首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Four pigeons responded in a concurrent-chains procedure in which terminal-link schedules were fixed-interval (FI) 10 s and FI 20 s. Across sessions, the location of the shorter terminal-link changed according to a pseudorandom binary sequence. Each session, the variable-interval initial-link schedule value was sampled from a uniform distribution that ranged from 0.01 to 30 s. On some terminal links, food was withheld to obtain measures of temporal control. Terminal-link delays determined choice (log initial-link response ratios) and timing (start and stop times on no-food trials) measures, which stabilized within the 1st half of each session. Preference for the shorter terminal-link delay was a monotonically decreasing function of initial-link duration. There was no evidence of control by initial-link durations from previous sessions.  相似文献   

2.
3.
Theories of timing have been applied to choice between delayed rewards by assuming that delays are represented in memory and that subjects sample from memory when choosing between alternatives. To search for covariation in single-trial measures of performance that might confirm this assumption, we used a procedure that allowed for convergent measurement of choice and timing behavior. Four pigeons responded in a concurrent chains/peak procedure in which the terminal links were fixed-interval (FI) 8s and FI 16s, across conditions the duration of the initial-link schedule was either short or long, and one quarter of the terminal links lasted for 48 s and ended without reinforcer delivery. Preference for the FI 8-s alternative was stronger with shorter initial links, replicating the 'initial-link effect'. Responding on no-food trials was unaffected by initial-link duration, and aggregated across trials, was typical of the peak procedure: response distributions were approximately Gaussian, with modes near the FI schedule durations, and variance was greater for the FI 16-s terminal link. Analysis of local measures of initial-link performance (e.g., pause to begin responding, time spent responding, number and duration of visits to each alternative, etc.) found that the initial-link effect was associated with an increase in the number and duration of visits per cycle to the nonpreferred alternative. Regression analyses showed that local initial-link measures contributed relatively little additional variance in predicting performance on individual no-food trials beyond that accounted for by FI schedule. Our results provide no clear evidence that initial- and terminal-link responding in concurrent chains are mediated by a common representation of terminal-link delays.  相似文献   

4.
5.
In the present study, effects of d-amphetamine on sensitivity to reinforcement amount under concurrent schedules were examined using a rapid-acquisition procedure. Four pigeons key pecked under single concurrent variable-interval 30-s schedules of grain presentation. Two different reinforcer-amount ratios (7:1 and 1:7) changed across sessions according to a 31-step pseudo-random binary sequence (PRBS). After at least four times through the PRBS, response ratios generally tracked the session-to-session changes in amount ratios; estimates of sensitivity ranged from 0.26 to 0.31 across the four pigeons. Effects of a range of doses of d-amphetamine (0.3-5.6 mg/kg) then were determined. For 3 of 4 pigeons, at least one dose, which did not dramatically alter overall response output or bias, decreased sensitivity to reinforcement amount. These results suggest that reducing sensitivity of responding to reinforcement amount may be one behavioral mechanism of stimulants, which may have implications for interpreting drug effects on self-control.  相似文献   

6.
Four pigeons were trained on concurrent variable-interval 30-s schedules. Relative reinforcer amounts arranged across the two alternatives were varied across sessions according to a pseudorandom binary sequence [cf., Hunter, I., Davison, M., 1985. Determination of a behavioral transfer function: white-noise analysis of session-to-session response-ratio dynamics on concurrent VI schedules. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 43, 43-59]; the ratios (left/right) were either 1/7 or 7/1. Reinforcer amount was manipulated by varying the number of 1.2s hopper presentations. Sessions ended after 30 reinforcers (15 for each alternative). After approximately 30 sessions, response ratios for all pigeons began to track the changes in amount ratio (i.e., subjects' responding showed a moderate increase in sensitivity of responding to reinforcer amount). Characteristics of responding were similar to procedures in which reinforcer rate and immediacy have been manipulated, although sensitivity estimates for amount were lower than those previously obtained with rate and immediacy. This procedure may serve as a useful method for studying the effects of certain environmental manipulations (e.g., drug administration) on sensitivity to reinforcer amount.  相似文献   

7.
Procedural variables are concerned with the way subjects contact stimulus, response, and reinforcer variables and relations in an experiment. Procedural variables may therefore be understood as tools that allow researchers and theorists to explore the generality of their conceptions of independent variables, behavioral processes, and quantitative laws of behavior. The present article reviews data from several studies showing how procedural variables can influence the outcome of operant choice experiments, even when nominal independent variables remain constant. An important theoretical issue raised by these data is whether researchers have most usefully identified the independent variables that they incorporate into their quantitative models. Particular attention is paid to how procedural variables can influence the measure of reinforcement frequency over time that best correlates with the distribution of choice responding over time.  相似文献   

8.
To compare the generalized matching law (Baum, W.M., 1974b. On two types of deviation from the matching law: bias and undermatching. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 22, 231-242) and contingency discriminability model (Davison, M., Jenkins, P.E., 1985. Stimulus discriminability, contingency discriminability, and schedule performance. Anim. Learn. Behav. 13, 77-84) as accounts of concurrent schedule performance, we conducted a residual meta-analysis of response- and time-allocation data from 20 studies (n's=886 and 774, respectively). Both models were fitted to the individual-subject data from each study, and residuals were obtained. Polynomial regressions were then performed on the pooled residuals to determine whether systematic trends were present as a function of predicted values. For the contingency discriminability model, the cubic coefficients were positive and statistically significant for both response- and time-allocation data. By contrast, no statistically significant systematic trend was obtained in the residuals for the generalized matching law. These results suggest that the relationship between log response allocation and log reinforcer allocation does not deviate significantly from linearity over an approximate range of +1.25 to -1.25 log units, consistent with the generalized matching law. Although qualitative criteria are also important in comparing models of behavioral phenomena, residual meta-analysis provides a powerful quantitative methodology for model selection and should prove useful in future research.  相似文献   

9.
Previous studies with concurrent-chains procedures have shown that preference for a terminal-link signaling a higher reinforcement rate decreases as initial-link durations increase. Using a concurrent-chains procedure, the present experiment examined the effects of manipulating initial-link duration on preference and resistance to disruption with rats nose poking for different rates of food reinforcement in the terminal links. Consistent with previous findings, preference for a terminal link with a higher reinforcement rate decreased with longer initial links. Conversely, relative resistance to disruption in the terminal link with a higher reinforcement rate increased with longer initial links. These findings are counter to the prediction of behavioral momentum theory that preference and resistance to change should be positively related.  相似文献   

10.
Four pigeons responded in a concurrent-schedule procedure in which reinforcer rates and magnitudes changed unpredictably across sessions according to independent random series. Programmed relative reinforcement rates and magnitudes were always either 2:1 or 1:2. Pigeons' response allocation tended to stabilize within sessions and multiple regression analyses showed that it was determined by rates and magnitudes from the current session. Sensitivity coefficients were positive and statistically significant for current-session reinforcement and magnitude ratios. Although there were individual differences in sensitivity to rate and magnitude, their interaction was not significant across subjects. Rate and magnitude both controlled responding in single sessions and individual interreinforcer intervals. Analyses of responding within sessions showed that preference was more extreme when the richer rate and larger magnitude were associated with the same alternative than when they were associated with different alternatives. Overall, results support the concatenated generalized matching law's assumptions of additivity and independence as applied to choice in transition.  相似文献   

11.
This study evaluated the effect of a signal on resistance to change using a multiple schedule of reinforcement. Experiment 1 presented pigeons with three schedules: a signaled delay to reinforcement schedule (a two-link chain schedule with a variable-interval 120-s initial link followed by a 5-s fixed-time schedule), an unsignaled delay schedule (a comparable two-link tandem schedule), and an immediate, zero-delay variable-interval 125-s schedule. Two separate disruption procedures assessed resistance to change: extinction and adding a variable-time 20-s schedule of reinforcement to the inter-component interval. Resistance to change tests were conducted twice, once with the signal stimulus (the terminal link of the chain schedule) present and once with it absent. Results from both disruption procedures showed that signal absence reduced resistance to change for the pre-signal stimulus. In probe choice tests subjects strongly preferred the signal stimulus over the unsignaled stimulus and exhibited no reliable preference when given a choice between the signal stimulus and immediate stimulus. Experiment 2 presented two equal signaled schedules where, during resistance to change tests, the signal remained for one schedule and was removed for the second. Resistance to change was consistently lower when the signal was absent.  相似文献   

12.
Behavioral momentum theory is an evolving theoretical account of the strength of behavior. One challenge for the theory is determining the role of signal stimuli in determining response strength. This study evaluated the effect of an unsignaled delay between the initial link and terminal link of a two-link chain schedule on resistance to change using a multiple schedule of reinforcement. Pigeons were presented two different signaled delay to reinforcement schedules. Both schedules employed a two-link chain schedule with a variable interval 120-s initial link followed by a 5-s fixed time terminal link schedule. One of the schedules included a 5-s unsignaled delay between the initial link and the terminal link. Resistance to change was assessed with two separate disruption procedures: extinction and adding a variable time 20-s schedule of reinforcement to the inter-component interval. Baseline responding was lower in the schedule with the unsignaled delay but resistance to change for the initial link was unaffected by the unsignaled delay. The results suggest that not all unsignaled delays are equal in their effect on resistance to change.  相似文献   

13.
朱传柳  夏小乐  史伟  王武 《微生物学通报》2017,44(10):2269-2279
【目的】以巴氏醋酸杆菌工业菌株沪酿1.01和模式菌株Acetobacter pasteurianus ATCC33445为研究对象,研究工业菌株与模式菌株在高浓度醋酸胁迫下的产酸发酵时呼吸链酶活、相关基因转录水平的变化规律。【方法】检测两种菌株在0、1%、2%、3%不同初始醋酸浓度下的生物量、产酸以及酶活,并通过实时荧光定量PCR检测呼吸链相关酶合成基因的相对转录水平变化。【结果】两种菌株在1%初始醋酸浓度下产酸能力最强,发酵48 h平均产酸速度达到0.667 g/(L·h);两种菌株的ADH(乙醇脱氢酶)和ALDH(乙醛脱氢酶)酶活也达到最高,平均为12.01和9.77 U/mg;相关酶合成基因的相对转录水平较无底酸均提高。当初始醋酸浓度上升至2%和3%时,菌体酶活、产酸能力逐渐下降,呼吸链上的adh、cyt bd、cyt o和fap A基因转录水平上升,其余基因都降低。【结论】确定了巴氏醋酸杆菌在高初始浓度醋酸条件下,菌体会自发提高adh基因的转录水平,启动底物磷酸化,并将醋酸泵出到胞外;同时aldh基因的表达受到抑制,降低产酸,从而维持体内较低醋酸浓度。此外,呼吸链上其他外排相关酶转录水平也会提升,如cyt bd和cyt o,辅助底物磷酸化进程,加快释放能量。  相似文献   

14.
Growth of the cutaneous lesions of Leishmania mexicana and L. tropica major (P strain) in CFLP mice was markedly inhibited by concurrent Trypanosoma brucei infections. Restoration of normal growth of the lesion occurred within 1 week of the mice being treated with a trypanocidal drug. The presence of the concurrent T. brucei infections did not affect the development of acquired immunity to L. tropica, manifested as ulceration and healing of the lesion, nor did it induce any detectable immunity to L. mexicana. The possible underlying mechanisms are discussed.  相似文献   

15.
Concurrent use of herbal medicine (HM) and conventional medicine (CM) is increasing. However, little is known about the prevalence of drug-induced liver injury (DILI) related to this concurrent use. In order to investigate changes in liver enzymes during concurrent use of HM and CM and to assess the prevalence of DILI related to their concurrent use, we screened for liver injury among inpatients at our institution who were administered both HM and CM for at least 14 days while hospitalized between 2006 and 2010. We used the Council for International Organization of Medical Science (CIOMS) laboratory criteria to define liver injury. Of the 892 patients included in the study, 34 (3.81%) had liver injury on admission and 21 (2.35%) had liver injury at discharge. Of the 48 cases that fulfilled the CIOMS laboratory criteria for liver injury, 34 had preexisting liver injury. The remaining 14 were analyzed, and five were concluded to have DILI, resulting in a prevalence of 5/892 (0.56%, with a 95% confidence interval of 0.07-1.05%). Overall, clinical symptoms of liver injury were mild. We thus contend that concurrent use of HM and CM is relatively safe.  相似文献   

16.
Schistosomes are parasitic blood flukes that reside in human mesenteric veins or urinary bladder veins, depending on species of the parasite. The syncytial tegument of these parasites represents a dynamic interface that regulates nutritional and immunological interactions between the parasite and the host. It is known that the components for biogenesis and maintenance of the tegument are supplied via vesicles from the nucleated cell bodies beneath the syncytium and muscle layer. To investigate the common motor components of vesicular transport in the tegument of schistomes, we extracted Schistosoma mansoni tegumental microtubule associated proteins utilizing detergent/high-salt procedure and raised antiserum against these proteins. The antiserum was applied to screen Schistosoma haematobium λgt11 expression library and some of the isolated clones were sequenced. Blast search for the sequences against NCBI database identified clones that are dynein light chains and myosin genes. Further analysis of schistosome dynein genes in the databases identified three families of dynein light chains (Dlcs). The Tctex family protein sequences are significantly different from the mammalian homologs and, therefore, offer a potential vaccine/drug target against schistosomes.  相似文献   

17.
A method is presented for acquiring high‐spatial‐resolution spectral maps, in particular for Raman micro‐spectroscopy (RMS), by selectively sampling the spatial features of interest and interpolating the results. This method achieves up to 30 times reduction in the sampling time compared to raster‐scanning, the resulting images have excellent correlation with conventional histopathological staining, and are achieved with sufficient spectral signal‐to‐noise ratio to identify individual tissue structures. The benefits of this selective sampling method are not limited to tissue imaging however; it is expected that the method may be applied to other techniques which employ point‐by‐point mapping of large substrates. (© 2012 WILEY‐VCH Verlag GmbH & Co. KGaA, Weinheim)  相似文献   

18.
Summary Self-incompatibility in flowering plants is controlled by the S-gene, encoding stylar S (allele-specific) glycoproteins. In addition to three previously characterized Petunia hybrida S-proteins, we identified by N-terminal sequence analysis another stylar S-protein, co-segregating with the S b-allele. Purified S-proteins reveal biological activity, as is demonstrated for two of them by the allele-specific inhibition of pollen tube growth in vitro. Moreover, the four isolated S-proteins are ribonucleases (S-RNases). Specific activities vary from 30 (S1) to 1000 (S2) units per min per mg protein. We attempted to investigate the functionality of the carbohydrate portion of the S-RNases. Deglycosylation studies with the enzyme peptide-N-glycosidase F (PNGase F) reveals differences in the number of N-linked glycan chains present on the four S-RNases. Variability in the extent of glycosylation accounts for most of the molecular weight differences observed among these proteins. By amino acid sequencing, the positions of two of the three N-glycosylation sites on the S2-RNase could be located near the N-terminus. Enzymic removal of the glycan side chains has no effect on the RNase activity of native S-RNases. This suggests another role of the glycan moiety in the self-incompatibility mechanism.  相似文献   

19.
Four pigeons responded under a 7-component mixed schedule in which each component arranged a different left:right reinforcer ratio (27:1, 9:1, 3:1, 1:1, 1:3, 1:9, 1:27). Components were unsignaled, and the order within each session was randomly determined. After extensive exposure to these contingencies, effects of a range of doses of d-amphetamine (0.3-5.6 mg/kg) on estimates of sensitivity to reinforcement at several levels of analysis were assessed. Under non-drug conditions, the structure of choice was similar to that previously reported under this procedure. That is, responding adjusted within components to the reinforcer ratio in effect (i.e., sensitivity estimates were higher in the 2nd than in the 1st half of components), and individual reinforcers produced “preference pulses” (i.e., each food presentation produced an immediate, local, shift in preference toward the response that just produced food). Although there was a general tendency for d-amphetamine to reduce overall sensitivity to reinforcement, the size of this effect and its reliability varied across pigeons. Further analysis, however, revealed that intermediate d-amphetamine doses consistently reduced sensitivity immediately following reinforcer presentations; that is, these doses consistently attenuated preference pulses.  相似文献   

20.
It has been suggested that disturbances in endocannabinoid signaling contribute to the development of depressive illness; however, at present there is insufficient evidence to allow for a full understanding of this role. To further this understanding, we performed an analysis of the endocannabinoid system in an animal model of depression. Male rats exposed to chronic, unpredictable stress (CUS) for 21 days exhibited a reduction in sexual motivation, consistent with the hypothesis that CUS in rats induces depression-like symptoms. We determined the effects of CUS, with or without concurrent treatment with the antidepressant imipramine (10 mg/kg), on CP55940 binding to the cannabinoid CB(1) receptor; whole tissue endocannabinoid content; and fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH) activity in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, hypothalamus, amygdala, midbrain and ventral striatum. Exposure to CUS resulted in a significant increase in CB(1) receptor binding site density in the prefrontal cortex and a decrease in CB(1) receptor binding site density in the hippocampus, hypothalamus and ventral striatum. Except in the hippocampus, these CUS-induced alterations in CB(1) receptor binding site density were attenuated by concurrent antidepressant treatment. CUS alone produced a significant reduction in N-arachidonylethanolamine (anandamide) content in every brain region examined, which was not reversed by antidepressant treatment. These data suggest that the endocannabinoid system in cortical and subcortical structures is differentially altered in an animal model of depression and that the effects of CUS on CB(1) receptor binding site density are attenuated by antidepressant treatment while those on endocannabinoid content are not.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号