共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The effective use of echolocation requires not only measuring the delay between the emitted call and returning echo to estimate
the distance of an ensonified object. To locate an object in azimuth and elevation, the bat’s auditory system must analyze
the returning echoes in terms of their binaural properties, i.e., the echoes’ interaural intensity and time differences (IIDs
and ITDs). The effectiveness of IIDs for echolocation is undisputed, but when bats ensonify complex objects, the temporal
structure of echoes may facilitate the analysis of the echo envelope in terms of envelope ITDs. Using extracellular recordings
from the auditory midbrain of the bat, Phyllostomus discolor, we found a population of neurons that are sensitive to envelope ITDs of echoes of their sonar calls. Moreover, the envelope-ITD
sensitivity improved with increasing temporal fluctuations in the echo envelopes, a sonar parameter related to the spatial
statistics of complex natural reflectors like vegetation. The data show that in bats envelope ITDs may be used not only to
locate external, prey-generated rustling sounds but also in the context of echolocation. Specifically, the temporal fluctuations
in the echo envelope, which are created when the sonar emission is reflected from a complex natural target, support ITD-mediated
echolocation. 相似文献
2.
Cynthia F. Moss Hans -Ulrich Schnitzler 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1989,165(3):383-393
1. | Echolocating bats use the time delay between emitted sounds and returning echoes to determine the distance to an object. This study examined the accuracy of target ranging by bats and the effect of echo bandwidth on the bat's performance in a ranging task. |
2. | Six big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) were trained in a yes-no procedure to discriminate between two phantom targets, one simulating a stationary target that reflected echoes at a fixed delay and another simulating a jittering target that reflected echoes undergoing small step-changes in delay. |
3. | Eptesicus fuscus emits a frequency modulated sonar sound whose first harmonic sweeps from approximately 55 to 25 kHz in about 2 ms. Sound energy is also present in the second and third harmonics, contributing to a broadband signal in which each frequency in the sound can provide a time marker for its arrival at the bat's ears. We estimated range jitter discrimination in bats under conditions in which the echo information available to the bat was manipulated. Baseline performance with unfiltered echoes was compared to that with filtered echoes (low-pass filtered at 55 kHz and at 40 kHz; high-pass filtered at 40 kHz). |
4. | The results indicate that the low-frequency portion of the first harmonic (25–40 kHz) is sufficient for the bat to discriminate echo delay changes of 0.4 microseconds. This echo delay discrimination corresponds to a distance discrimination of less than 0.07 mm. |
3.
de Araujo IE Gutierrez R Oliveira-Maia AJ Pereira A Nicolelis MA Simon SA 《Neuron》2006,51(4):483-494
The motivation to start or terminate a meal involves the continual updating of information on current body status by central gustatory and reward systems. Previous electrophysiological and neuroimaging investigations revealed region-specific decreases in activity as the subject's state transitions from hunger to satiety. By implanting bundles of microelectrodes in the lateral hypothalamus, orbitofrontal cortex, insular cortex, and amygdala of hungry rats that voluntarily eat to satiety, we have measured the behavior of neuronal populations through the different phases of a complete feeding cycle (hunger-satiety-hunger). Our data show that while most satiety-sensitive units preferentially responded to a unique hunger phase within a cycle, neuronal populations integrated single-unit information in order to reflect the animal's motivational state across the entire cycle, with higher activity levels during the hunger phases. This distributed population code might constitute a neural mechanism underlying meal initiation under different metabolic states. 相似文献
4.
Ulanovsky N Fenton MB Tsoar A Korine C 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2004,271(1547):1467-1475
Animals using active sensing systems such as echolocation or electrolocation may experience interference from the signals of neighbouring conspecifics, which can be offset by a jamming avoidance response (JAR). Here, we report JAR in one echolocating bat (Tadarida teniotis: Molossidae) but not in another (Taphozous perforatus: Emballonuridae) when both flew and foraged with conspecifics. In T. teniotis, JAR consisted of shifts in the dominant frequencies of echolocation calls, enhancing differences among individuals. Larger spectral overlap of signals elicited stronger JAR. Tadarida teniotis showed two types of JAR: (i) for distant conspecifics: a symmetric JAR, with lower- and higher-frequency bats shifting their frequencies downwards and upwards, respectively, on average by the same amount; and (ii) for closer conspecifics: an asymmetric JAR, with only the upper-frequency bat shifting its frequency upwards. In comparison, 'wave-type' weakly electric fishes also shift frequencies of discharges in a JAR, but unlike T. teniotis, the shifts are either symmetric in some species or asymmetric in others. We hypothesize that symmetric JAR in T. teniotis serves to avoid jamming and improve echolocation, whereas asymmetric JAR may aid communication by helping to identify and locate conspecifics, thus minimizing chances of mid-air collisions. 相似文献
5.
6.
7.
8.
Neuweiler G 《Trends in ecology & evolution》1989,4(6):160-166
The types of echolocation signal and the auditory capacities of echolocating bats are adapted to specific acoustical constraints of the foraging areas. Bats hunting insects above the canopy use low frequencies for echolocation; this is an adaptation to prey detection over long distances. Bats foraging close to and within foliage avoid masking of insect echoes by specializing on 'fluttering target' detection. 'Gleaning' bats are adapted to the auditory detection of very faint noises generated by ground-dwelling prey, and are capable of analysing fine changes in the echo spectrum, which may indicate a stationary prey changing its posture on a substrate. This review of recent research demonstrates that, in bats, foraging ecology and audition are intricately interrelated and interdependent. 相似文献
9.
High-frequency hearing is required for echolocating bats to locate, range and identify objects, yet little is known about its molecular basis. The discovery of a high-frequency hearing-related gene, KCNQ4, provides an opportunity to address this question. Here, we obtain the coding regions of KCNQ4 from 15 species of bats, including echolocating bats that have higher frequency hearing and non-echolocating bats that have the same ability as most other species of mammals. The strongly supported protein-tree resolves a monophyletic group containing all bats with higher frequency hearing and this arrangement conflicts with the phylogeny of bats in which these species are paraphyletic. We identify five parallel evolved sites in echolocating bats belonging to both suborders. The evolutionary trajectories of the parallel sites suggest the independent gain of higher frequency hearing ability in echolocating bats. This study highlights the usefulness of convergent or parallel evolutionary studies for finding phenotype-related genes and contributing to the resolution of evolutionary problems. 相似文献
10.
Neural responses in the inferior colliculus of echolocating bats to artificial orientation sounds and echoes 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The responses of single units and evoked potentials to a pair of artificial sounds, mimicking theorientation sound and echo, and to tape recorded actual orientation sounds were studied in terms of recovery cycle. the recovery cycle of single units could be classified into four groups: (1) short suppression (4%), (2) delayed inhibition (11%), (3) temporal recovery with or without a supernormal phase (7%), and (4) undelayed inhibition (78%) lasting 4 to 26 msec. therefore the majority of neurons were not excited by the second sound (echo) of a pair when it was delivered within several milliseconds after the first (out-going orientation sound). the duration of the recovery cycle was a function of the intensity of a pair of sounds. the weaker the first tone pulse relative to the second, the more rapid the recovery to the second. therefore, the reception of echoes is probably improved by contraction of middle ear muscles resulting in attenuation of self-stimulation by the out-going pulse. The collicular evoked potential consisted of two components, a fast one mainly due to the incoming fibers from lower levels and a slow one due to the main body of the inferior colliculus. The slow component showed slow recovery cycles as did the majority of single units while the fast one recovered very quickly. No noticeable difference in recovery cycles was found between awake and anesthetized animals. The functional meaning of inhibitory periods in the recovery cycle and role of the inferior colliculus in echo-location are discussed. 相似文献
11.
Classification of insects by echolocating greater horseshoe bats 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Gerhard von der Emde Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1990,167(3):423-430
Summary Echolocating greater horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum) detect insects by concentrating on the characteristic amplitude- and frequency modulation pattern fluttering insects impose on the returning echoes. This study shows that horseshoe bats can also further analyse insect echoes and thus recognize and categorize the kind of insect they are echolocating.Four greater horseshoe bats were trained in a twoalternative forced-choice procedure to choose the echo of one particular insect species turning its side towards the bat (Fig. 1). The bats were able to discriminate with over 90% correct choices between the reward-positive echo and the echoes of other insect species all fluttering with exactly the same wingbeat rate (Fig. 4).When the angular orientation of the reward-positive insect was changed (Fig. 2), the bats still preferred these unknown echoes over echoes from other insect species (Fig. 5) without any further training. Because the untrained bats did not show any prey preference, this indicates that the bats were able to perform an aspect-anglein-dependent classification of insects.Finally we tested what parameters in the echo were responsible for species recognition. It turned out that the bats especially used the small echo-modulations in between glints as a source of information (Fig. 7). Neither the amplitudenor the frequencymodulation of the echoes alone was sufficient for recognition of the insect species (Fig. 8). Bats performed a pattern recognition task based on complex computations of several acoustic parameters, an ability which might be termed cognitive.Abbreviations
AM
amplitude modulation
-
CF
constant frequency
-
FM
frequency modulation
-
S+
positive stimulus
-
S-
negative stimulus 相似文献
12.
Albert S. Feng 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2011,197(5):595-603
Echolocating bats assess target range by the delay in echo relative to the emitted sonar pulse. Earlier studies in FM bats
showed that a population of neurons in auditory centers above the inferior colliculus (IC) is tuned to echo delay, with different
neurons tuned to different echo delays. A building block for delay-tuned responses is paradoxical latency shift (PLS), featuring
longer response latencies to more intense sounds. PLS is first created in the IC, where neurons exhibit unit-specific quantum increase
in response latency with increasing sound level. Other IC neurons display oscillatory discharges whose period is unit-specific
and level tolerant, indicating that this is attributable to cell’s intrinsic properties. High-threshold inhibition of oscillatory
discharge produces PLS, indicating that oscillatory discharge is a building block for PLS. To investigate the cellular basis
of oscillatory discharges, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from IC neurons in leopard frogs (which also exhibit
oscillatory discharges and PLS). These recordings show that IC neurons are heterogeneous displaying diverse biophysical phenotypes;
each phenotype (and cell) has its own membrane time constant, input resistance, and strengths of I
h, I
kir, I
kv—these intrinsic properties give rise to cell-specific resonance which can be observed through current and afferent stimulations. 相似文献
13.
14.
Echolocating bats can identify three-dimensional objects exclusively through the analysis of acoustic echoes of their ultrasonic emissions. However, objects of the same structure can differ in size, and the auditory system must achieve a size-invariant, normalized object representation for reliable object recognition. This study describes both the behavioral classification and the cortical neural representation of echoes of complex virtual objects that vary in object size. In a phantom-target playback experiment, it is shown that the bat Phyllostomus discolor spontaneously classifies most scaled versions of objects according to trained standards. This psychophysical performance is reflected in the electrophysiological responses of a population of cortical units that showed an object-size invariant response (14/109 units, 13%). These units respond preferentially to echoes from objects in which echo duration (encoding object depth) and echo amplitude (encoding object surface area) co-varies in a meaningful manner. These results indicate that at the level of the bat's auditory cortex, an object-oriented rather than a stimulus-parameter-oriented representation of echoes is achieved. 相似文献
15.
Dina K.N. Dechmann Silke L. Heucke Luca Giuggioli Kamran Safi Christian C. Voigt Martin Wikelski 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2009,276(1668):2721-2728
Group foraging has been suggested as an important factor for the evolution of sociality. However, visual cues are predominantly used to gain information about group members'' foraging success in diurnally foraging animals such as birds, where group foraging has been studied most intensively. By contrast, nocturnal animals, such as bats, would have to rely on other cues or signals to coordinate foraging. We investigated the role of echolocation calls as inadvertently produced cues for social foraging in the insectivorous bat Noctilio albiventris. Females of this species live in small groups, forage over water bodies for swarming insects and have an extremely short daily activity period. We predicted and confirmed that (i) free-ranging bats are attracted by playbacks of echolocation calls produced during prey capture, and that (ii) bats of the same social unit forage together to benefit from passive information transfer via the change in group members'' echolocation calls upon finding prey. Network analysis of high-resolution automated radio telemetry confirmed that group members flew within the predicted maximum hearing distance 94±6 per cent of the time. Thus, echolocation calls also serve as intraspecific communication cues. Sociality appears to allow for more effective group foraging strategies via eavesdropping on acoustical cues of group members in nocturnal mammals. 相似文献
16.
17.
Issachar L. López-Cuamatzi Víctor H. Vega-Gutiérrez Iván Cabrera-Campos Eduardo Ruiz-Sanchez Jorge Ayala-Berdon Romeo A. Saldaña-Vázquez 《Mammal Review》2020,50(3):304-313
- Echolocation is the ability of some animals to orient themselves through sound emission and interpretation of the echoes. This is bats’ main sense for orientation and recognising biotopes that provide food, water, and roosts. It is widely accepted that echolocation call frequency is related to body mass, and this relationship has been described as the ‘allometric hypothesis’, which proposes a negative correlation between these variables.
- There is evidence that, in many cases, the allometric hypothesis does not apply. Additionally, studies supporting this hypothesis were done at the family level, resulting in a broad range of correlation values with r ranging from −0.36 to −0.76, and only insectivorous bats were included. Due to the notable exceptions and the lack of a quantitative synthesis of this hypothesis including all echolocating bats, we evaluated the allometric hypothesis of echolocation calls for this group.
- Using a meta-analysis and phylogenetic generalised least-squares techniques, we evaluated the relationship between echolocation call peak frequency and the body mass of bats.
- We found a negative relationship between body mass and echolocation call peak frequency for the 85 bat species that were included in our analysis (r = −0.3, p = 0.005). The relationship was consistent when we analysed the data at the insectivorous guild level, and in bats belonging to the families Vespertilionidae, Rhinolophidae, Emballonuridae, and the genus Myotis. However, the wide range of r values suggests that the strength of the relationship between peak frequency and body mass varies within the order Chiroptera.
- Our results support the allometric hypothesis of sound production in echolocating bats. However, the low coefficient we found suggests that factors other than body mass may influence the peak frequency of echolocation calls produced by bats.
18.
19.
Mariana L. Melcón Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler Annette Denzinger 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2009,195(1):69-77
The approach phase of landing vespertilionid bats ends with a group of calls, which either consists of buzz I alone or buzz
I and buzz II. To understand the possible role of buzz II, we trained Myotis myotis to land on a vertical grid, and compared the flight and echolocation behavior during approach in trials with and without
buzz II. During the approach, we did not find any differences in the echolocation behavior until the end of buzz I which indicated
whether buzz II was emitted or not. However, bats flying from the periphery of the flight channel, such that they had to make
a small turn at the very last moment, finished the sequence with a buzz II. Bats flying on a rather stereotyped trajectory
near the center of the flight channel without last instant corrections emitted buzz I alone. Our results indicate that buzz
II occurred only on trajectories that implied a higher risk to fail at landing. The information delivered by buzz II reaches
the bat too late to be used for landing. Therefore, we hypothesize that buzz II may help the bats to evaluate unsuccessful
attempts and to eventually react adequately. 相似文献
20.
Many species of echolocating bats emit intense orientation sounds. If such intense sounds directly stimulated their ears, detection of faint echoes would be impaired. Therefore, possible mechanisms for the attenuation of self-stimulation were studied with Myotis lucifugus. The acoustic middle-ear-muscle reflex could perfectly and transiently regulate the amplitude of an incoming signal only at its beginning. However, its shortest latency in terms of electromyograms and of the attenuation of the cochlear microphonic was 3-4 and 4-8 msec, respectively, so that these muscles failed to attenuate orientation signals by the reflex. The muscles, however, received a message from the vocalization system when the bat vocalized, and contracted synchronously with vocalization. The duration of the contraction-relaxation was so short that the self-stimulation was attenuated, but the echoes were not. The tetanus-fusion frequency of tha stapedium muscle ranged between 260 and 320/sec. Unlike the efferent fibres in the lateral-line and vestibular systems, the olivo-cochlear bundle showed no sign of attenuation of self-stimulation. 相似文献