共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
We show that, in 1862, Richard Burton collected the type specimen of Pan troglodytes vellerosus not on Mount Cameroon, as has been generally assumed, but in Gabon. Therefore, P. t. vellerosus is not the correct name for the chimpanzee population of western Cameroon and southern Nigeria, if that population is taxonomically distinct. As First Reviser, we choose the name Pan troglodytes ellioti for this population of chimpanzees, based on Anthropopithecus ellioti named by Matschie [Matschie P (1914) Neue Affen aus Mittelafrika. Sitzungsber Ges Naturforsch Freunde Berlin 1914:323–342] from a specimen in the Humboldt Museum, Berlin, collected in Bascho (=Basho), Cameroon, and given to the museum in 1905. 相似文献
2.
Coprophagy occurred during major periods of feeding on fruits of Dialium spp. (Caesalpiniaceae) in a group of orphaned chimpanzees released in Conkouati Douli National Park, Republic of Congo. Since stress, boredom or food scarcity could not explain coprophagy according to our daily behavioral and veterinary control observations, we suggest that Dialium seeds were the item of interest in the feces. Two types of Dialium seeds were commonly found in the feces after chimpanzees swallowed the mesocarp and whole seeds together. These seeds were either whole and hard or whole/broken and soft imbibed. A mechanical and/or chemical effect of the gut passage may enable the chimpanzees to chew and ingest the seeds, thus providing nutritional intake. 相似文献
3.
Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania: A preliminary study
Hitoshige Hayaki Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(2):187-197
Dominance relationships among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were analyzed. Although all
adolescent males were unequivocally subordinate to all adult males, dominance relationships within the age classes were much
less clear. Especially among adolescent males, few pant-grunts or agonistic interactions occurred. While adolescent males
frequently pant-grunted at adult males, these latter males, except the alpha and the youngest, rarely pant-grunted to one
another. This suggests that a difference of social status exists between adolescent and adult males. Adult males rarely display
overt dominance to one another probably because the presence of other males affects their interactions. Moreover, they seem
to try to keep their dominance relationship ambiguous when making it overt is not advantageous to them. This may be a political
way for males to coexist with one another in a unit-group. 相似文献
4.
Michio Nakamura William C. McGrew Linda F. Marchant Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》2000,41(3):237-248
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, scratch other individual's bodies while they groom them. This behavioral
pattern of “social scratch” is another example of locality-specific social behavior, or custom, as it is not found in the
Gombe National Park, Tanzania, about 150 km north of Mahale, nor has it been reported from any other sites of chimpanzee study.
Frequency of social scratch was correlated with frequency of social grooming, but not with frequency of self-scratch. Frequencies
of social scratch per grooming bout among adult and adoles-cent males, and from lactating females to infants or juveniles,
were high, and among males, higher-ranking males especially received more. These facts indicate some social function of the
behavior. Social scratch was directed mostly to the dorsal side of the body. However, when lactating females social scratched
to infants or juveniles, they scratched other body parts. Social scratch was not lateralized to left or right. We present
four hypotheses on the functional origin and on the learning process of this cultural behavioral pattern. 相似文献
5.
The use of perforating sticks and flexible stalks in combination for termite fishing and a complex tool-set of three components
used sequentially (stout chiel, bodkin, and dip-stick) to penetrate melipone and ground-dwelling bee hives byPan troglodytes troglodytes are documented or, inferred from circumstantial evidence. Functionally, termite extraction tools were similar to other locations
in west and central Africa, but the plants and the number of raw material species used were different. Tools varied in the
degree of modification (fraying ends). Chimpanzees in the Lossi forest seem to be able to use the tools not in a stereotyped
fashion, but in a flexible, insightful way. The extraction of Melipone honey using large pieces of wood as pounding tools
has rarely been recorded elsewhere. The most impressive technological solution to the honey-getting problem by wild chimpanzees
was shown by this study. This is the only known, use of a tool-set of three components in sequence to extract honey by wild
chimpanzees. 相似文献
6.
Captive female chimpanzees who have had no opportunity to observe mothers with infants or to interact with infants often show inappropriate maternal behavior, particularly with their first-born infant, and this usually results in the removal of the infant to be human-reared. The present study used two techniques to encourage appropriate maternal behavior in ten pregnant female chimpanzees. These females were housed together with unrelated infant chimpanzees to adopt, or with lactating female chimpanzees and infants to observe. In five cases both techniques were used, in two cases only the first technique was used, and in three cases only the second technique was used. All ten female chimpanzees showed appropriate maternal behavior when their infants were born, in contrast to a group of eight female chimpanzees who had no such experience whose infants had to be removed for human-rearing. It is suggested that these techniques, or adaptations of them, could be applied to many other captive female chimpanzees with similar results. 相似文献
7.
Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
Matsusaka T Nishie H Shimada M Kutsukake N Zamma K Nakamura M Nishida T 《Primates; journal of primatology》2006,47(2):113-122
Use of leaves or sticks for drinking water has only rarely been observed during long-term study of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Mahale. Recently, however, we observed 42 episodes of tool-use for drinking water (73 tools and two cases of using tool-sets) between 1999 and 2004. Interestingly, all of the performers were immature chimpanzees aged from 2 to 10 years. Immature chimpanzees sometimes observed the tool-using performance of others and subsequently reproduced the behavior, while adults usually paid no attention to the performance. This tool-use did not seem to occur out of necessity: (1) chimpanzees often used tools along streams where they could drink water without tools, (2) they used tools for drinking water from tree holes during the wet season when they could easily obtain water from many streams, and (3) the tool-using performance sometimes contained playful aspects. Between-site comparisons revealed that chimpanzees at drier habitats used tools for drinking water more frequently and in a more conventional manner. However, some variations could not be explained by ecological conditions. Such variations and the increase in this tool-use in recent years at Mahale strongly suggest that social learning plays an important role in the process of acquiring the behavior. We should note here that such behaviors that lack obvious benefits or necessity can be prevalent in a group. 相似文献
8.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(4):343-360
Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, was studied, by analyzing processes of play and interindividual relationships
in play. The results are discussed in relation to communication mechanisms. Play was initiated in several ways. Communication
about play seems to depend on the receiver's interpretation: They can interpret the sender's behavior as play, referring to
(1) play signals accompanied by the behavior, (2) transformation of the behavior in timing, strength, or rhythm, and (3) situation
of the occurrence. Initiation attempts sometimes failed because one hesitated in playing with the other. Although the stronger
often reduced his/her activity during play, play tended to escalate in activity. Players may enjoy such escalation. Play also
had a mechanism not to escalate into fighting. Play was influenced by individuals other than the players. The third party's
movement often affected the players' interaction. The term play does not indicate a behavior itself but the context of the
behavior: The players interpret their behaviors in their play context. 相似文献
9.
The M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, began to feed on three agricultural fruit species, guava, mango and lemon.
It took them 7–8 years until they began to taste these fruits since the villagers left the park area in 1974. Although adult
chimpanzees are conservative in their feeding habits, they are capable of rapidly acquiring new feeding habits, or new traditions,
once they notice that the food is suitable. 相似文献
10.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1988,29(2):147-161
Association partners of young chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park were analyzed. Juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees
associated frequently with their mothers, although mother-offspring association decreased as the offspring grew up. Males
tended to leave their mothers and associate with adult males, while females remained frequently associating with their mothers
in early adolescence. In late adolescence and young adulthood, males usually associated with adult males and cycling adult
females. Females may transfer into neighboring unit-groups in this stage. Although an immigrant female tended to be alone
when her estrous cycle stopped, she associated with many individuals, in particular with adult males, when she resumed cycling.
Some orphans were observed to associate frequently with particular adults. The findings were discussed in relation to the
unique characteristics of chimpanzee social system. 相似文献
11.
Michael A. Huffman Jonathan E. Page Michael V. K. Sukhdeo Shunji Gotoh Mohamedi S. Kalunde Thushara Chandrasiri G. H. Neil Towers 《International journal of primatology》1996,17(4):475-503
Swallowing whole leaves by chimpanzees and other African apes has been hypothesized to have an antiparasitic or medicinal
function, but detailed studies demonstrating this were lacking. We correlate for the first time quantifiable measures of the
health of chimpanzees with observations of leaf-swallowing in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. We obtained a total
of 27 cases involving the use ofAspilia mossambicensis (63%),Lippia plicata (7%),Hibiscus sp. (15%),Trema orientalis (4%), andAneilema aequinoctiale (11%), 15 cases by direct observation of 12 individuals of the Mahale M group. At the time of use, we noted behavioral symptoms
of illness in the 8 closely observed cases, and detected single or multiple parasitic infections (Strongyloides fulleborni,
Trichuris trichiura, Oesophagostomum stephanostomum) in 10 of the 12 individuals. There is a significant relationship between
the presence of whole leaves (range, 1–51) and worms of adultO. stephanostomum (range, 2–21) in the dung. HPLC analysis of leaf samples collected after use showed that thiarubrine A, a compound proposed
to act as a potent nematocide in swallowingAspilia spp., was not present in leaves ofA. mossambicensis or the three other species analyzed. Alternative nematocidal or egg-laying inhibition activity was not evident. Worms ofO. stephanostomum were recovered live and motile from chimpanzee dung, trapped within the folded leaves and attached to leaf surfaces by trichomes,
though some were moving freely within the fecal matter, suggesting that the physical properties of leaves may contribute to
the expulsion of parasites. We review previous hypotheses concerning leaf-swallowing and propose an alternative hypothesis
based on physical action. 相似文献
12.
Pruetz JD 《Primates; journal of primatology》2007,48(4):316-319
Much attention has been paid to how humans both adapt and acclimate to heat stress, primarily due to the relevance of these
issues to hominid evolution in open Plio-Pleistocene environments. However, little is known about the responses of human’s
closest living relative, the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), to similar environmental stressors. In southeastern Senegal, one of the hottest and driest habitats that chimpanzees (P. t. verus) live in today, apes rely on behavioral mechanisms of dealing with thermal stress. Chimpanzees’ use of caves was based primarily
on indirect evidence (feeding traces, feces, and hairs) gathered from one cave from January to December 2004, but data from
observational records collected from May 2001 through March 2006 supplement these data. The hypothesis that chimpanzees’ use
of caves is a response to heat was tested by collecting data on temperatures within the largest cave and in different habitats
used by chimpanzees, such as gallery forest and woodland. Results indicate that chimpanzees primarily use caves as shelters
during the hottest times of year and that caves are consistently and significantly cooler than open habitats. Insight into
the way that chimpanzees in Senegal cope with extreme temperatures may help us to better understand the behavior of early
hominids in such an environment. 相似文献
13.
Shigeo Uehara Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa Kazuhiko Hosaka Miya Hamai 《Primates; journal of primatology》1994,35(1):49-55
Usurpation of alpha status by males is a relatively rare event among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park,
western Tanzania, where long-term field studies have been carried out since 1965. Here we report three such cases and compare
the fate of those defeated alpha males. The formation of effective alliances with other adult males is considered to be crucial
in determining what will happen to the defeated alpha male in the rank order after the loss of his status. 相似文献
14.
Brief Communication: Quantitative‐ and molecular‐genetic differentiation in humans and chimpanzees: Implications for the evolutionary processes underlying cranial diversification 下载免费PDF全文
Timothy D. Weaver 《American journal of physical anthropology》2014,154(4):615-620
Estimates of the amount of genetic differentiation in humans among major geographic regions (e.g., Eastern Asia vs. Europe) from quantitative‐genetic analyses of cranial measurements closely match those from classical‐ and molecular‐genetic markers. Typically, among‐region differences account for ~10% of the total variation. This correspondence is generally interpreted as evidence for the importance of neutral evolutionary processes (e.g., genetic drift) in generating among‐region differences in human cranial form, but it was initially surprising because human cranial diversity was frequently assumed to show a strong signature of natural selection. Is the human degree of similarity of cranial and DNA‐sequence estimates of among‐region genetic differentiation unusual? How do comparisons with other taxa illuminate the evolutionary processes underlying cranial diversification? Chimpanzees provide a useful starting point for placing the human results in a broader comparative context, because common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) are the extant species most closely related to humans. To address these questions, I used 27 cranial measurements collected on a sample of 861 humans and 263 chimpanzees to estimate the amount of genetic differentiation between pairs of groups (between regions for humans and between species or subspecies for chimpanzees). Consistent with previous results, the human cranial estimates are quite similar to published DNA‐sequence estimates. In contrast, the chimpanzee cranial estimates are much smaller than published DNA‐sequence estimates. It appears that cranial differentiation has been limited in chimpanzees relative to humans. Am J Phys Anthropol 154:615–620, 2014. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 相似文献
15.
Michael A. Huffman Shunji Gotoh Linda A. Turner Miya Hamai Kozo Yoshida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1997,38(2):111-125
A longitudinal study of nematode infection in chimpanzees was conducted between 1989 and 1994 on the M group chimpanzees of
the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania during two annual dry and rainy season periods and a third rainy season. Chemical
and physical antiparasite properties of medicinal plant use against the strongyle nematodeOesophagostomum stephanostomum have recently been reported at Mahale. Here, the incidence of nematode infections were analyzed for seasonal trends to elucidate
the possible influence of parasite infection on previously reported seasonality of medicinal plant use and to test the hypothesis
that the use of these plants is stimulated byO. stephanostomum. The number of chimpanzees infected byO. stephanostomum was significantly higher in the rainy season than in the dry season of both 1989–1990 and 1991–1992. However, the incidence
ofTrichuris trichura andStrongyloides fuelleborni showed no seasonality. Reinfection of individuals byO. stephanostomum occurred in synchrony with annual variation in rainfall: there was a sharp rise in the occurrence of new infections per individual
within one to two months after the beginning of the first heavy rains of the season. This pattern coincides with the reproductive
cycle of this nematode species.O. stephanostomum (95%) infections were associated significantly more frequently with medicinal plant use than eitherT. trichiura (50%) orS. fuelleborni (40%) infections. These observations are consistent with previous reports for the increased use of these plants during the
rainy season and are consistent the hypothesis that medicinal plant use is stimulated byO. stephanostomum infection. 相似文献
16.
Hiroyuki Takasaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(2):121-129
The mating behavior of receptive females in the M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains, western Tanzania, was studied by intensive sampling over a period of 5 months. Restrictive matings
were observed only between parous females mostly in the ovulatory stage and prime adult males, in particular the alpha. Young,
nulliparous and/or non-ovulatory females tended to mate with immature or low-ranking adult males. By contrast, older, parous
and/or ovulatory females tended to mate with adult and higher-ranking males. These confirmed the results of a previous extensive
study of the same group. In addition, gradual shift from opportunistic to restrictive matings after inter-unit-group transfer
was revealed for 2 ex-K-group parous females. The findings were discussed in relation to the life history of female chimpanzees.
A National Park since 1984. 相似文献
17.
Kateřina Pomajbíková Klára J. Petrželková Ilona Profousová Jana Petrášová Svetlana Kišidayová Zora Varádyová David Modrý 《American journal of physical anthropology》2010,142(1):42-48
Intestinal entodiniomorphid ciliates are commonly diagnosed in the feces of wild apes of the genera Pan and Gorilla. Although some authors previously considered entodiniomorphid ciliates as possible pathogens, a symbiotic function within the intestinal ecosystem and their participation in fiber fermentation has been proposed. Previous studies have suggested that these ciliates gradually disappear under captive conditions. We studied entodiniomorphid ciliates in 23 captive groups of chimpanzees, three groups of captive bonobos and six populations of wild chimpanzees. Fecal samples were examined using Sheather's flotation and Merthiolate‐Iodine‐Formaldehyde Concentration (MIFC) methods. We quantified the number of ciliates per gram of feces. The MIFC method was more sensitive for ciliate detection than the flotation method. Ciliates of genus Troglodytella were detected in 13 groups of captive chimpanzees, two groups of bonobos and in all wild chimpanzee populations studied. The absence of entodiniomorphids in some captive groups might be because of the extensive administration of chemotherapeutics in the past or a side‐effect of the causative or prophylactic administration of antiparasitic or antibiotic drugs. The infection intensities of ciliates in captive chimpanzees were higher than in wild ones. We suppose that the over‐supply of starch, typical in captive primate diets, might induce an increase in the number of ciliates. In vitro studies on metabolism and biochemical activities of entodiniomorphids are needed to clarify their role in ape digestion. Am J Phys Anthropol 2010. © 2009 Wiley‐Liss, Inc. 相似文献
18.
19.
Comparative locomotor behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos: The influence of morphology on locomotion
Diane M. Doran 《American journal of physical anthropology》1993,91(1):83-98
Results from a 10 month study of adult male and female bonobos (Pan paniscus) in the Lomako Forest, Zaire, and those from a 7 month study of adult male and female chimpanzees in the Tai Forest, Ivory Coast (Pan troglodytes verus), were compared in order to determine whether there are species differences in locomotor behavior and substrate use and, if so, whether these differences support predictions made on the basis of interspecific morphological differences. Results indicate that bonobos are more arboreal than chimpanzees and that male bonobos are more suspensory than their chimpanzee counterpart. This would be predicted on the basis of male bonobo's longer and more narrow scapula. This particular finding is contrary to the prediction that the bonobo is a “scaled reduced version of a chimpanzee” with little or no positional behavior difference as had been suggested. This study provides the behavioral data necessary to untangle contradictory interpretations of the morphological differences between chimpanzees and bonobos, and raises a previously discussed (Fleagle: Size and Scaling in Primate Biology, pp. 1–19, 1985) but frequently overlooked point–that isometry in allometric studies does not necessarily equate with behavioral equivalence. Several researchers have demonstrated that bonobos and chimpanzees follow the same scaling trends for many features, and are in some sense functionally equivalent, since they manage to feed and reproduce. However, as reflected in their morphologies, they do so through different types and frequencies of locomotor behaviors. © 1993 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
20.
Postconflict management strategies have been defined as any postconflict interaction that mitigates the negative consequences of the preceding agonistic conflict. Although most studies have investigated postconflict interactions between former opponents or between victims and uninvolved bystanders, interactions between aggressors and bystanders have received much less attention. In this study, we examined a database of 1,102 agonistic interactions and their corresponding postconflict periods in two outdoor-housed groups of captive chimpanzees in order to test the occurrence of postconflict third-party affiliation of aggressors. Our results confirmed the occurrence of appeasement, i.e. postconflict affiliation by a bystander toward an aggressor, but failed to detect the occurrence of postconflict affiliation directed from aggressors toward bystanders. Appeasement rates did not differ according to the sex of the involved individuals. In addition, appeasement occurred more often in the absence of reconciliation than after its occurrence suggesting that appeasement may act as an alternative to reconciliation when the latter fails to occur. Both study groups showed behavioral specificity for appeasement, i.e. context-specific use of certain behaviors, supporting the view that chimpanzees exhibit highly visible explicit postconflict affiliation. 相似文献