共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
J C Petiot J Parrot 《Comptes rendus des séances de la Société de biologie et de ses filiales》1980,174(6):1046-1052
Maximum information transmitted within 17 ms exposure is 4.1. bits/signal with white dots against black background and 3.5. bits/signal with black dots against white background. Higher efficiency is observed at peripheral parts with both displays, but especially for the first type of display (50 young Female Subjects). 相似文献
2.
Locked nucleic acid (LNA) and 2'-O-methyl nucleotide (OMeN) are the most extensively studied nucleotide analogues. Although both LNA and OMeN are characterized by the C3'-endo sugar pucker conformation, which is dominant in A-form DNA and RNA nucleotides, they demonstrate different binding behaviours. Previous studies have focused attention on their properties of duplex stabilities, hybridization kinetics and resistance against nuclease digestion; however, their ability to discriminate mismatched hybridizations has been explored much less. In this study, LNA- and OMeN-modified oligonucleotide probes have been prepared and their effects on the DNA duplex stability have been examined: LNA modifications can enhance the duplex stability, whereas OMeN modifications reduce the duplex stability. Next, we studied how the LNA:DNA and OMeN:DNA mismatches reduced the duplex stability. Melting temperature measurement showed that different LNA:DNA or OMeN:DNA mismatches indeed influence the duplex stability differently. LNA purines can discriminate LNA:DNA mismatches more effectively than LNA pyrimidines as well as DNA nucleotides. Furthermore, we designed five LNA- and five OMeN-modified oligonucleotide probes to simulate realistic situations where target-probe duplexes contain a complementary LNA:DNA or OMeN:DNA base pairs and a DNA:DNA mismatch simultaneously. The measured collective effect showed that the duplex stability was enhanced by the complementary LNA:DNA base pair but decreased by the DNA:DNA mismatch in a position-dependent manner regardless of the chemical identity and position of the complementary LNA:DNA base pair. On the other hand, the OMeN-modified probes also showed that the duplex stability was reduced by both the OMeN modification and the OMeN:DNA mismatch in a position-dependent manner. 相似文献
3.
It has been shown that vibration level, feed force, and exposure duration cause unfavourable effects on the work performance when hand-held vibrating tools are used by operators. Present study analyzed the effects of these variables on the heart rate and blood pressure of the operators carrying out the drilling task. Under three different levels of vibration (0.3, 0.5, and 1 m/s2), the operators performed the drilling for 3 minutes with three different levels of feed force (100, 200, and 300 N) in study 1, and for three different durations (10, 15, and 20 min) with a feed force of 200 N in study 2. Thirty male subjects participated in the two studies and data were analyzed on the basis of two factor repeated measure kind of experimental design. Results showed that in the kind of drilling task undertaken the level of vibration was statistically significant. However, the main effects of feed force and vibration exposure duration were statistically insignificant. These findings are discussed in the light of previous researches conducted on the subject. 相似文献
4.
Central base pair flipping and discrimination by PspGI 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
Szczepanowski RH Carpenter MA Czapinska H Zaremba M Tamulaitis G Siksnys V Bhagwat AS Bochtler M 《Nucleic acids research》2008,36(19):6109-6117
5.
M. Juusola 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1993,172(4):511-521
The response properties of R1–6 type photoreceptors of the blowfly (Calliphora vicina) were investigated using 1- to 600-ms light steps with contrasts from -1.00 to +1.12 at different adapting backgrounds. To prevent activation of voltage-dependent K+-channels, some photoreceptors were treated with ionophoretically injected tetraethylammonium. The linearity and time-course of the photoresponses depended on the adapting background and the duration of the contrast stimulus. At low backgrounds photoresponses were approximately linear regardless of the contrast step duration. However, at higher backgrounds photoreceptors produced, with long contrast steps of opposite polarities, larger and slower hyperpolarizing responses than depolarizing ones, but linear responses with equal time-courses to transient (2ms) contrast changes. The early rising phases of photoresponses were independent of the contrast signal duration, deviated to the same extent from the steady-state potential and steepened as the value of contrast was increased. The photoreceptor contrast gain increased with the background, but its value depended on the duration, the magnitude, and the polarity of the contrast step. Tetraethylammonium linearised the photoreceptor membrane, increased and delayed photoresponses probably by blocking the shunting of the outwardly rectifying potassium channels. These effects did not strongly influence on the photoreceptors' characteristic non-linear contrast dependence on light adaptation.Abbreviations LED
light emitting diode
- TEA
tetraethylammonium ion 相似文献
6.
The effect of task duration on odor discrimination in aging was studied. Twenty-seven young male adults and 24 young female adults between 18 and 30 years of age, and 17 older male adults between 45 and 65 years of age completed an odor discrimination task. The odor discrimination task consisted of two parts of 16 trials each in which, from three bottles consisting of two identical and one aberrant odor, the aberrant odor had to be identified. The two parts were identical except that the aberrant odor was interchanged with the identical odors in the second as compared with the first part. Results revealed a decrease in odor discrimination with age. Moreover, with increased task duration odor discrimination performance decreased considerably in older male adults while it remained unchanged in young male adults. In addition, in young adults a small advantage in females as compared with males was found in the first part of the odor discrimination task, but this effect disappeared with increased task duration. In conclusion, task duration should be taken into consideration as a factor influencing odor discrimination in aging. 相似文献
7.
Binocular interaction: contrast matching and contrast discrimination are predicted by the same model 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
How do signals from the 2 eyes combine and interact? Our recent work has challenged earlier schemes in which monocular contrast signals are subject to square-law transduction followed by summation across eyes and binocular gain control. Much more successful was a new 'two-stage' model in which the initial transducer was almost linear and contrast gain control occurred both pre- and post-binocular summation. Here we extend that work by: (i) exploring the two-dimensional stimulus space (defined by left- and right-eye contrasts) more thoroughly, and (ii) performing contrast discrimination and contrast matching tasks for the same stimuli. Twenty-five base-stimuli made from 1 c/deg patches of horizontal grating, were defined by the factorial combination of 5 contrasts for the left eye (0.3-32%) with five contrasts for the right eye (0.3-32%). Other than in contrast, the gratings in the two eyes were identical. In a 2IFC discrimination task, the base-stimuli were masks (pedestals), where the contrast increment was presented to one eye only. In a matching task, the base-stimuli were standards to which observers matched the contrast of either a monocular or binocular test grating. In the model, discrimination depends on the local gradient of the observer's internal contrast-response function, while matching equates the magnitude (rather than gradient) of response to the test and standard. With all model parameters fixed by previous work, the two-stage model successfully predicted both the discrimination and the matching data and was much more successful than linear or quadratic binocular summation models. These results show that performance measures and perception (contrast discrimination and contrast matching) can be understood in the same theoretical framework for binocular contrast vision. 相似文献
8.
9.
Caifu Chen Young-Kwon Hong Sara D. Ontiveros Michael Egholm William M. Strauss 《Mammalian genome》1999,10(1):13-18
The satellite repeat structure of the mammalian centromere contains the CENP-B protein binding site. Using the peptide nucleic
acid-fluorescence in situ hybridization (PNA-FISH), we show by direct PNA-DNA binding that all detectable CENP-B sites in
a mammalian genome might have the same sequence. Two species-specific PNA 17-mers, pMm and pMc, were identified from CENP-B
binding sites of Mus musculus and M. caroli, respectively. Fluorescence in situ hybridization confirmed that pMc hybridized to M. caroli centromeres only; however, pMm cross-hybridized to M. musculus and human centromeres. By using a series of CENP-B PNA 17-mers containing 1, 2, 3, 5, and 7 base-pair mismatches to their
DNA counterparts, we further demonstrate that PNA-FISH can discriminate between two CENP-B DNA sequences that differ by a
single base-pair in mouse and human centromeres, suggesting the degree of conservation of CENP-B sequences throughout the
genome. In comparison with DNA oligonucleotides, PNA oligomers demonstrate the higher sequence specificity, improved stability,
reproducibility, and lower background. Therefore, PNA oligomers have significant advantages over DNA oligonucleotide probes
in analyzing microsatellites in a genome.
Received: 16 June 1998 / Accepted: 3 September 1998 相似文献
10.
D Schild 《Biophysical journal》1988,54(6):1011
11.
Frans W. Maes 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1984,155(2):263-270
Summary This paper presents a model of the neural coding and discrimination of sensory intensity. The model consists of five stages: (1) the coding of stimulus intensity in peripheral receptors or neurons by a rate code. The relevance of comparing different analysis intervals for the response is pointed out; (2) neural processing, according to either labeled-line or across-fiber pattern theory. In addition, two possible non-linearities in the processing are considered: a threshold mechanism, and contrast enhancement by reciprocal inhibition; (3) a neural discriminator, based on signal-detection theory; (4) a memory stage; (5) an effector organ providing a behavioral output. Emphasis is put on stages 2 and 3.The model produces predictions of the differential threshold, which should be directly testable in a behavioral two-alternative forced-choice paradigm. The model will be applied to gustatory intensity discrimination in rat in a subsequent study (Maes and Erickson 1984). The Discussion pays attention to the relative contributions of peripheral and central noise sources. It also compares the present model with Beidler's (1958) approach through just noticeable differences (JND's). The model presented here seems more adequate in providing an understanding of sensory information processing.Abbreviations
AFP
across fiber pattern
-
DA
discrimination acuity
-
DT
differential threshold
-
JND
just noticeable difference
-
LL
labeled line
-
NTS
nucleus tractus solitarius 相似文献
12.
We have measured response times for the detection of a single target presented against a set of reference elements which are characterised by combinations of four different stimulus parameters; colour, contrast polarity, magnification and orientation. The aim of the experiments was to determine the response characteristics of visual mechanisms which mediate target detection through the discrimination of orientation and magnification. In the first experiments, we determined sensitivity to differences in colour and contrast polarity, and show that the mechanisms responsible for the discrimination of orientation and of magnification are both selective in their responses to colour and to contrast polarity. There are, nonetheless, residual interactions between patterns of different contrast polarities and between those of different colour, and in the latter case, weak interactions persist under equiluminance conditions. In a second set of experiments, we examined the interactions between orientation and magnification. We conclude that the responses of visual mechanisms which mediate target detection through discrimination of orientation are markedly dependent on stimulus magnification whereas those which mediate detection through discrimination of magnification are, in contrast, relatively insensitive to stimulus orientation. 相似文献
13.
In previous experiments two extreme modes of visual discrimination performance have been investigated by measuring small differences in pattern shape at points along a continuum of pattern shapes. These two modes, associated with discrete and continuous encoding processes, were obtained by simultaneously manipulating the number of pattern components in the display and the effective duration of the display. In this experiment, discrimination performance was measured for a fixed number of pattern components, namely three, and variable display time course. The stimuli used were curved lines drawn from a continuum with curvature parameter s. There were three stimulus time courses: (1) 2-s stimulus display, (2) 100-ms stimulus display, and (3) 100-ms stimulus display followed by a post-stimulus mask. Discrimination performance declined smoothly and monotonically with s for (1), but varied non-monotonically with s revealing a central peak for (3). Performance for (2) was intermediate between that for (1) and that for (3). A reduction in effective stimulus duration alone was thus sufficient to cause a transition from continuous to discrete modes of discrimination performance, a result which may be compatible with an explanation of variable discrimination modes based on a method of successive internal approximations of the stimulus. 相似文献
14.
The transducer function mu for contrast perception describes the nonlinear mapping of stimulus contrast onto an internal response. Under a signal detection theory approach, the transducer model of contrast perception states that the internal response elicited by a stimulus of contrast c is a random variable with mean mu(c). Using this approach, we derive the formal relations between the transducer function, the threshold-versus-contrast (TvC) function, and the psychometric functions for contrast detection and discrimination in 2AFC tasks. We show that the mathematical form of the TvC function is determined only by mu, and that the psychometric functions for detection and discrimination have a common mathematical form with common parameters emanating from, and only from, the transducer function mu and the form of the distribution of the internal responses. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these relations, which have bearings on the tenability of certain mathematical forms for the psychometric function and on the suitability of empirical approaches to model validation. We also present the results of a comprehensive test of these relations using two alternative forms of the transducer model: a three-parameter version that renders logistic psychometric functions and a five-parameter version using Foley's variant of the Naka-Rushton equation as transducer function. Our results support the validity of the formal relations implied by the general transducer model, and the two versions that were contrasted account for our data equally well. 相似文献
15.
Johanne Delisle Lucie Royer Michè le Bernier‐Cardou Alain Labrecque 《Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata》2012,144(2):231-243
As the growing season is expected to begin earlier under climate change, insects should initiate reproduction several days or weeks earlier than they used to. In eastern Canada, hemlock looper (HL) Lambdina fiscellaria (Guenée) (Lepidoptera: Geometridae) females generally oviposit in September, with eggs entering an obligatory diapause quickly after their deposition. We therefore simulated an early start of the HL reproduction cycle of 2, 4, 6, or 8 weeks to examine the extent to which freshly laid eggs from two populations (island and mainland) can withstand exposure to four temperature conditions (15, 20, 25 °C, or fluctuating temperature in an outdoor insectary), with all treatments ending on 1 September 2007. On this date, half the eggs from each population were immediately incubated at 15 °C, while the rest were stored in an outdoor insectary until their incubation at 15 °C the following spring. In a separate experiment, the effect of temperature on pre‐diapause duration was determined from the number of days required for eggs to change colour after oviposition. The pre‐diapause phase was completed faster as temperature increased. Regardless of incubation date and population, percent hatch decreased significantly after 6‐8 weeks of exposure to 25 °C or in the outdoor insectary. Under most treatments, the odds of dying as pharate larvae increased with exposure duration. When eggs were incubated at 15 °C immediately after treatment, time to hatch and diapause duration remained constant over treatments, except at 25 °C when they both decreased. After 8 weeks of exposure to 15 or 20 °C, eggs transferred outdoors were more likely to hatch precociously than those exposed to 25 °C or insectary conditions. Globally, mortality seemed greater among eggs stored outdoors than among those kept indoors. Most eggs that survived the winter hatched synchronously after incubation in spring. Overall, larger eggs from the island population survived better than smaller eggs from the mainland population. 相似文献
16.
Vanesa M. Fernández Andrés Arenas Walter M. Farina 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2009,195(8):759-768
Honeybees of different ages and reproductive castes cohabit in the hive where they are exposed to many odors that might affect
associative learning. Our aim was to analyze the role of odors pre-exposed as volatiles on appetitive learning in honeybees
of different ages and search for their long-term effect both under natural and laboratory conditions. By evaluating memory
acquisition and retention through a differential proboscis extension response conditioning, we found that hive-exposed odors
offered as a reinforced conditioned stimulus during training promoted a learning-reduced effect [latent inhibition (LI)].
On the other hand, no effect was found when the non-reinforced conditioned stimulus was pre-exposed. The LI effect varied
with the odor identity. However, only slight differences were found with the age of the bees. Exposure-conditioning intervals
longer than 24 h did not show an LI effect unless the odor concentration was increased or exposure was prolonged. Our results
show that pre-exposed volatiles could either reduce learning performance, if this odor is later associated with food, or be
irrelevant in the case that alternative scented resources circulate within the colony. The differential effects found according
to the olfactory exposure characteristics could strongly influence the propagation of chemosensory information within the
hive. 相似文献
17.
Matthews WJ 《PloS one》2011,6(5):e19815
It has been suggested that repeated stimuli have shorter subjective duration than novel items, perhaps because of a reduction in the neural response to repeated presentations of the same object. Five experiments investigated the effects of repetition on time perception and found further evidence that immediate repetition reduces apparent duration, consistent with the idea that subjective duration is partly based on neural coding efficiency. In addition, the experiments found (a) no effect of repetition on the precision of temporal discrimination, (b) that the effects of repetition disappeared when there was a modest lag between presentations, (c) that, across participants, the size of the repetition effect correlated with temporal discrimination, and (d) that the effects of repetition suggested by a temporal production task were the opposite of those suggested by temporal judgments. The theoretical and practical implications of these results are discussed. 相似文献
18.
Rouzbeh Motiei-Langroudi Mohsen Omrani Mohammad Ali Khoshnoodi Firouz Ghaderi Pakdel Emad Yazdanpanah Abdol Hossein Abbassian 《Somatosensory & motor research》2013,30(1-2):19-24
The aim of the present study was to examine the association of high blood lactate levels, induced with a maximal cycling or with an intravenous infusion, with spinal cord excitability. The study was carried out on 17 male athletes; all the subjects performed a maximal cycling test on a mechanically braked cycloergometer, while 6 of them were submitted to the intravenous infusion of a lactate solution (3?mg/kg in 1?min). Before the exercise or the injection, also at the end as well as 5 and 10?min after the conclusion, venous blood lactate was measured and excitability of the spinal α-motoneurons was evaluated by using the H reflex technique. In both experimental conditions, it has been observed that an exhaustive exercise is associated with a strong increase of blood lactate (but not of blood glucose) and with a significant reduction of spinal excitability. Since a similar augment of blood lactate induced by an intravenous infusion, in subjects not performing any exercise, is not associated with significant changes of spinal excitability, it can be concluded that the increase of blood lactate levels during a maximal exercise is not per se capable of modifying the excitability of spinal α-motoneurons. 相似文献
19.
Motiei-Langroudi R Omrani M Khoshnoodi MA Pakdel FG Yazdanpanah E Abbassian AH 《Somatosensory & motor research》2006,23(1-2):19-24
Temporal information is an embedded feature of our sensory and motor experiences. How is temporal information encoded in the brain? In the two-stage theory of timing, an explicit representation of timing is responsible for the movement initiation while movement duration is coded implicitly. We investigated the correlation of movement duration and amplitude in a repetitive one-dimensional non-visually guided movement to find out if temporal information could be coded independently from movement. Subjects were asked to learn the distance between two points by moving their hands repeatedly along the distance between two sticks, while they could not see their hands and hand path. After a training phase, a delay of either 2 or 20 s was imposed and the subjects were asked to reproduce the learned distance. There was no correlation between distance difference and time difference in either delay condition. In the 20 s delay experiment, in comparison to the 2 s delay experiment, there was a significant increase in distance reproduction error. However, there was no significant change in time differences in either of the experiments. In addition, the time difference between the training and test trials was independent from the direction of the distance difference (i.e., overshot, undershot, or accurate). In conclusion, time may be coded as an independent measure after the delay period, so it should be a kind of explicitly coded information. 相似文献