首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Summary A low molecular weight heat shock protein which localizes to chloroplasts has been identified in several plant species. This protein belongs to a eukaryotic superfamily of small HSPs, all of which contain a conserved carboxyl-terminal domain. To investigate further the structure of this HSP, we isolated and sequenced cDNA clones for the chloroplast LMW HSPs from Petunia hybrida and Arabidopsis thaliana. The cloning of chloroplast HSPs from these two species enabled us to compare the amino acid sequences of this protein from plant species (petunia, Arabidopsis, pea, soybean and maize) that represent evolutionarily divergent taxonomic subclasses. Three conserved regions were identified, which are designated as regions I, II and III. Regions I and II are also shared by cytoplasmic LMW HSPs and therefore are likely to have functional roles common to all eukaryotic LMW HSPs. In contrast, consensus region III is not found in other LMW HSPs. Secondary structure analysis predicts that this region forms an amphipathic -helix with high conservation of methionine residues on the hydrophobic face and 100% conservation of residues on the hydrophilic face. This structure is similar to three helices, termed methionine bristles, which are found in a methionine-rich domain of a 54 kDa protein component of signal recognition particle (SRP54). The conservation of regions I and II among LMW cytoplasmic and chloroplast HSPs suggests that these HSPs perform related functions in different cellular compartments. However, identification of the methionine bristle domain suggests that chloroplast HSPs also have unique functions or substrates within the special environment of the chloroplast or other plastids.Abbreviations HS heat shock - HSP heat shock protein - LMW low molecular weight  相似文献   

2.
Heterotrimeric G-proteins are involved in a variety of cellular responses, but relatively little is known about their function and biochemistry in plants. Antibodies raised against the tobacco heterotrimeric G-protein -subunit (G) were used to analyse its distribution in tobacco leaves. In young tissue the protein level was relatively high, while it declined substantially during later stages of leaf development. Cell fractionation revealed that G is tightly associated with plasma membrane, but can also be detected in purified nuclei.  相似文献   

3.
A model of vertical signal flow across a layered cortical structure is presented and analyzed. Neurons communicate through spikes, which evoke an excitatory or inhibitory postsynaptic potential (spike response model). The layers incorporate two anatomical features - dendritic and axonal arborization patterns and distance-dependent time delays. The vertical signal flow through the network is discussed for various stimulus conditions using two different, but typical, axonal arborization patterns. We find stationary as well as oscillatory response, but the oscillatory response may be restricted to a single layer. Confronted with conflicting stimuli the network separates the patterns through phase-shifted oscillations. We also discuss two hypothetical animals, to be called cat and mouse. These have different axonal arborizations, which give rise to a different oscillatory response (if any) of the various layers.  相似文献   

4.
Complex cells in the cat's visual cortex show nonlinearities in processing of image luminance and movement. To study mechanisms, initally we have represented the chain of neurons from retina to cortex as a black-box model. Independent information about the visual system has helped us cast this Wiener-kernel model into a dynamic-linear/static-nonlinear/dynamiclinear (LNL) cascade. We then use system identification techniques to define the nature of these transformations directly from responses of the neuron to a single presentation of a stimulus composed of a sequence of white-noise-modulated luminance values. The two dynamic linear filters are mainly low-pass, and the static nonlinearity is mainly of even polynomial degree. This approximate squaring function may be effected in the animal by soft-thresholding each of the linear ON- and OFF-channel signals and then summing them, which account for ON-OFF responses and for the squaring operation needed for computation of motion energy, both observed in these neurons.  相似文献   

5.
Continuous movement of striped patterns was presented on either side of a tethered fruitfly, Drosophila melanogaster, in order to simulate the displacement of stationary landmarks within the visual field of the freely moving fly. The horizontal components of the stimulus elicit, predominantly, yaw-torque responses during flight, or turning responses on the ground, which counteract involuntary deviations from a streight course in the corresponding mode of locomotion. The vertical components elicit, predominantly, covariant responses of lift and thrust which enable the fly to maintain a given level of flight. Monocular stimulation is sufficient to produce antagonistic responses, if the direction of the stimulus is reversed. The following constituents of the responses were derived mainly from properties of wing beat and body posture on photographs of fixed flight under visual stimulation. Wing stroke modulation (W. S. M.): The difference, and the sum, of the stroke amplitudes on either side are independently controlled by horizontal and vertical movement components, respectively. The maximum range of modulation per wing (12.3°) is equivalent to a 63% change in thrust on the corresponding side. Leg stroke modulation (L.S.M.): In the walking fly each pair of legs is under control of visual stimulation. The details of leg articulation are still unknown. Abdominal deflection (A.D.): An actively induced posture effect. Facilitates steering during free flight at increased air speed. Hind leg deflection (H.L.D.): Same as before. On most of the photographs the hind legs were deflected simultaneously and in the same direction as the abdomen. Hitch inhibition (H.I.): The term hitch denotes a transient reduction of stroke amplitude which seems to occur spontaneously and independently on either side of the fly. The hitch angle (12.2±3.8° S.D.) is most probably invariant to visual stimulation. Hitches are comparatively frequent in the absence of pattern movement. Their inhibition under visual stimulation is equivalent to an increase of the average thrust of the corresponding wing. The different constituents contribute to the optomotor responses according to the following tentative scheme (Fig. 7). The torque response is essentially due to the effects of W.S.M., A.D., H.L.D. and H.I., and the turning response to L.S.M. and possibly H.L.D., if the landmarks drift from anterior to posterior. So far, H.I. seems to be the only source of the torque response, and L.S.M. the only source of the turning response, if the landmarks drift in the opposite direction. The lift/thrust response results essentially from the effects of W.S.M. and H.I., no matter whether the landmarks drift from inferior to superior or in the opposite direction. The results obtained so far suggest that the optomotor control of course and altitude in Drosophila requires at least eight independent input channels or equivalent means for the separation of the descending signals from the visual centres. Further extension and refinement of the wiring scheme is required in order to improve the identification of the sensory inputs of the motor system and the classification of optomotor defective mutants.  相似文献   

6.
Mechanism of food detection in Rana temporalis tadpoles was studied using a rectangular choice tank with end compartments (stimulus zones) providing exclusively visual and/or chemical food cues. Boiled spinach served as the food. The test tadpoles were starved for 24 h before use. They were released from the center of the choice tank (n=24) after 5 min of acclimation to test for end bias and food-detecting mechanism. The number of tadpoles in the two stimulus zones was recorded at 5-min intervals from 10 to 30 min. In the end-bias tests (without food cues) tadpole distribution was comparable at all times in the two compartments of the choice tank, exhibiting no end bias. In tests with the visual food cues provided in one of the stimulus zones, the tadpole distribution was also random. On the other hand, in experiments involving chemical cues emanating from food the tadpoles preferentially associated with the food source in significantly greater numbers compared to the zone lacking food or providing only a visual cue. The experiments with individual test tadpoles also revealed that they detect food based on chemical cues and ruled out copycat behavior. These findings on R. temporalis tadpoles reveal that chemical senses predominate over the visual senses in detection of food and foraging.  相似文献   

7.
We present a spike-triggered averaging method capable of mapping the visual receptive fields of several neurons simultaneously. The stimulation is general and the mapping proceeds automatically without the need to match the stimulation to the cells' preference for position, orientation, direction, etc. The maps are spatiotemporal; receptive field (RF) structures are quantitatively determined in three dimensions: the two dimensions of visuotopic space, and time. The method presented is one of a family of reverse correlation or spike-triggered averaging techniques (DeBoer and Kuyper 1968) capable of revealing linear aspects of stimulus-response coupling. The formal relationship of these methods to stimulus-response crosscorrelation is shown. The analysis is extended to provide some second-order axis-of-motion information (direction marks). The stimulus is a constantly illuminated, randomly jumping bright or dark spot, not an elongated bar. Spot diameters between one-third to 1 × RF width are effective. The method ascertains for each recorded action potential or spike the prior visual field position of the spot. The average or most probable spot positions define the receptive field spatially. Repeating the process for a succession of times prior to observed spikes defines the field temporally, presented here as a succession of spatial maps. We term this portrayal a receptive field cinematogram, RFc or ciné. The RFc reveals and economically portrays the spread of excitability and suppression across the receptive field, culminating in the generation of a spike. RFcs for LGN neurons and for simple cells recorded in cat cortical areas 17 and 18 are presented and interpreted in terms of classic ON/OFF regions. The availability of temporal information permits the separation of an excitatory exit response, generated when a moving bright spot leaves an OFF region, from an excitatory entrance response occurring when a bright spot enters an ON region, because these responses occur at different times (exit responses earlier). Spike emission remains coupled to (cross-correlated with) stimulus events over time periods as long as 96 ms, implying that some stimulus drive or afferent visual input is delayed by as much as 96 ms more than other input. This is a striking instance of temporal dispersion in the visual system. In some cells, said to be spatiotemporally inseparable, the delay (latency) varies systematically across the visual field; i.e., the place for optimal stimulation varies with the time prior to spike emission. In these cells, the RFc shows receptive field structures which move across the visual field over trajectories equal to approximately twice the total conventional RF width. Exit and entrance responses, on the other hand, arise in a simple way from separated ON and OFF RF subregions. ON/ OFF mechanisms thus appear unrelated to spatiotemporal inseparability. The RFc method is easily automated, efficient, and characterizes multiple RFs simultaneously, as required in work with multiple electrode arrays.  相似文献   

8.
Electrosensory modulation of escape responses   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Once initiated, rapid escape responses of teleost fishes are thought to be completed without additional sensory modification. This suggests that the motor program for a particular response is selected for by the constellation of sensory cues existing at the time of the releasing stimulus. This paper presents initial evidence that a highly specialized, phylogenetically recent electrosensory system is integrated with a primitive motor system and allows an animal to continuously monitor its environment for producing accurate escape behaviors.Behavioral testing for directed startle responses in a Y-maze demonstrates that when presented immediately before an acoustic startle stimulus, electric fish (Eigenmannia virescens), direct their response away from the cue (a transient shorting of their electric field). Thus, electrosensory cues as brief as 100 ms provide directional information to the escape motor network.In electric fish that are curarized to facilitate intracellular recording, the normal electric organ discharge is attenuated. When an electronically generated replacement field of the same frequency and amplitude as the fish's normal signal is shorted, a fast-rising, 7 ms latency post-synaptic potential is evoked from the Mauthner cell. Similar PSPs are generated by turning the replacement stimulus on and off. In some recordings, removing the S1 replacement field elicits a rebound of other afferent activity to the Mauthner cell; replacing the field suppresses this activity.Abbreviations EHP extrinsic hyperpolarizing potential - EOD electric organ discharge - JAR jaming avoidance response - LED light emitting diode - PSP postsynaptic potential  相似文献   

9.
The visual system of the fly is able to extract different types of global retinal motion patterns as may be induced on the eyes during different flight maneuvers and to use this information to control visual orientation. The mechanisms underlying these tasks were analyzed by a combination of quantitative behavioral experiments on tethered flying flies (Musca domestica) and model simulations using different conditions of oscillatory large-field motion and relative motion of different segments of the stimulus pattern. Only torque responses about the vertical axis of the animal were determined. The stimulus patterns consisted of random dot textures (Julesz patterns) which could be moved either horizontally or vertically. Horizontal rotatory large-field motion leads to compensatory optomotor turning responses, which under natural conditions would tend to stabilize the retinal image. The response amplitude depends on the oscillation frequency: It is much larger at low oscillation frequencies than at high ones. When an object and its background move relative to each other, the object may, in principle, be discriminated and then induce turning responses of the fly towards the object. However, whether the object is distinguished by the fly depends not only on the phase relationship between object and background motion but also on the oscillation frequency. At all phase relations tested, the object is detected only at high oscillation frequencies. For the patterns used here, the turning responses are only affected by motion along the horizontal axis of the eye. No influences caused by vertical motion could be detected. The experimental data can be explained best by assuming two parallel control systems with different temporal and spatial integration properties: TheLF-system which is most sensitive to coherent rotatory large-field motion and mediates compensatory optomotor responses mainly at low oscillation frequencies. In contrast, theSF-system is tuned to small-field and relative motion and thus specialized to discriminate a moving object from its background; it mediates turning responses towards objects mainly at high oscillation frequencies. The principal organization of the neural networks underlying these control systems could be derived from the characteristic features of the responses to the different stimulus conditions. The input to the model circuits responsible for the characteristic sensitivity of the SF-system to small-field and relative motion is provided by retinotopic arrays of local movement detectors. The movement detectors are integrated by a large-field element, the output cell of the network. The synapses between the detectors and the output cells have nonlinear transmission characteristics. Another type of large-field elements (pool cells) which respond to motion in front of both eyes and have characteristic direction selectivities are assumed to interact with the local movement detector channels by inhibitory synapses of the shunting type, before the movement detectors are integrated by the output cells. The properties of the LF-system can be accounted for by similar model circuits which, however, differ with respect to the transmission characteristic of the synapses between the movement detectors and the output cell; moreover, their pool cells are only monocular. This type of network, however, is not necessary to account for the functional properties of the LF-system. Instead, intrinsic properties of single neurons may be sufficient. Computer simulations of the postulated mechanisms of the SF-and LF-system reveal that these can account for the specific features of the behavioral responses under quite different conditions of coherent large-field motion and relative motion of different pattern segments.  相似文献   

10.
15N–1H residual dipolar couplings (RDC) have been used as additional restraints to refine the solution structure of the ribotoxin -sarcin. The RDC values were obtained by partial alignment of -sarcin in the binary mixture of n-dodecyl hexa(ethylene glycol)/hexanol. A total of 131 RDCs were measured and 106 were introduced in the final steps of the calculation protocol following the main calculation based on nuclear Overhauser enhancements and torsion angle restraints. A homogeneous family of 81 conformers was obtained. The resulting average pairwise root-mean-square deviation corresponding to the superposition of the 20 best structures is 0.69±0.12 Å for the backbone and 1.29±0.14 Å for all heavy atoms. The new structural features derived from the refined structure, compared with the non-refined structure of -sarcin, consist of new hydrogen bonds and a better definition of the backbone conformation. In particular, the loop segment spanning Gly 60 to Lys 70 shows a single conformation, corresponding to the most populated family of conformers observed in the unrefined structure. The information derived from the analysis of the refined structure and the comparison with the homologous protein restrictocin could help in establishing further structure–function relationships concerning -sarcin which can be reasonably extrapolated to other members of the ribotoxin family.  相似文献   

11.
In the present report, we used serological, cellular, and restriction fragment length polymorphism (RFLP) to investigate the DR1 haplotype in the Israeli population. We describe an Israeli homozygous typing cell (HTC), HLA-DwLVA, which defines a new lymphocyte-activating determinant associated with Bw65, DR1 and distinct from Dwl. The parents of this donor, non-Ashkenazi Algerian Jews, are first cousins and share HLA-Cw8, Bw65, BfS, DR1, DQw1, DPw4. No specificity could be assigned to HLA-DwLVA using the 91 Ninth Workshop HTCs. Two families and forty unrelated DR1 individuals were studied with DwLVA and a panel of DR1/Dw1 HTCs. HLA-DwLVA showed segregation as a single determinant within families. This new specificity was present in 24 out of 40 (60%) unrelated DR1 individuals, indicating that in the Israeli population DwLVA is the main lymphocyte-defined determinant associated with the serologically defined DRI specificity, in contrast to non-Jewish Caucasoids where DR1 is significantly associated with Dw1. The vast majority of DwLVA-positive carriers were also Bw65 carriers, indicating that Bw65, DR1, DwLVA may represent a typical allele combination in the Israeli population. The RFLP analysis established the correlation of certain RFLPs with Dw1 and DwLVA. In addition, we describe a cluster of RFLPs that may correspond to a new Dw subtype associated with DR1, for which no serological and cellular reagents have been described so far.  相似文献   

12.

Background

Reactions to sensory events sometimes require quick responses whereas at other times they require a high degree of accuracy–usually resulting in slower responses. It is important to understand whether visual processing under different response speed requirements employs different neural mechanisms.

Methodology/Principal Findings

We asked participants to classify visual patterns with different levels of detail as real-world or non-sense objects. In one condition, participants were to respond immediately, whereas in the other they responded after a delay of 1 second. As expected, participants performed more accurately in delayed response trials. This effect was pronounced for stimuli with a high level of detail. These behavioral effects were accompanied by modulations of stimulus related EEG gamma oscillations which are an electrophysiological correlate of early visual processing. In trials requiring speeded responses, early stimulus-locked oscillations discriminated real-world and non-sense objects irrespective of the level of detail. For stimuli with a higher level of detail, oscillatory power in a later time window discriminated real-world and non-sense objects irrespective of response speed requirements.

Conclusions/Significance

Thus, it seems plausible to assume that different response speed requirements trigger different dynamics of processing.  相似文献   

13.

Background

One common criterion for classifying electrophysiological brain responses is based on the distinction between transient (i.e. event-related potentials, ERPs) and steady-state responses (SSRs). The generation of SSRs is usually attributed to the entrainment of a neural rhythm driven by the stimulus train. However, a more parsimonious account suggests that SSRs might result from the linear addition of the transient responses elicited by each stimulus. This study aimed to investigate this possibility.

Methodology/Principal Findings

We recorded brain potentials elicited by a checkerboard stimulus reversing at different rates. We modeled SSRs by sequentially shifting and linearly adding rate-specific ERPs. Our results show a strong resemblance between recorded and synthetic SSRs, supporting the superposition hypothesis. Furthermore, we did not find evidence of entrainment of a neural oscillation at the stimulation frequency.

Conclusions/Significance

This study provides evidence that visual SSRs can be explained as a superposition of transient ERPs. These findings have critical implications in our current understanding of brain oscillations. Contrary to the idea that neural networks can be tuned to a wide range of frequencies, our findings rather suggest that the oscillatory response of a given neural network is constrained within its natural frequency range.  相似文献   

14.
Summary The visually guided orientation behaviour of stationarily flying Musca domestica (females) has been investigated. Under such conditions, the flight activity does not influence the visual stimulus (openloop) and the tendency of a fly to orientate towards some visual object can be recorded as a yaw torque reaction (orientation response).—Orientation responses to flickering stripes reveal two different mechanisms of visual integration, namely a local flicker detecting mechanism and a specific kind of dynamic lateral interactions (Figs. 3, 5). The lateral interactions are mediated by a field of interconnections of receptors which are separated by at least 4 to 6 vertical rows of ommatidia (Figs. 3, 8). While stimulation of not more than 3 vertical rows of ommatidia activates only flicker detection, stimuli of more than 6° width may in addition exert an excitatory or an inhibitory influence as a consequence of the associated nonlinear interactions (Figs. 5, 7). The relevance of these lateral interactions for tracking and chasing behaviour is discussed. It is suggested that the fly's visual pattern discrimination rests essentially on these lateral interactions.  相似文献   

15.
We report a model that reproduces many of the behavioral properties of smooth pursuit eye movements. The model is a negative-feedback system that uses three parallel visual motion pathways to drive pursuit. The three visual pathways process image motion, defined as target motion with respect to the moving eye, and provide signals related to image velocity, image acceleration, and a transient that occurs at the onset of target motion. The three visual motion signals are summed and integrated to produce the eye velocity output of the model. The model reproduces the average eye velocity evoked by steps of target velocity in monkeys and humans and accounts for the variation among individual responses and subjects. When its motor pathways are expanded to include positive feedback of eye velocity and a switch, the model reproduces the exponential decay in eye velocity observed when a moving target stops. Manipulation of this expanded model can mimic the effects of stimulation and lesions in the arcuate pursuit area, the middle temporal visual area (MT), and the medial superior temporal visual area (MST).  相似文献   

16.
Summary The retinal rods are usually assumed to function, at least under certain stimulus conditions, as single-photon-detectors. Thus the problem arises how a stream of events (the elementary photon responses) with Poisson statistics, but in general with a modulated mean, can be processed. Since the human visual system has a dynamic range of some 10 decades and optic nerve fibers cannot carry higher event rates than, say, 400–1,000 events/sec, event rate reduction seems to be necessary. Event rate reduction of modulated Poisson processes is possible with K-scalers (devices that only pass the K-th event following the previous output event). In view of the enormous dynamic range mentioned above, adaptation of the scaling factor is necessary. An adapting scaler that implements the Weber-law proves to furnish an interesting receptor model. Such a Weber-machine can function as a single-event detector at low input intensities and as an event rate differentiator at high intensities.Absolute threshold measurements have led to the idea that at least 2–12 photons have to be absorbed in a retinal sampling-unit within some coincidence interval T in order to perceive a flash. Such a coincidence detection can be combined with the idea of adapting scalers in a deVries-Rose machine or square-root-coincidence scaler (van de Grind and Bouman, 1968). Placing the deVries-Rose machines as centres of convergence at the bipolar level and the Weber machines at the receptor level, we get a model that makes understandable both absolute threshold phenomena and increment threshold data for the scotopic as well as the photopic range of luminances.The incorporation of adaptation phenomena by the creation of adaptation pools is possible if the horizontal cells are postulated to regulate the scaling factors of the receptors (Weber machines) and the amacrine cells those of the bipolars (deVries-Rose machines).The stepresponses of the two types of machines as displayed in P.S.T.-histograms can be directly compared with those of many visual (and auditory) cells as reported in the literature. This greatly reinforces our trust in the present approach.  相似文献   

17.
Summary Specimens of the crayfishProcambarus clarkii turn to face in the direction of a brief tactile stimulus delivered to a walking leg. The control system that guides this directed behavior was investigated under closed-loop and open-loop conditions. The accuracy of turns exhibited in these experiments was compared to baseline accuracy established by animals restrained from forward and backward walking but allowed to rotate in the yaw plane. Procambarus clarkii individuals deprived of visual feedback tended to undershoot the target angle. Response accuracy increased when a uniform field of stripes moved across the visual field in accordance with the turning movements of the animal. Response accuracy did not match the accuracy observed under baseline conditions, however, unless the responding animal encountered a novel visual image, such as the silhouette of a crayfish, in the moving visual field.Visual feedback thus influences the accuracy of turning in crayfish in two important ways. Movement of stripes across the visual field of a crayfish feeds back positively and promotes rapid turning during the initial phase of a response. This effect obtains regardless of the direction or rate of movement of the stripes in the visual field. The appearance of a novel image in the visual field feeds back negatively to inhibit at least partially further turning. Feedback from the visual system appears to fine tune basic turning movements initiated by a tactile stimulus and crudely directed according to that input. Turning behavior in the crayfish resembles in this respect compensatory eye movements in the lobster and escape responses in a number of arthropods.Neural mechanisms that may explain the experimental results are discussed with particular emphasis on the possibility of interaction between voluntary turning responses and optomotor reactions.  相似文献   

18.
The turning responses of clawed toads (Xenopus laevis) to surface waves were examined in animals with an intact lateral line or with different combinations of lateral lines reversibly inactivated by CoCl2. The responses were characterized with respect to response frequency, turning accuracy, turning side, response time, and swim distance. After the inactivation most animals still responded to surface waves but the responses were different from those of animals with an intact lateral line. They also differed according to the combination of inactivated lines. In all experiments the responses for stimuli in some sectors of the surface did not differ from controls. The location of these sectors co-varied with the position of the intact lines, i.e., normal responses were found for frontal stimulus directions when head lines were intact and for caudolateral stimulus directions when trunk lines were intact. Their size was larger when lines on both sides of the body were intact and smaller when only lines on one side were intact. When the number of functional lines was reduced to one or two on one side of the body the turning angles shown within the sector of normal responses were maintained for stimulus directions outside these sectors. These results can be interpreted as indicating that head and trunk lines represent different position values. When only a single line was functional the toads still turned towards the stimulus source more often than by chance.It is hypothesized that Xenopus uses two mechanisms to determine the direction of surface waves. One uses the position values of head and trunk lines; this mechanism is comparable to the place value postulated for individual head neuromasts of surface feeding fish. The other uses the information encoded in the activity pattern that is elicited in one line when the surface wave travels over the line. This second mechanism yields information about stimulus side but not about stimulus angle.  相似文献   

19.
Summary Operant behavior is studied in tethered Drosophila flies using visual motion, heat or odour as operandum and yaw torque, thrust or direction of flight as operans in various combinations (Fig. 1). On the basis of these results a conceptual framework of operant behavior is proposed: (1) It requires a goal (desired state) of which the actual state deviates. (2) To attain the goal a range of motor programs is activated (initiating activity, see Fig. 7). (3) Efference copies of the motor programs are compared to the sensory input referring to the deviation from the desired state (e.g. by cross-correlation). (4) In case of a significant coincidence the respective motor program is used to modify the sensory input in the direction towards the goal. (5) Consistent control of a sensory stimulus by a behavior may lead to a more permanent behavioral change (conditioning). In this scheme operant activity (1–4) and operant conditioning (1–5) are distinguished.Abbreviations ALU arbitrary length unit - d horizontal angular width of visual pattern - IR infrared - SEM standard error of the means - T yaw torque - Th thrust - performance index - horizontal angle between visual pattern position and longitudinal body axis of the fly - vertical angular extension of visual pattern  相似文献   

20.
Summary To investigate how GABAergic function affects seizure development, the effects of a GABA antagonist, bicuculline, on neocortical and hippocampal kindling were examined in chronically prepared rabbits. Kindling-inducing stimulations consisted of stimulus trains repeated at 5-min interstimulus intervals to produce so-called rapid kindling. The changes in after-discharge (AD) durations induced by each of 15 trials of stimulus trains per session were compared before and 30 min after i.p. injection of bicuculline solution (2 mg/kg) in each of three kindling groups consisting of 5 rabbits each, i.e. visual cortical, motor cortical and hippocampal kindling groups. In the visual cortex and to a less extent, the motor cortex kindling groups, the AD durations were shortened after bicuculline injection and did not show the progressive prolongation seen before the injection. In contrast, the hippocampal kindling group showed a further marked prolongation of the AD durations after the injection. The bicuculline-induced blockade of neocortical kindling suggests facilitative GABAergic action on seizure development, while the drug-induced enhancement of hippocampal kindling reflects the known inhibitory GABAergic action.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号