceeds this threshold, the model predicts evolution of offspring size towards refuges above or below cannibalized size range depending on initial offspring size. Cannibalistic mortality cannot be so great that the population is non‐viable, however, the range of parameter values describing cannibalistic intensity allowed within these boundaries is wide. On this basis, we suggest that cannibalism is a potential mechanism for offspring size selection. 相似文献
10.
S. C. Strum 《International journal of primatology》1994,15(5):739-765
I reexamined male competitive options via long-term data on adolescent and adult male consort success and data on male weights, age, size, residency status, and friendships with females collected from one troop of wild baboons. The options available to a male result from a complex interaction of variables and change with maturation, migration, and age. The acquisition of aggressive and social competence, the development of alternative nonaggressive strategies, the loss of physical prowess, and perhaps the effect of inbreeding avoidance all play a role. Aggressive and nonaggressive strategies may be mutually exclusive options for brief periods in a male’s life because of ontogenetic, historical, or physical constraints. But for most of the time, the two are likely to coexist. I assess the evolutionary significance of optional strategies of competition and discuss the conditions that might select for optional strategies. 相似文献
11.
12.
13.
Eric L. Charnov 《Evolutionary ecology》1991,5(3):339-342
Summary Certain dimensionless numbers related to life histories are approximately conserved within some taxa; this suggests that the underlying life-history tradeoffs satisfy yet-to-be-discovered symmetry principles. Some possible examples are discussed. 相似文献
14.
Males of the hermit crab, Pagurusfilholi, often grasp the edges of shells occupied by females and drag them during the mating season. This behavior was experimentally confirmed to be a precopulatory guarding behavior displayed by males for ripe females, and males were found to recognize females which were within about 5 days of spawning. Most theoretical models for mating preference assume the choosing sex (the male in the present case) has complete reproductive information about potential mates, and predict that males will preferably choose more fecund females and/or females that will require less guarding time (i.e. that will spawn sooner) as partners. Several male-choice experiments between two ripe females, both previously guarded by other males, were carried out to examine the above predictions. Males did not prefer females of larger size, higher fecundity or with less time remaining until spawning. These results suggest that males may not have complete information about potential partners, rather that male hermit crabs may adopt a mating strategy of pairing with the first ripe female they encounter. Even with such incomplete mate assessment, males may enhance their reproductive success by recognizing ripe females that will spawn within a given time (about 5 days in the present case). 相似文献
15.
Cohabitation of juvenile females with mature males promotes sexual cannibalism in fishing spiders 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Precopulatory sexual cannibalism, or female predation of a potentialmate before mating, illustrates an extreme intersexual conflict.Unlike sexual cannibalism occurring after mating, precopulatorysexual cannibalism cannot be construed as a male strategy. Thus,research on the adaptive significance of this phenomenon hasfocused on female benefits. In the present study, I test theidea that precopulatory sexual cannibalism represents an adaptivefemale trade-off between the material costs and benefits ofmating with a male (forgoing food, securing sperm) and preyingon a male (forgoing sperm, securing food). I pay particularattention to the rarely tested prediction that precopulatorysexual cannibalism by virgin females should increase as eachfemale's expectation of future mating opportunities increases.I use the phenomenon of cohabitation between adult males andjuvenile females nearing sexual maturity as a means to manipulatefemale expectation of future mate availability. Results indicatethat feeding on a male has significant positive effects on severalmeasures of female fecundity. However, the likelihood of precopulatoryattacks was not explained by a female's recent feeding history.Finally, as predicted, juvenile female cohabitation with maturemales (expectation of future mating opportunities) heightensthe prevalence of precopulatory attacks by virgin females, suggestingthat juvenile experience can alter a female's propensity forsexual cannibalism. This is the first study to suggest thatjuvenile experience can alter a female's propensity for sexualcannibalism. This finding emphasizes the point that studiesof sexual selection and mating systems need to consider theeffects of juvenile experience on adult behavior. 相似文献
16.
1. Cannibalism has often been suggested as an important mechanism to reach the necessary developmental stage and size before a critical time horizon is reached, but this role has been largely unexplored. We studied effects of cannibalism on the life history of the damselfly Lestes viridis under combinations of a time constraint (by manipulating the perceived time available in the growth season) and a biotic constraint (density). 2. Larvae had a faster development and growth rate when reared at high time stress (late photoperiod). They also had a higher growth rate and mass at emergence when cannibalism occurred (density 2 and 4). Cannibalism occurred earlier at higher density. Accelerated life history responses (faster development and growth rate) and a higher mass at emergence were dependent upon the timing of cannibalism. Responses were more pronounced or only present if cannibalism occurred early in the larval period. 3. Our data suggest that cannibalism may not only act as a lifeboat mechanism by enabling cannibals to survive detrimental ecological conditions, but may also act as a compensatory mechanism to keep life history variables near‐optimal at life history transitions, even under sub‐optimal conditions. 相似文献
17.
REBECCA S. LEGRAND DOUGLASS H. MORSE 《Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London》2000,71(4):643-664
Sexual size dimorphism is often a likely outcome of the interplay between natural selection and sexual selection, with female size dictated primarily by natural selection that maximizes fecundity and male size by sexual selection that maximizes reproductive opportunities. Attention to male fitness has focused heavily on direct male-male conflict selecting for superior male size and/or fighting ability, although male reproductive traits vary immensely among animals. An alternative, advanced by Michael Ghiselin, posits highly mobile dwarf males as a strategy for finding relatively immobile females in low-density populations. Adult male crab spiders Misumena vatia , sit-and-wait predators, are strikingly smaller, much more active, and relatively longer-legged than their females. This size difference results largely from males having two fewer instars than females, which simultaneously results in marked protandry. Populations of M. vatia often were small and of low density, with a female-biased sex ratio and an operational sex ratio that changed strikingly over the season. Sexual selection through scramble competition (locating the female first) should favour this suite of characters in males of low-density populations. Although direct male-male contests favoured large males, the low densities of adult males and the dispersed, relatively immobile females led to low levels of direct intrasexual contest. Females exaggerated the problem of males in finding them by providing few cues to their presence, a pattern consistent with indirect mate choice. In addition to favouring high mobility, scramble competition favoured males that selected flowers attracting many prey, the sites most often occupied by females. 相似文献
18.
Previous studies have shown no significant effect of experimentaltail length manipulation in female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica)at the beginning of a breeding season on reproductive successor behavior during that breeding season. In the present study,we investigate if tail length manipulation had any effect onreproductive performance the following year, the so-called long-termeffect, in contrast to the short-term effects already studied.We found that females with experimentally elongated externaltail feathers at the beginning of a breeding season producedless offspring during the breeding season the following yearthan did females with shortened or unmanipulated tails. Theseresults suggest that tail elongation caused flight deficienciesthat deteriorated the condition of females and eventually reducedreproductive success. The finding of long-term effects but nosignificant short-term effects for female tail elongation suggeststhat female barn swallows have the ability to adjust immediateparental investment. Detrimental effects of long tails in femalesin terms of decreased reproductive success might explain whyfemale tails are not as long as those of males. Finally, femalesmated to long-tailed (sexually attractive) males decreased theirreproductive success the following year more than did femalesmated to short-tailed males, possibly owing to differentialparental effort causing a deterioration of their condition. 相似文献
19.
20.
Life history theory predicts tradeoffs among reproductive traits, but the physiological mechanisms underlying such tradeoffs remain unclear. Here we examine reproductive tradeoffs and their association with yolk steroids in an oviparous lizard. Female leopard geckos lay two eggs in a clutch, produce multiple clutches in a breeding season, and reproduce for several years. We detected a significant tradeoff between egg size and the number of clutches laid by females during their first two breeding seasons. Total reproductive effort was strongly condition-dependent in the first season, but much less so in the second season. Although these and other tradeoffs were unmistakable, they were not associated with levels of androstenedione, oestradiol, or testosterone in egg yolk. Female condition and egg size, however, were inversely related to dihydrotestosterone (DHT) levels in egg yolk. Finally, steroid levels in egg yolk were not directly related to steroid levels in the maternal circulation when follicles were developing, indicating that steroid transfer to eggs is regulated. These findings suggest that maternal allocation of DHT could mitigate tradeoffs that lead to poor offspring quality (i.e. poor female condition) and small offspring size (i.e. small egg size). 相似文献