共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Barton RA 《Journal of evolutionary biology》2007,20(4):1504-1511
Changes in neocortex size were a prominent feature of mammalian brain evolution, but the implications for cortical structure, and consequently for the functional significance of such changes in overall cortical size, are poorly understood. A basic question is whether functionally differentiated cortical areas evolved independently of one another (adaptive specialization) or were allometrically constrained to co-vary tightly with the size of the whole. Here, I provide comparative evidence for adaptive specialization of cortical structure. First, the sizes of individual areas differ significantly between taxa after controlling for overall cortical size. Second, an analysis of separate visual cortical areas reveals that these exhibit statistically correlated evolution, independent of variation in nonvisual areas. Third, visual cortex size exhibits correlated evolution with peripheral visual adaptations (eye morphology and optic nerve size) and with photic niche. Thus, the evolution of mammalian cortical structure was closely associated with specialization for different sensory niches. 相似文献
2.
Although autism is a disorder of very early onset, knowledge on how it is first expressed in infancy has, until recently, remained limited. In recent years new strategies of research, including prospective studies, have substantially increased our knowledge regarding autism in infants. Research findings have suggested the very early emergence of significant differences in social information processes. In addition to having important implications for research, these findings also offer new opportunities for screening and early identification and, hopefully, for improved outcome. 相似文献
3.
4.
Miko?aj Hernik Pasco Fearon Gergely Csibra 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2014,281(1781)
Animal actions are almost universally constrained by the bilateral body-plan. For example, the direction of travel tends to be constrained by the orientation of the animal''s anteroposterior axis. Hence, an animal''s behaviour can reliably guide the identification of its front and back, and its orientation can reliably guide action prediction. We examine the hypothesis that the evolutionarily ancient relation between anteroposterior body-structure and behaviour guides our cognitive processing of agents and their actions. In a series of studies, we demonstrate that, after limited exposure, human infants as young as six months of age spontaneously encode a novel agent as having a certain axial direction with respect to its actions and rely on it when anticipating the agent''s further behaviour. We found that such encoding is restricted to objects exhibiting cues of agency and does not depend on generalization from features of familiar animals. Our research offers a new tool for investigating the perception of animate agency and supports the proposal that the underlying cognitive mechanisms have been shaped by basic biological adaptations in humans. 相似文献
5.
Peter J. Marshall Andrew N. Meltzoff 《Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences》2014,369(1644)
Studying human infants will increase our understanding of the nature, origins and function of neural mirroring mechanisms. Human infants are prolific imitators. Infant imitation indicates observation–execution linkages in the brain prior to language and protracted learning. Investigations of neural aspects of these linkages in human infants have focused on the sensorimotor mu rhythm in the electroencephalogram, which occurs in the alpha frequency range over central electrode sites. Recent results show that the infant mu rhythm is desynchronized during action execution as well as action observation. Current work is elucidating properties of the infant mu rhythm and how it may relate to prelinguistic action processing and social understanding. Here, we consider this neuroscience research in relation to developmental psychological theory, particularly the ‘Like-Me’ framework, which holds that one of the chief cognitive tasks of the human infant is to map the similarity between self and other. We elucidate the value of integrating neuroscience findings with behavioural studies of infant imitation, and the reciprocal benefit of examining mirroring mechanisms from an ontogenetic perspective. 相似文献
6.
Tomoko Sakai Mie Matsui Akichika Mikami Ludise Malkova Yuzuru Hamada Masaki Tomonaga Juri Suzuki Masayuki Tanaka Takako Miyabe-Nishiwaki Haruyuki Makishima Masato Nakatsukasa Tetsuro Matsuzawa 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2013,280(1753)
Developmental prolongation is thought to contribute to the remarkable brain enlargement observed in modern humans (Homo sapiens). However, the developmental trajectories of cerebral tissues have not been explored in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), even though they are our closest living relatives. To address this lack of information, the development of cerebral tissues was tracked in growing chimpanzees during infancy and the juvenile stage, using three-dimensional magnetic resonance imaging and compared with that of humans and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Overall, cerebral development in chimpanzees demonstrated less maturity and a more protracted course during prepuberty, as observed in humans but not in macaques. However, the rapid increase in cerebral total volume and proportional dynamic change in the cerebral tissue in humans during early infancy, when white matter volume increases dramatically, did not occur in chimpanzees. A dynamic reorganization of cerebral tissues of the brain during early infancy, driven mainly by enhancement of neuronal connectivity, is likely to have emerged in the human lineage after the split between humans and chimpanzees and to have promoted the increase in brain volume in humans. Our findings may lead to powerful insights into the ontogenetic mechanism underlying human brain enlargement. 相似文献
7.
8.
S. Gareth Edwards Lisa J. Stephenson Mario Dalmaso Andrew P. Bayliss 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2015,282(1812)
Here, we report a novel social orienting response that occurs after viewing averted gaze. We show, in three experiments, that when a person looks from one location to an object, attention then shifts towards the face of an individual who has subsequently followed the person''s gaze to that same object. That is, contrary to ‘gaze following’, attention instead orients in the opposite direction to observed gaze and towards the gazing face. The magnitude of attentional orienting towards a face that ‘follows’ the participant''s gaze is also associated with self-reported autism-like traits. We propose that this gaze leading phenomenon implies the existence of a mechanism in the human social cognitive system for detecting when one''s gaze has been followed, in order to establish ‘shared attention’ and maintain the ongoing interaction. 相似文献
9.
Na2SeO3 could affect the anion flux of Band 3 of inside-out erythrocyte membrane vesicles (IOVs). Such effect was believed to be
based on the interaction of SH groups of Band 3 with Na2SeO3. This effect could be eliminated when the cytoplasmic domain of Band 3 was proteolytically removed by trypsin. This suggested
that SH groups in the cytoplasmic domain were involved in such interaction. Measurement of the pH dependence of intrinsic
fluorescence intensity provided evidence that conformational changes of Band 3 occurred as a consequence of interaction with
selenite. KI quenching of intrinsic fluorescence of Band 3 could also show that there was a conformational change in the cytoplasmic
domain of Band 3 after reaction with Na2SeO3. Such conformational change in turn could be transmitted to the membrane domain of Band 3 monitored by quenching of intrinsic
fluorescence of Band 3 using hypocrellin B (HB) (a photosensitive pigment obtained from a parasitic fungus growing in Yunnan,
China).
It is suggested that the cytoplasmic domain of Band 3 is not necessary for its anion flux, but is essential for the regulation
(e.g., by Se) of its active site located at the membrane domain, and hence, it may provide evidence of communication between
the cytoplasmic domain and the membrane domain of Band 3. 相似文献
10.
Newborns have an innate system for preferentially looking at an upright human face. This face preference behaviour disappears at approximately one month of age and reappears a few months later. However, the neural mechanisms underlying this U-shaped behavioural change remain unclear. Here, we isolate the functional development of the cortical visual pathway for face processing using S-cone-isolating stimulation, which blinds the subcortical visual pathway. Using luminance stimuli, which are conveyed by both the subcortical and cortical visual pathways, the preference for upright faces was not observed in two-month-old infants, but it was observed in four- and six-month-old infants, confirming the recovery phase of the U-shaped development. By contrast, using S-cone stimuli, two-month-old infants already showed a preference for upright faces, as did four- and six-month-old infants, demonstrating that the cortical visual pathway for face processing is already functioning at the bottom of the U-shape at two months of age. The present results suggest that the transient functional deterioration stems from a conflict between the subcortical and cortical functional pathways, and that the recovery thereafter involves establishing a level of coordination between the two pathways. 相似文献
11.
Evidence for adaptive design in human gaze preference 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Conway CA Jones BC DeBruine LM Little AC 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2008,275(1630):63-69
Many studies have investigated the physical cues that influence face preferences. By contrast, relatively few studies have investigated the effects of facial cues to the direction and valence of others' social interest (i.e. gaze direction and facial expressions) on face preferences. Here we found that participants demonstrated stronger preferences for direct gaze when judging the attractiveness of happy faces than that of disgusted faces, and that this effect of expression on the strength of attraction to direct gaze was particularly pronounced for judgements of opposite-sex faces (study 1). By contrast, no such opposite-sex bias in preferences for direct gaze was observed when participants judged the same faces for likeability (study 2). Collectively, these findings for a context-sensitive opposite-sex bias in preferences for perceiver-directed smiles, but not perceiver-directed disgust, suggest gaze preference functions, at least in part, to facilitate efficient allocation of mating effort, and evince adaptive design in the perceptual mechanisms that underpin face preferences. 相似文献
12.
Social functioning is a key component of recovery after a potentially traumatic experience, and the buffering role of the social support in trauma resilience and recovery has been very well documented. Factors contributing to resilience and recovery are notable because although most people will experience a traumatic event during their lifetimes, only 6% to 10% are diagnosed with post‐traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The relationship between an individual and their social environment is determined both by the quality of the social environment itself, and by the individual's perception and understanding of information conveyed by the other people around them. However, little research has considered the contribution of these internal social cognitive processes to PTSD risk or resilience. The current review draws on the existing literature on social cognitive functioning in trauma exposure and PTSD, identifying key questions and themes for future research. We utilized a meta‐analytic approach to assess the evidence for alterations in social cognition in PTSD, finding a consistent large deficit in social cognitive performance in PTSD groups relative to trauma‐exposed and healthy controls. We then reviewed the literature on the interaction of genes and the social environment, supporting the hypothesis that social cognitive deficits are a preexisting risk factor for PTSD. Finally, we reviewed relevant neuroimaging findings, which suggest that alterations in social cognition affect the perception of threat cues in PTSD. Overall, research on social cognition and PTSD is still emerging, but existing findings suggest this is an important and understudied area for the understanding of PTSD. 相似文献
13.
This review concentrates on the history of the subfield of microbiology referred to as the population organization-and communication-related research direction (POCRRD). The focal points of POCRRD include intercellular interactions, information exchange between cells, and multicellular structures (colonies, biofilms, flocs, etc.). Special attention in this review is given to the contribution of Russian scientists to the development of POCRRD. In terms of POCRRD, microorganisms are viewed as social creatures that constantly communicate and form supraorganismic, intrinsically heterogeneous systems. 相似文献
14.
Gap junctional communication is required to maintain mouse cortical neural progenitor cells in a proliferative state 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
The mechanisms that determine whether neural stem cells remain in a proliferative state or differentiate into neurons or glia are largely unknown. Here we establish a pivotal role for gap junction-mediated intercellular communication in determining the proliferation and survival of mouse neural progenitor cells (NPCs). When cultured in the presence of basic fibroblast growth factor (bFGF), NPCs express the gap junction protein connexin 43 and are dye-coupled. Upon withdrawal of bFGF, levels of connexin 43 and dye coupling decrease, and the cells cease proliferating and differentiate into neurons; the induction of gap junctions by bFGF is mediated by p42/p44 mitogen-activated protein kinases. Inhibition of gap junctions abolishes the ability of bFGF to maintain NPCs in a proliferative state resulting in cell differentiation or cell death, while overexpression of connexin 43 promotes NPC self-renewal in the absence of bFGF. In addition to promoting their proliferation, gap junctions are required for the survival of NPCs. Gap junctional communication is therefore both necessary and sufficient to maintain NPCs in a self-renewing state. 相似文献
15.
《Journal of enzyme inhibition and medicinal chemistry》2013,28(3):447-455
Cholinesterases are associated with pathology characteristic of conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease and are therefore, considered targets for neuroimaging. Ester derivatives of N-methylpiperidinol are promising potential imaging agents; however, methodology is lacking for evaluating these compounds in vitro. Here, we report the synthesis and evaluation of a series of N-methylpiperidinyl thioesters, possessing comparable properties to their corresponding esters, which can be directly evaluated for cholinesterase kinetics and histochemical distribution in human brain tissue. N-methylpiperidinyl esters and thioesters were synthesized and they demonstrated comparable cholinesterase kinetics. Furthermore, thioesters were capable, using histochemical method, to visualize cholinesterase activity in human brain tissue. N-methylpiperidinyl thioesters can be rapidly evaluated for cholinesterase kinetics and visualization of enzyme distribution in brain tissue which may facilitate development of cholinesterase imaging agents for application to conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease. 相似文献
16.
Dora Kampis Eugenio Parise Gergely Csibra ágnes Melinda Kovács 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2015,282(1819)
A major feat of social beings is to encode what their conspecifics see, know or believe. While various non-human animals show precursors of these abilities, humans perform uniquely sophisticated inferences about other people''s mental states. However, it is still unclear how these possibly human-specific capacities develop and whether preverbal infants, similarly to adults, form representations of other agents'' mental states, specifically metarepresentations. We explored the neurocognitive bases of eight-month-olds'' ability to encode the world from another person''s perspective, using gamma-band electroencephalographic activity over the temporal lobes, an established neural signature for sustained object representation after occlusion. We observed such gamma-band activity when an object was occluded from the infants'' perspective, as well as when it was occluded only from the other person (study 1), and also when subsequently the object disappeared, but the person falsely believed the object to be present (study 2). These findings suggest that the cognitive systems involved in representing the world from infants'' own perspective are also recruited for encoding others'' beliefs. Such results point to an early-developing, powerful apparatus suitable to deal with multiple concurrent representations, and suggest that infants can have a metarepresentational understanding of other minds even before the onset of language. 相似文献
17.
Peter E. Maxim 《American journal of primatology》1982,2(1):63-85
Using a previously developed quantitative scale of social behavior, interactions between group members in seven groups of rhesus macaques were studied. Analysis of 636 behavior chains showed that the large number of different chain sequences (445) was due in part to the large number of contexts in which interactions were occurring. A catalog of 33 contexts of interaction was empirically developed. Particular behaviors were found to have different frequencies of occurrence in different contexts. These frequency differences can be accounted for by the verbal message statements derived for each behavior. 相似文献
18.
19.
Latif N Sarathchandra P Taylor PM Antoniw J Brand N Yacoub MH 《Cell biochemistry and biophysics》2006,45(3):255-264
Cell-cell interactions and adhesion determine cellular architectural organization, proliferation, signaling, differentiation,
and death. We have identified the molecular components of different cell-cell junctions in human valve interstitial cells
(ICs) both in situ and in culture. ICs were isolated, cultured, and phenotyped for cell surface and cytoplasmic markers by flow cytometry and
immunocytochemistry. Western blotting was used to identify and quantify the molecular components of these cell-cell junctions
in human valve ICs and compared with expression in smooth muscle and fibroblast cell types. N-cadherin and desmoglein were
weakly detected on a low percentage of ICs, and the other classical cadherins were not detected. α- and β-catenin, but not
γ-catenin, were expressed at equivalent levels by all valve ICs. Valve ICs did not express connexin-32 and-40; however, connexin-26
and-43 were equally expressed by a low percentage of ICs, demonstrating cell surface and cytoplasmic expression, and connexin-45
was weakly expressed. The other cell types also expressed N-cadherin, α- and β-catenin, desmoglein and connexin-43. The expression
of these junctional molecules was predominantly by valve ICs on the inflow side of the valves. Human valve ICs have the ability
to communicate with other valve ICs and mediate cell-cell adhesion via N-cadherin, connexin-26 and-43, and desmoglein. The
junctions between valve ICs could support an interconnecting and coordinated cellular unit capable of controlling the functionality
of the valve. 相似文献
20.
We have previously shown that several gap junction uncouplers increase the uptake of glucose in astrocytes. The aim of the present work was to study whether the increase in glucose uptake was a consequence of the inhibition of gap junction communication and the purpose of this effect. Our results show that alpha-glycyrrhetinic acid and endothelin-1 increase the uptake of glucose in highly, but not in poorly, coupled astrocytes. This effect depended on connexin 43 levels and was abolished when the inhibition of gap junction communication was prevented by tolbutamide or ouabain. The inhibition of gap junctions increased the rate of glucose incorporation into DNA and RNA, which was inhibited by treatment with dehydroepiandrosterone, an inhibitor of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase, the regulatory enzyme of the pentose phosphate pathway. The inhibition of gap junctions significantly increased astrocyte proliferation, which was counteracted by tolbutamide. These effects were not observed in poorly coupled astrocytes expressing low levels of connexin 43. The increase in astrocyte proliferation caused by gap junction inhibition was prevented when either glucose uptake or the pentose phosphate pathway were inhibited. We conclude that the inhibition of gap junction communication induces astrocyte proliferation, resulting in an enhancement of glucose uptake and its utilization through the pentose phosphate pathway to provide ribose-5-phosphate for the synthesis of nucleic acids. 相似文献