首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The importance of visual landmarks during homing in pigeons (Columba livia) remains a contentious issue. Three experiments which explore the role of visual landmarks at release sites are reported here. The effects of releasing homing pigeons after a 5-minute period in either a clear or an opaque sided release box were investigated. In the clear sided box pigeons were able to observe local surroundings at a release site, but this view was obstructed in the opaque sided box. In experiment 1 pigeons were released from familiar locations close to home (between 2 and 5.6 km): being unable to view landmarks prior to release significantly slowed homing speeds. In experiment 2 pigeons were released at familiar locations further from home (between 8.4 and 10 km): being unable to view landmarks prior to release did not significantly affect homing speeds. In experiment 3 pigeons were trained to home from distant release sites but were tested at closer, unfamiliar sites located on the likely homing routes used by the pigeons in training. No significant difference in homing speeds were observed when pigeons were released from either the clear or opaque sided box. The significance of these results for understanding the role of visual landmarks within a pigeon's familiar area is discussed.  相似文献   

2.
During the incubation period, white-chinned petrels, Procellaria aequinoctialis, repeatedly make foraging trips of several thousand kilometres in the open ocean from their small isolated breeding island. To test whether these birds rely on geomagnetic information to home, we displaced five individuals and released them in the open sea 300-360 km from home. They were prevented both from using route-based information during the passive displacement from the breeding island to the release site, and from using the geomagnetic field at the release site and on their way home (by carrying a mobile magnet on the head). They were equipped with satellite transmitters to record their homing movements. The five petrels homed efficiently (in 17-32 h) along paths that were on average slightly less than twice the homing distance. This showed that, at least when within a few hundred kilometres of home, white-chinned petrels are able to home successfully by relying only on nongeomagnetic site-dependent information. Copyright 2003 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.   相似文献   

3.
In social insects, the activity rhythm of foragers and their action range determinate the activity of the colony. In vespids, which are mostly predators, the foraging range of workers determines their maximum predation pressure round the nest. One of these species, Vespa velutina, a recently invasive species introduced into Europe, exerts a strong predation on honeybees at the hive. Therefore, the definition of its activity rhythm and spatial range of predation is of primary importance. Using radio frequency identification tags (RFID), two experiments were carried out to (a) determine their return ability (called homing) in releasing 318 individuals at different distance from their colony and (b) monitor their foraging activity rhythm and the duration of their flights based on 71 individuals followed 24 hr/24 during 2 months. The homing ability of V. velutina was evaluated to be up to 5,000 m and was not affected by the cardinal orientation of release point. The lag time to return to the nest increased with the distance of release. Most of the flight activity took place between 07:00 a.m. and 08:00 p.m., hornets doing principally short flights of less than an hour. Foraging range was thus estimated ca. 1,000 m around the nest. This study of V. velutina assisted by RFID tags provides for the first time a baseline for its potential foraging distance that increase our knowledge of this species to (a) refine more accurately models for risk assessment and (b) define security perimeter for early detection of predation on invasion front.  相似文献   

4.
Pahl M  Zhu H  Tautz J  Zhang S 《PloS one》2011,6(5):e19669
Honeybee foragers frequently fly several kilometres to and from vital resources, and communicate those locations to their nest mates by a symbolic dance language. Research has shown that they achieve this feat by memorizing landmarks and the skyline panorama, using the sun and polarized skylight as compasses and by integrating their outbound flight paths. In order to investigate the capacity of the honeybees' homing abilities, we artificially displaced foragers to novel release spots at various distances up to 13 km in the four cardinal directions. Returning bees were individually registered by a radio frequency identification (RFID) system at the hive entrance. We found that homing rate, homing speed and the maximum homing distance depend on the release direction. Bees released in the east were more likely to find their way back home, and returned faster than bees released in any other direction, due to the familiarity of global landmarks seen from the hive. Our findings suggest that such large scale homing is facilitated by global landmarks acting as beacons, and possibly the entire skyline panorama.  相似文献   

5.
This is the first record of Xenos moutoni, a specialized parasite of hornets (Vespa spp.), in Korea. Among six Vespa species collected with bait traps in Andong, central South Korea, approximately 10% of female Vespa analis (gynes plus workers) were parasitized, whereas the other five species (V. crabro, V. mandarinia, V. simillima, V. ducalis, and V. dybowskii) were not. Almost all parasitized hornets had a single female parasite in the gaster.  相似文献   

6.
European badgers (Meles meles) live in groups. Although they can distinguish between a member of their own group, a member of a neighbouring group and a stranger, their ability to understand that neighbouring individuals belong to different groups inhabiting different places, and possibly to build up some representation of the spatial organisation of neighbouring groups remains to be shown. In this study, we conducted a pilot homing experiment to test such ability. Radio-collared badgers were displaced to the home ranges of neighbouring groups and their homing performances were compared to those of badgers displaced either to the periphery of their own group's home range or beyond the neighbouring home ranges. When released in their own home range, badgers homed rapidly and efficiently, whereas when released beyond the neighbouring groups' home ranges (whatever the distance) they did not home. In contrast, badgers released in the home range of a neighbouring group performed some random search there, before returning to their setts quite efficiently. These results suggest that badgers may consider their neighbours as members of different groups inhabiting different places close to their own home range, but their ability to build up some spatial representation of neighbourhood relationships could not be demonstrated.  相似文献   

7.
We studied the ability of Akodon azarae (Rodentia, Muridae) to return to their preferred habitat, when released at a perpendicular distance (25, 50 or 75 m) from the edge towards the cropfield, and a parallel distance (100 m) away from the site of first capture within the edge habitat. Return success was estimated as the proportion of animals recovered in edges. The recapture rate between the field and the border was significantly higher than the recapture rate estimated according to successive captures in the border. Successful returns did not decrease significantly with increasing release distance, but animals released at 50 m from edges were less successful in returning to borders than the other release-distance groups. Although the median time taken to the first recapture in edges did not differ among the release-distance groups, rodents released at 25 m and 50 m returned to edges faster than those released at 75 m. A. azarae showed both a successful return to the edge and a trend to return to the home range area. We conclude that A. azarae can return to edges from cropfields at distances that are larger than those they usually travel, allowing the use of fields when they present good conditions for reproduction and survival. Successful return is probably the result of direct movements rather than random wandering.  相似文献   

8.
The aim of this study was to determine if juvenile northern elephant seals, Mirounga angustirostris , translocated from their rookery would return to it quickly and reliably. During the spring and fall of 1990 and 1991 we captured 75 seals at Año Nuevo State Reserve, CA, U. S. A. and translocated them to release sites up to 100 km away. Eighty-eight percent of the seals returned to the capture site within 4.7 ± 4.3 d. Homing rate increased with age, but even the youngest seals (8–10 mo) homed at a 73% rate. Homing rate did not vary significantly with sex, season, or year. Data from diving instruments suggested that the seals often followed direct routes home, arrived on the rookery significantly more often at night than during the day, and when released together, returned separately. Mean homing speed of 18 seals with complete diving records was 39 km/d (range 3–70 km/d). Instruments on seals had no detectable effect on homing rate or homing speed. The translocation paradigm provides a powerful tool for conducting intensive shortterm studies on free-ranging seals.  相似文献   

9.

After being displaced, juvenile reef fishes are able to return home over large distances. This strong homing behaviour is extraordinary and may allow insights into the longer-term spatial ecology of fish communities. For example, it appears intuitive that strong homing behaviour should be indicative of long-term site fidelity. However, this connection has rarely been tested. We quantified the site fidelity of juvenile fishes of four species after returning home following displacement. Two species, parrotfishes and Pomacentrus moluccensis, showed significantly reduced site fidelity after returning home. On average, they disappeared from their home sites almost 3 d earlier than expected. Mortality or competitive exclusion does not seem to be the main reasons for their disappearance. Rather, we suggest an increased propensity to relocate after encountering alternative reef locations while homing. It appears that some juvenile fishes may have a higher innate spatial flexibility than their strict homing drive suggests.

  相似文献   

10.
Precise homing of 24 brook charr Salvelinus fontinalis (157–215 mm LF), displaced from their site of capture in a natural stream, was rare both in fish displaced upstream and those displaced downstream. Most fish settled in preferred habitats (pool or glide) away from their home site and showed restricted movement up to the pre-spawning period. Partial return responses, however, were stronger in fish displaced downstream than in fish displaced upstream. Most fish displaced downstream showed directional upstream movement soon after displacement, whereas fish displaced upstream did not show directional upstream movement, suggesting that it is primarily olfactory cues from upstream locations, and not positive rheotaxis, which mediate the return response. Homing success was unrelated to distance of displacement, body size, or population density in the home site, but daily mobility increased with body size for fish displaced downstream. There was no evidence of switching between specific areas between night and day, nor of differences between daytime and night-time mobility, suggesting that diel movement cycles did not bias the evaluation of homing success. Site attachment in stream-dwelling brook charr may be characterized as persistent (undisturbed fish remain stationary over long periods), but readily disrupted by involuntary displacement.  相似文献   

11.
Site fidelity refers to the restriction of dispersal distance of an animal and its tendency to return to a stationary site. To our knowledge, the homing ability of freshwater turtles and their fidelity is reportedly very low in Asia. We examined mark–recapture data spanning a 4‐year period in Diaoluoshan National Nature Reserve, Hainan Province, China, to investigate the site fidelity and homing behavior of big‐headed turtles Platysternon megacephalum. A total of 11 big‐headed turtles were captured, and all individuals were used in this mark–recapture study. The site fidelity results showed that the adult big‐headed turtles (n = 4) had a 71.43% recapture rate in the original site after their release at the same site, whereas the juveniles (n = 1) showed lower recapture rates (0%). Moreover, the homing behavior results showed that the adults (n = 5) had an 83.33% homing rate after displacement. Adult big‐headed turtles were able to return to their initial capture sites (home) from 150 to 2,400 m away and precisely to their home sites from either upstream or downstream of their capture sites or even from other streams. However, none of the juveniles (n = 4) returned home, despite only being displaced 25–150 m away. These results indicated that the adult big‐headed turtles showed high fidelity to their home site and strong homing ability. In contrast, the juvenile turtles may show an opposite trend but further research is needed.  相似文献   

12.
The aim of the present investigation was two-fold: a) to observe the homing of the Oriental hornet, Vespa orientalis (Hymenoptera, Vespinae) from different distances; and b) to study the photothermoelectric activity of hornet cuticle obtained from the subjects of goal (a) and kept frozen for a number of days prior to its testing. In both the above mentioned phases of the investigation, an attempt was made to assess how the covering of the hornets' cuticle with Ultra Violet B (UVB) blockers affects their activity as compared to the control. Flying hornets were observed to return to the nest from distances of up to 7 km, once they had learned the way back. However, covering of the cuticle with UVB blockers increases the percentage of 'non-returners' to nearly 100%. Covering the cuticle completely or partly with a number of UVB blockers (except for Sisley) proves lethal for the hornets within 24 hours. A statistical model on homing is proposed of the effect of range, of covering with UVB blockers and covering ocelli with Tippex. In the wing of the hornet there is increase in the electric current with rise in the temperature and decrease in the current upon drop of the temperature, but light has no effect on this alar (wing) current. Contrariwise, the body cuticle of the hornet responds to both temperature and illumination in terms of its electric current. Coating of the cuticle with UVB blockers causes in the wing (under all conditions of illumination) and in the cuticle (only in the dark) a moderation in the amplitude of the photothermoelectric current.  相似文献   

13.
黄腹角雉活动区的无线电遥测研究   总被引:12,自引:2,他引:10  
孙悦华  郑光美 《动物学报》1992,38(4):385-392
1987年11月至1988年6月,在浙江省乌岩岭自然保护区,对9只黄腹角雉进行了无线电遥测研究。结果表明:黄腹角雉月活动区大小为0.015—0.30km~2,夏季的活动区明显小于冬春季,这种变化与取食有密切关系;黄腹角雉不存在季节性垂直迁移现象;即使繁殖期来临,雄性黄腹角雉活动区之间仍存在较大重叠,说明并非所有雄鸟在繁殖期来临后都占区;黄腹角雉对水源丰富的区域更为喜爱。  相似文献   

14.
Adult male Leucorchestris arenicola can walk round-trips of several tens of meters in search of females. Most excursions end with the spiders returning to their burrow. For small animals homing over distances of several meters is theoretically impossible without the aid of external cues. It was investigated, whether the spiders use local cues or they rely solely on global cues. Individually marked male spiders were captured during their excursions and displaced several meters inside an opaque box. Ten out of twelve displaced spiders returned to their burrows. This shows that the male L. arenicola are using local cues during their homing, as the comparatively small displacement distances could not be detected by means of global, e.g. celestial cues. In order to test whether the spiders could be using olfactory guidance, the burrows were displaced by 2 m while the spiders were out on their journeys. In 12 out of 15 experiments, the spiders did not find their burrows. These results show that the burrows do not function as olfactory beacons for the homing spiders.  相似文献   

15.
Displacement of juvenile Atlantic salmon Salmo salar within an artificial stream was either spontaneous (fish left areas of shallow water in response to experimental reduction in water level) or imposed (fish were removed by the experimenter from areas of shallow water and placed at a distance from their home site). Prior to displacement, the fish showed a high degree of site fidelity in terms of preferential use of specific areas within the stream, but the extent to which this persisted once they had left/been removed from their preferred sites was variable. Direction of displacement was not a critical factor, but homing was significantly less likely to occur following spontaneous as opposed to imposed displacement. In the case of imposed displacement, fish that were more strongly site attached prior to displacement were more likely to return to their home site after this manipulation.  相似文献   

16.
On the Kenyan coast, Thalamita crenata confines itself to a defined system of crevices and forages, swimming in a few cm of water, within a radius of about 5 m from its shelter. A field study was designed to analyse this crab's ability to find its shelter after being moved away from it. Crabs were displaced, being kept under water, with full vision of the sky and landscape and released 5 m away from their refuges, at a maximum depth of 50 cm. They were able to return to their shelters within 1 h and followed initial directions which were well orientated towards home. T. crenata was still well orientated and successful in returning home during nocturnal displacements and even after trials in which the landscape was altered. Only blind crabs were neither initially orientated towards home nor successful in returning within two tidal cycles of their release. The hypothesis that this swimming crab could use orientating information obtained during the outward displacement was then tested. Specimens were dislocated following a non-linear outward path, without vision of the surrounding landscape; other crabs were carried to a false release point and then carried in a closed container to the actual release point. Finally, three kinds of detour experiments were performed. In all these trials the directions chosen by the crabs were still clustered around the home direction and homing success was again high. These results exclude homing mechanisms based on random search strategies or on egocentric mechanisms, such as path integration. The most probable hypothesis is that T. crenata organizes some visual cues in a map-like arrangement and, detecting these cues from any release point within its home range, uses this map to return home.  相似文献   

17.
Homing behaviors are seen across the animal kingdom, with the magnitude and scale of these movements varying to a great degree. Invertebrates, such as crayfish, prove to be excellent models for comparative research, with relatively simple nervous systems that have been extensively studied combined with a rich behavioral repertoire that has been well documented. Here we explore whether two crayfish species (a primary burrower and a tertiary burrower) can home to artificial burrows and if so, whether their homing abilities differ. The two species of crayfish, Orconectes rusticus and Fallicambarus fodiens, were examined for their ability to home to artificial burrows in a laboratory setting. Animals were placed in a homing arena, motivated to search by the presence of food, and subsequently return to the burrows. The movements of each individual crayfish were digitized and analyzed for changes in walking speed, velocity and heading angle relative to burrows, total distance travelled and path turning angles. Crayfish of both species successfully returned home, although F. fodiens had a higher homing success rate than O. rusticus. Detailed kinematic analysis showed that walking speeds almost doubled during homing to burrows and heading angles were significantly closer to zero than on the outward journeys for both species. Path turning angles were significantly lower for O. rusticus than for F. fodiens. These findings indicate that these species of crayfish can successfully home without the visual landmark of burrow chimneys and that the primary burrower (F. fodiens) appears to be more successful with straighter return paths. The differences in these two species to home and the underlying mechanisms of homing may be due to the differences in burrow use with primary burrowers showing more efficient homing than tertiary burrowers.  相似文献   

18.
ABSTRACT. Leg movements of Camponotus americanus workers during straight swimming and turning are described herein. Thrust is generated through the different speeds and drag control between power v. return strokes in the forelegs. During the power stroke, femur, tibia and tarsus are straightened and thereby increase resistance; they bend backward during the return stroke. These thrusting legs move in a vertical plane which is similar to their position during walking. The backward stretching mesothoracic and metathoracic legs act, in conjunction with the gaster, as a rudder. Swimming in ants can be derived from walking; the major transformation being a suppression of the rhythmic movements of the middle and hind legs.  相似文献   

19.
蚂蚁捷径返巢及其朝向机制的研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
徐智敏  沈钧贤 《昆虫学报》2000,43(3):242-247
定量研究了铺道蚁Tetramorium caespitum的载物返巢行为。实验证明这种蚂蚁在获得食物后,并不重复搜寻食物期间所走过的迂回路线,而是沿着近似直线的新路径迅速回巢。太阳及天空偏振光模式能用作它们返巢的提示,非偏振光源也能指引蚂蚁成功地捷径返巢。这些结果表明有高级社会性结构的铺道蚁通过脑以光导航。  相似文献   

20.
The effect of group size on behavioral parameters of the Oriental hornet,Vespa orientalis, was assessed experimentally under laboratory conditions. Hornet groups of various sizes (ranging from 1 to 100 individuals per group) comprised of young individuals (0–24 hr of age) devoid of a queen were placed in artificial breeding boxes (ABBs). The following three quantitative parameters were evaluated: the amount and rate of building as a function of the number of hornets in the group, the rate of oviposition as, related to group size and the longevity of hornets as a function of their group size. The probability for the occurrence of these events was similarly considered and additional behavioral parameters were only assessed qualitatively. Results of this investigation revealed a relation between the three mentioned quantitative behavioral parameters and the number of hornets per group. The number of hornets per group was positively related to the extent of building, the number of cells built by a group is , but negatively related to the rate of building. As for the delay of building, a non-monotone relation was found. The relation between number of hornets per group and the oviposition delay was found to be non-monotone; the number of hornets per group and their longevity were found to be inversely related. Discrepanices were recorded on the very small (1–2 individuals) or very large (100 individuals) hornet groups.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号