首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Part I. Characteristics of locomotor behaviour of goldfish ( Carassius auratus ) were monitored automatically and the response to sensory cues provided by conspecifics was analysed. Goldfish were attracted to free-swimming conspecifics and oriented to them klinokinetically by decreasing their mean angle size of turns and increasing both their turning frequency and mean step length between turns. These locomotor changes were stimulus bound. Although the responses were mediated visually, blinded goldfish, which were not overtly attracted, nevertheless oriented orthokinetically to non-visual cues from the conspecific.
Part II. Simulation of the conspecific by models demonstrated that oval and horizontal rectangular shapes of the same area and their size interactions, elicited significant attraction whereas square and vertical rectangular shapes of the same area did not. Increasing speeds and/or size of the model tended to bring about stronger attraction.  相似文献   

2.
Two primary defence behaviours, fore-leg extension to enhance crypsis and swimming to bottom and remaining motionless, of a predatory water bug,Ranatra dispar, are described along with their subsequent effect on foraging behaviour. It was hypothesised that hungry predators would respond less and for a shorter duration compared with recently fed individuals when exposed to a model threat stimulus, thereby tending to take a higher risk of predation during feeding than satiated animals. A greater proportion of animals responded overall with the leg extension response compared with the swimming response, although the mean duration of the former was significantly shorter than the latter response. A significantly higher proportion of nonfasted predators responded, and for a longer duration than fasted individuals. The type of model used significantly effected the proportion of animals that responded with leg extensions but not on its duration. In contrast, both number of animals and the duration of the swim/motionless behaviour were significantly effected by stimulus type. The subsequent effect of these 2 behavioural responses on feeding behaviour was examined and showed that although about the same number of predators removed prey from their mouthparts during both responses, significantly more prey were dropped, and therefore lost, during swimming. The results clearly indicate the significant effects that defensive behaviours have on time budgets in foraging behaviour.  相似文献   

3.
Consistent with human gambling behaviour but contrary to optimal foraging theory, pigeons showed maladaptive choice behaviour in experiment 1 by choosing an alternative that provided on average two food pellets over an alternative that provided a certain three food pellets. On 20 per cent of the trials, choice of the two-pellet alternative resulted in a stimulus that always predicted ten food pellets; on the remaining 80 per cent of the trials, the two-pellet alternative resulted in a different stimulus that always predicted zero food pellets. Choice of the three-pellet alternative always resulted in three food pellets. This choice behaviour mimics human monetary gambling in which the infrequent occurrence of a stimulus signalling the winning event (10 pellets) is overemphasized and the more frequent occurrence of a stimulus signalling the losing event (zero pellets) is underemphasized, compared with the certain outcome associated with not gambling (the signal for three pellets). In experiment 2, choice of the two-pellet alternative resulted in ten pellets with a probability of 20 per cent following presentation of either stimulus. Choice of the three-pellet alternative continued to result in three food pellets. In this case, the pigeons reliably chose the alternative that provided a certain three pellets over the alternative that provided an average of two pellets. Thus, in experiment 1, the pigeons were responding to obtain the discriminative stimuli signalling reinforcement and the absence of reinforcement, rather than to obtain the variability in reinforcement.  相似文献   

4.
Individual variation in the response to environmental challenges depends partly on innate reaction norms, partly on experience-based cognitive/emotional evaluations that individuals make of the situation. The goal of this study was to investigate whether pre-existing differences in behaviour predict the outcome of such assessment of environmental cues, using a conditioned place preference/avoidance (CPP/CPA) paradigm. A comparative vertebrate model (European sea bass, Dicentrarchus labrax) was used, and ninety juvenile individuals were initially screened for behavioural reactivity using a net restraining test. Thereafter each individual was tested in a choice tank using net chasing as aversive stimulus or exposure to familiar conspecifics as appetitive stimulus in the preferred or non preferred side respectively (called hereafter stimulation side). Locomotor behaviour (i.e. time spent, distance travelled and swimming speed in each tank side) of each individual was recorded and analysed with video software. The results showed that fish which were previously exposed to appetitive stimulus increased significantly the time spent on the stimulation side, while aversive stimulus led to a strong decrease in time spent on the stimulation side. Moreover, this study showed clearly that proactive fish were characterised by a stronger preference for the social stimulus and when placed in a putative aversive environment showed a lower physiological stress responses than reactive fish. In conclusion, this study showed for the first time in sea bass, that the CPP/CPA paradigm can be used to assess the valence (positive vs. negative) that fish attribute to different stimuli and that individual behavioural traits is predictive of how stimuli are perceived and thus of the magnitude of preference or avoidance behaviour.  相似文献   

5.
Quadros AF  Bugs PS  Araujo PB 《ZooKeys》2012,(176):155-170
Many arthropods, including terrestrial isopods, are capable of entering a state of tonic immobility upon a mechanical disturbance. Here we compare the responses to mechanical stimulation in three terrestrial isopods Balloniscus glaber, Balloniscus sellowii and Porcellio dilatatus. We applied three stimuli in a random order and recorded whether each individual was responsive (i.e. showed tonic immobility) or not and the duration of the response. In another trial we related the time needed to elicit tonic immobility and the duration of response of each individual. Balloniscus sellowii was the least responsive species and Porcellio dilatatus was the most, with 23% and 89% of the tested individuals, respectively, being responsive. Smaller Balloniscus sellowii were more responsive than larger individuals. Porcellio dilatatus responded more promptly than the Balloniscus spp. but it showed the shortest response. Neither sex, size nor the type of stimulus explained the variability found in the duration of tonic immobility. These results reveal a large variability in tonic immobility behavior, even between closely related species, which seems to reflect a species-specific response to predators with different foraging modes.  相似文献   

6.
Interspecific territoriality is frequently reported between closely related species; however, few studies have demonstrated interspecific territoriality between distantly related species living in sympatry. We conducted playback experiments to investigate territorial behaviour in male and female White‐bellied Wrens (Uropsila leucogastra) in response to simulated conspecific and heterospecific intruders during the breeding and non‐breeding seasons. We explored whether heterospecific songs of the Happy Wren (Pheugopedius felix), a distantly related species and ecological competitor, elicited antagonistic responses from focal White‐bellied Wrens, and whether such responses differed between the sexes. We also examined whether male and female responses to conspecific and heterospecific rivals varied with season. We found that male White‐bellied Wrens always responded to conspecific song, and responded significantly more to heterospecific song compared to a control stimulus (Tropical Parula, Setophaga pitiayumi). In contrast, although female White‐bellied Wrens responded strongly to conspecific song, their response to heterospecific song did not differ significantly from the control stimulus. The proportion of males that responded to heterospecific songs and the proportion of females that responded to conspecific songs varied seasonally, showing significantly lower responses during the breeding season. The intense responses of male White‐bellied Wrens to playback of heterospecific songs suggest that they recognise ecological competitors based on their vocal signals. Furthermore, the decrease in agonistic interactions during the breeding season is in line with the hypothesis that aggressive behaviour may be detrimental to reproductive and parental activity, and the hypothesis that heterospecific animals pose less of a threat during the breeding season.  相似文献   

7.
Male toads were tested behaviourally for their prey catching responses to worm-like stimuli before being prepared for visual unit and slow potential shift (SPS) recording from the optic tectum. The neuronal responses of toads to a prey-like visual stimulus reflected their motivational tendency prior to operations. One second of DC stimulation to the tectum was followed by an SPS of reversed polarity during which time a visual prey-like stimulus was presented. A negative SPS following positive DC stimulation was associated with enhanced neuronal responses to a visual stimulus. The positive SPS that followed negative stimulation was associated with a decline in neural responses below background when a visual stimulus was additionally given. The SPS was largely a result of DC stimulation that interacted with the motivational tendency to produce enhanced neuronal responses, while the potential was negative and vice versa.  相似文献   

8.
The present study was designed (1) to characterize the subliminal responses of dorsal horn neurons to stimulation of the sural nerve, and (2) to correlate the type of response to this stimulus with the responses to natural mechanical stimulation of the skin. To accomplish this, intracellular and extracellular recordings were carried out in L6 and L7 dorsal horn neurons in the cat. The excitatory responses of each cell to electrical stimulation of the sural nerve and to mechanical stimulation of the skin were noted. Of 35 dorsal horn cells recorded intracellularly, 11 responded with impulses to sural nerve stimulation, 9 responded with excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) but not impulses, and 15 had no excitatory responses to this stimulus. The type of response to sural nerve stimulation was strongly correlated with receptive field modality. Most cells receiving an input from high-threshold cutaneous mechanoreceptors responded with impulses or gave no excitatory response to sural nerve stimulation, whereas most cells that had only low-threshold mechanoreceptor input responded with EPSPs only or gave no response. In cells with only low-threshold (LT) mechanoreceptive input, response to sural nerve stimulation was highly correlated with receptive field locus. Those LT cells with no excitatory responses to sural nerve stimulation had receptive fields confined to the foot and/or toes, whereas those that gave EPSPs had more proximal receptive fields. The possible significance of these data with reference to changes observed after lesions, such as increased response to sural nerve stimulation, increased receptive field size, and somatotopic reorganization, is discussed.  相似文献   

9.
The present study was designed (1) to characterize the subliminal responses of dorsal horn neurons to stimulation of the sural nerve, and (2) to correlate the type of response to this stimulus with the responses to natural mechanical stimulation of the skin. To accomplish this, intracellular and extracellular recordings were carried out in L6 and L7 dorsal horn neurons in the cat. The excitatory responses of each cell to electrical stimulation of the sural nerve and to mechanical stimulation of the skin were noted.

Of 35 dorsal horn cells recorded intracellularly, 11 responded with impulses to sural nerve stimulation, 9 responded with excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) but not impulses, and 15 had no excitatory responses to this stimulus. The type of response to sural nerve stimulation was strongly correlated with receptive field modality. Most cells receiving an input from high-threshold cutaneous mechanoreceptors responded with impulses or gave no excitatory response to sural nerve stimulation, whereas most cells that had only low-threshold mechanoreceptor input responded with EPSPs only or gave no response. In cells with only low-threshold (LT) mechanoreceptive input, response to sural nerve stimulation was highly correlated with receptive field locus. Those LT cells with no excitatory responses to sural nerve stimulation had receptive fields confined to the foot and/or toes, whereas those that gave EPSPs had more proximal receptive fields. The possible significance of these data with reference to changes observed after lesions, such as increased response to sural nerve stimulation, increased receptive field size, and somatotopic reorganization, is discussed.  相似文献   

10.
We analysed how the assembling behaviour of Triatoma infestans is modulated by the convergence of chemical cues released by their faeces and the spectral quality of the light associated with refuges. Second-instar larvae were confronted with refuges associated with a visual stimulus (either blue, green or red lights having the same intensity, or darkness) and a chemical cue (presence or absence of faeces). In this context, faeces constitute a major attractant for bugs. In the absence of faeces, bugs always assembled in dark places. Green light was always rejected despite the presence of faeces, i.e. the assembling behaviour was controlled by a photonegative reaction to this light. In the presence of red light, orientation towards the chemical cue dominated over the bugs' photonegative reaction to this light. Such a light was avoided in the absence of faeces but not in their presence. Also, negative phototaxis to blue light could be counteracted by the presence of the chemical cue. Thus, a concrete interaction between visual and olfactory cues occurred in these experiments: the bugs' response changed depending on the specific combination of spectral light and faeces. Finally, bugs responded differentially to blue, red and green lights of the same intensity. They assembled preferentially on red, followed by blue and then by green. Thus, discrimination between lights of different spectral quality is possible, probably through an achromatic mechanism.  相似文献   

11.
Predator-induced respiratory responses of juvenile vendace, whitefish, perch and roach were studied in an intermittent-flow respirometer by creating visual contact between test fishes and predators (juvenile northern pike). Vendace and whitefish always responded to the presence of pike with increased oxygen consumption. The response of perch was the opposite: oxygen consumption decreased to near the standard respiratory level. Roach responded more variably: some individuals increased their oxygen uptake while others decreased it. Changes in the rate of respiration during exposure to predation are supposed to be caused by the differences in locomotory activity due to induced antipredator behaviour. According to their responses, these species could be grouped into escapers (coregonids), hiders (perch) or indeterminates (roach). The species-specific differences in the antipredator behaviour originate in morphological and physiological characteristics which together determine the effectiveness of a particular antipredator style (i.e. either hiding or escaping).  相似文献   

12.
Interindividual variability in response to environmental stimuliis believed to have a major impact on collective behaviors insocial insects. The present study presents a detailed investigationof the variability in individual fanning behavior underlyingthe collective control of nest climate in bumblebee (Bombusterrestris) colonies. Four colonies were repeatedly exposedto increasing temperature and CO2 levels. The response thresholdof each worker (defined as the mean stimulus intensity at whicha worker responded by fanning) was determined. Temperature responsethresholds of 118 workers and CO2 response thresholds of 88workers were analyzed. Workers differed in their response thresholds.Some consistently responded to low stimulus intensities, othersconsistently responded to high stimulus intensities. No consistentcorrelation between temperature and CO2 thresholds was foundwithin individuals. Response thresholds of fanning bees decreasedover successive trials, providing empirical support for theidea of specialization through individual threshold reinforcement.In addition to variability in individual response thresholds,workers of a colony differed in two other parameters of responsiveness:response probability (the probability of responding to a stimulusonce it exceeded an individual's response threshold) and responseduration (the persistency with which fanning was performed oncean individual responded). The results of the present study suggestthat response threshold, response probability and response durationare important independent parameters of individual responsivenessin the collective control of nest climate in bumblebee colonies.  相似文献   

13.
We tested whether goldfish, Carassius auratus, discriminate hydrodynamic stimuli caused by moving objects. Blindfolded goldfish responded to a passing object with changes in inter-gill-movement intervals. To learn whether goldfish can discriminate water motions caused by different moving objects we habituated them to a certain object stimulus. If the stimulus was altered, e.g., by altering speed, direction of motion, or size or shape of the object, fish again showed a temporary suspension of breathing when the object passed by. If animals failed to respond to an altered stimulus, we paired this stimulus with a weak electric shock during training. Goldfish discriminated object motion direction. In addition, in two choice experiments goldfish discriminated water motions caused by objects which moved with different speeds (e.g., 5 cm s(-1) versus 6 cm s(-1)), or by objects which differed in size (e.g., 1 cm x 1 cm versus 1.4 cm x 1.4 cm cross section), or shape (e.g., a round versus a triangular object). If object size and/or shape was varied quasi-randomly such that the faster moving object not always caused the greatest water velocities, fish still discriminated object speed.  相似文献   

14.
The responses of mechanoreceptor neurons in the antennal chordotonal organ have been examined in cockroaches by intracellular recording methods. The chordotonal organ was mechanically stimulated by sinusoidal movement of the flagellum. Stimulus frequencies were varied between 0.5 and 150 Hz. Receptor neurons responded with spike discharges to mechanical stimulation, and were classed into two groups from plots of their average spike frequencies against stimulus frequency. Neurons in one group responded to stimulation over a wide frequency range (from 0.5 to 150 Hz), whereas those in a second group were tuned to higher frequency stimuli. The peak stimulus frequency at which receptor neurons showed maximum responses differed from cell to cell. Some had a peak response at a stimulus frequency given in the present study (from 0.5 to 150 Hz), whereas others were assumed to have peak responses beyond the highest stimulus frequency examined. The timing for the initiation of spikes or of a burst of spikes plotted against each stimulus cycle revealed that spike generation was phase-locked in most cells. Some cells showed phase-independent discharges to stimulation at lower frequency, but increasing stimulus frequencies spike initiation began to assemble at a given phase of the stimulus cycle. The response patterns observed are discussed in relation to the primary process of mechanoreception of the chordotonal organ.  相似文献   

15.
Abstract In Lolium multiflorum nodal segments, bending responses both to geostimulation and unilateral indole-3-acetic acid (IAA) application exhibited much variability in their lag times and speeds of early bending. Despite this variability, mean response curves to gravity and auxin stimulus were markedly similar with each having a phase of immediate, negative bending followed by phases of slow, positive bending and eventually more rapid, positive bending within 40 min of initial treatment. Comparison of lag times for response to geostimulation and unilateral IAA application, whether derived from the mean of individual replicates, or from mean curve data, showed that at least 4 min is available in this geotropic system for establishment of asymmetric auxin levels that could lead to differential growth. The hypothesis that variability in georesponse in Lolium nodal segments is linked to variable sensitivity of geosensitive tissue to auxin was tested using matching longitudinally-halved nodal segments and evidence was obtained in support of the hypothesis from lag time but not from early bending speed data. The implications of the findings for an involvement of endogenous IAA in shoot geotropism together with the necessity to understand better the complex behaviour of bending response in individual replicates are discussed.  相似文献   

16.
The interactions between electrical polarizations of the olfactory epithelium and odour stimulations were investigated at the level of the extracellular spike activity of the receptor cells in the frog. 1. In most cases, surface positive polarizations enhanced the excitatory olfactory responses, negative polarizations suppressed these responses; both interactive effects were graded. 2. The response of receptor cells to electrical polarization was markedly reduced or suppressed for several seconds following olfactory stimulation. This effect and the time course of the recovery period depended on the nature and the concentration of the olfactory stimulus. 3. The decrease in electrical excitability seemed to be independent of whether the recorded neuron had responded or not to the prior olfactory stimulation. 4. It is suggested that the olfactory stimulation caused the total constant current to change its distribution in the different cell pathways. Changes in conductance induced by olfactory stimuli could implicate the supporting cells. 5. The experimental findings are discussed with reference to a model of receptor cell function that assumes a deep, axo-somatic localization of the action potential trigger-zone.  相似文献   

17.
18.
Spike responses of area 4 neurons in the projection area of the contralateral forelimb to acoustic stimulation (1 sec), which became the conditioned stimulus after training, and to dropping of the platform beneath the test limb, which served as reinforcing stimulus, were studied in trained and untrained cats. Responses only of those neurons which were activated during a passive movement caused by dropping of the platform were studied. In trained animals the number of these neurons which responded to the conditioned stimulus if a reflex occurred was 100%, and in the absence of conditioned-reflex movements to the conditioned stimulus it was 70%, much greater than the number of neurons responding to the same acoustic stimulus in untrained animals (45%). On peristimulus histograms of responses of the test neuron population in untrained and trained animals to acoustic stimulation (in the absence of movements) only the initial spike response with a latent period of under 50 msec and a duration of up to 100 msec could be clearly distinguished. In the presence of reflex movement multicomponent spike responses were observed: an initial spike response and early and late after-responses linked with performance of conditioned-reflex limb flexion. Early after-responses 100–200 msec in duration, appearing after a latent period of 100–150 msec, were linked to the time of application of the conditioned stimulus, whereas the appearance and duration of late after-responses were determined by the time of onset of conditioned-reflex movement. The magnitude of the neuronal response to reinforcement in trained animals does not depend on the appearance of the conditioned movement.A. A. Bogomolets Institute of Physiology, Academy of Sciences of the Ukrainian SSR, Kiev. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 17, No. 1, pp. 93–102, January–February, 1985.  相似文献   

19.
Two techniques have been used to study the quantitative relationship between stimulus intensity and oriented phototactic response (topotaxis) in Chlamydomonas. The net response of a cell population was monitored photometrically and was recorded continuously against time. The responses of individual cells were observed through a microscope and their swimming tracks were recorded on film. The net response of the population is positive at low stimulus intensity and negative at high intensity. The direction of response can be reversed within two seconds by raising or lowering the intensity. The intensity-response curve for phototaxis is similar to the dose-response curve for phototropism. The net response has no distinct threshold; it increases linearly with log intensity; then it decreases and finally becomes negative. The individual-cell studies reveal that the intensity-dependent increase in net topotactic response is due primarily to an increase in the number of cells responding and in the directness of their swimming path. As stimulus intensity is raised, the swimming path becomes increasingly well-aligned with the stimulus beam, whether net response is positive throughout the intensity range tested, negative throughout that range, or changing from positive to negative. Changes in swimming rate do not contribute significantly to the intensity-dependent changes in net response. Swimming rate shows virtually no change throughout the intensity range of positive topotaxis and shows only a small increase in the negative range. However, a transient decrease in swimming rate (stop response) is often observed at the onset of stimulation. The implications of these results for the orientation mechanism are discussed.  相似文献   

20.
Anurans emit distress calls when attacked by predators as a defensive mechanism. As distress calls may trigger antipredator behaviour even in individuals that are not under attack, we tested whether this defensive behaviour induced behavioural changes in neighbouring conspecifics. We compared the behavioural responses of two species of Neotropical hylid frogs (genus Boana) to conspecific distress calls and white noise. Individuals of both species interrupted their vocal activity and decreased call rate after hearing the distress call. Natural variation on signal intensity calibrated among the nearest neighbours did not influence the response and we did not observe negative phonotaxis after any acoustic stimulus. Despite the fact that many predators are acoustically oriented, we could not determine if such response (reduced call rate) was induced by risk assessment or by the masking effect on advertisement calls. Boana faber responded similarly to white noise and distress calls, while B. bischoffi responded more intensely to distress calls. Duration of silence after playbacks in B. faber was longer than B. bischoffi. We suggest that, if the signals are interpreted as a risk cue by neighbouring conspecifics, each species may be preyed upon by different predators, as they may have led to distinct defensive strategies and different responses to distress calls. If risk assessment information is included in distress calls, it triggers behavioural responses only in the nearest neighbours, as we did not observe responses on the vocal activity of the interspecific chorus. Our results add relevant data about acoustic communication and interpretations by anurans, highlighting the importance of considering cues within common and widespread signals.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号