首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.

Background

Apart from findings on both functional and motor asymmetries in captive aquatic mammals, only few studies have focused on lateralized behaviour of these species in the wild.

Methodology/Principal Findings

In this study we focused on lateralized visual behaviour by presenting wild striped dolphins with objects of different degrees of familiarity (fish, ball, toy). Surveys were conducted in the Gulf of Taranto, the northern Ionian Sea portion delimited by the Italian regions of Calabria, Basilicata and Apulia. After sighting striped dolphins from a research vessel, different stimuli were presented in a random order by a telescopic bar connected to the prow of the boat. The preferential use of the right/left monocular viewing during inspection of the stimuli was analysed.

Conclusion

Results clearly showed a monocular viewing preference with respect to the type of the stimulus employed. Due to the complete decussation of the optical nerves in dolphin brain our results reflected a different specialization of brain hemispheres for visual scanning processes confirming that in this species different stimuli evoked different patterns of eye use. A preferential use of the right eye (left hemisphere) during visual inspection of unfamiliar targets was observed supporting the hypothesis that, in dolphins, the organization of the functional neural structures which reflected cerebral asymmetries for visual object recognition could have been subjected to a deviation from the evolutionary line of most terrestrial vertebrates.  相似文献   

2.
The character and values of changes of the pulmonary and systemic hemodynamics following neurogenic stimuli application on the cardiovascular system were studied in acute experiments on the anesthetized cats. Vagus nerve stimulation reduced the heart rate and decreased myocardial contractility in result, right and left atrial pressure increased, whereas pulmonary pressure and flow, venous return, cardiac output and venous return decreased. Pulmonary pressure reached maximal level and returned to the initial value earlier than the pulmonary flow. On the contrary, pulmonary pressure, following neurogenic pressor stimuli, reached maximal level and returned to the initial value later than the pulmonary flow; the sign of the changes of the pulmonary pressure could be positive or negative, whereas pulmonary flow were always increased. The venous return did not change, and for this reason it could not cause the increasing of pulmonary flow which was elevated following increasing of the heart rate and myocardial contractility. The shifts of the pulmonary pressure were correlated with the pulmonary resistance those, which were increased after the stellate ganglion stimulation and decreased following carotid reflex; they did not change in case of sciatic nerve stimulation. The shifts of the pulmonary pressure did not depended on the decreased right and left atrial pressures. When the pulmonary flow was always increased, the cardiac output following electrical stimulation of the stellate ganglion and sciatic nerve was elevated, and it was decreased following carotid reflex, i. e. linear correlation between these parameters were not found. Pulmonary and systemic arterial pressure changes were more obvious in case of direct neurogenic stimuli application comparing with reflectory ones; in both cases, the positive chrono- and inotropic cardiac effects were similar.  相似文献   

3.
Patterns of growth in wild bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
A. J. Read    R. S. Wells    A. A. Hohn    M. D. Scott 《Journal of Zoology》1993,231(1):107-123
The growth of bottlenose dolphins is described from observations made during a capture release programme that has operated in coastal waters of the eastern Gulf of Mexico from 1970 to the present. Measurements of standard length, girth and body mass were recorded from 47 female and 49 male dolphins, some captured as many as nine times. Ages were known from approximate birth dates or estimated from counts of dentinal growth layers. In all three measurements. females grew at a faster initial rate than males, but reached asymptotic size at an earlier age. This extended period of growth in males resulted in significant sexual dimorphism in length, girth and mass at physical maturity. The growth of both sexes was well described by three-parameter Gompertz models using either cross-sectional data or a mixture of longitudinal and cross-sectional data. There was considerable variation in size-at-age for both sexes in all year classes. Residuals of size measurements were used to derive measures of relative size for individual dolphins; most dolphins demonstrated little ontogenetic change in relative size. Body mass was adequately predicted by multiple regression equations that incorporated both length and girth as independent variables.  相似文献   

4.
Motor alimentary conditioned reflex was elaborated in Wistar line rats to presentation of various pairs of figures, those of larger area serving as positive signals. In tests on animals with intact brain and with unilateral hemispheric cortical inactivation, new pairs of figures were used besides basic stimuli. The level of analysis in testing experiment was lower than normal: by 5.6% at intact brain, by 16.4%--at right hemisphere inactivation, by 36.4%--at left hemisphere inactivation. Elimination of the left hemisphere produced prolongation of the conditioned reaction time, signs of frustration and aftereffect. In conditions of right hemisphere inactivation the level of discrimination of figures depended on their specific properties and the side of presentation. Conclusion is made on dominating role of the left hemisphere in analysis of abstract and concrete characteristics of visual stimuli, in control of temporal, spatial and emotional characteristics of reactions in situations when considerable abstraction and distinguishing of generalizing invariant sign "more-less" is necessary for elaboration of the conditioned reflex.  相似文献   

5.
Marijuana, smoked at moderate doses, produced a differential impairment of the reaction times of right-handed males to pictorial stimuli presented to the left and right cerebral hemispheres. After smoking marijuana responses to pictorial stimuli presented to the right hemisphere were slowed significantly less than to the left hemisphere. Responses to verbal stimuli (trigrams) were slowd equally in both hemispheres, preserving an initial left hemisphere superiority for this material. This suggests that marijuana may differentially change the processing speed or relative dominance of man's two cerebral hemispheres, depending on the nature of the material being processed.  相似文献   

6.
In conditions of tachistoscopic presentation of visual stimuli, healthy (male and female) right-handed subjects carried out a paired comparison of the stimuli presented unilaterally and in the center of the visual field. In case of recognition of images of words and objects, the number of correct answers and motor reaction time usually did not significantly differ at two interstimuli intervals (1 and 10 s). In comparing images of faces, there also were no differences by the number of reactions, and the reaction time was less at the intervals of 1 s. The left hemisphere dominated at the identification of words and female faces, the right one--at the recognition of male faces. When the right visual field was stimulated images of various classes were recognized more differentially than at the stimulation of the left visual field. The male subjects had more prominent interhemispheric differences than the females. The increase of the interstimuli interval from 1 to 10 s brought to a weakening of the functional interhemispheric asymmetry and decreasing of the differences between the male and female subjects. The obtained data show that in the processes connected with short-time memory, functional interhemispheric asymmetry is basically formed at the initial stages of the information processing.  相似文献   

7.
In order to study gender differences in development of the latent inhibition (LI) and the influence of laterality (hemisphere) factor, two groups of subjects were examined. In the preexposure phase, subjects of the control group were presented with a pair of geometric figures, which appeared either in the right or in the left visual hemifield (at random) with the exposure time of 80 ms. A subject had to compare the figures in a pair and to press a button in case of their identity. In the preexposure (experimental) group, the subject solved the same task, but half of them was presented with the same set of stimuli which appeared in the right hemifield, another half of subjects received these stimuli in the left hemifield. Simultaneously with the target stimuli, the opposite hemifield was stimulated with a nonattended symbol "[symbol: see text]". In the test phase, all the subjects (both of the control and experimental groups) had to respond to the symbol "[symbol: see text]" (previously nonattended in the experimental group), which appeared in part of the trials, by pressing the button. Pairs of stimuli which had been target ones in the preexposure phase, became masking stimuli in the test phase. Correct responses were reinforced by presentation of a special sound. The number of trials required for acquisition of the instrumental contingency served as an index of the LI. Analysis of variance showed that significant strong effect of the LI was observed only in men (comparison between the control and preexposure groups), while there was no significant difference between women of the control and preexposure groups. There was no laterality effect. Gender differences and hemispheric asymmetry in the LI organization are discussed.  相似文献   

8.
Asymmetry of movement direction was found in Wistar rats at establishing of motor alimentary conditioned reflex to simultaneously presented visual stimuli. In the course of learning the asymmetry weakened on the whole, but some individuals retained right- or left side preference. The analysis of asymmetry change before and after unilateral cortical inactivation revealed a special role of right hemisphere influences for the formation of right-side preference and of the left hemisphere--for the choice of the left direction. The lack of asymmetry was observed at the presence of the influences from the left hemisphere cortex depressing ipsilateral nigro-striate system and activating the contralateral one. Influences of the cortex of both hemispheres reduce the absolute value of the asymmetry coefficient; the left hemisphere has a special significance for manifestation of temporal asymmetry parameters. Photic interference is a factor modulating the asymmetry. It reduces the right hemisphere activity more than that of the left one; it intensifies right hemisphere influences, contributes to the involvement of the transcallosal conduction channel in the formation of spatial-motor asymmetry.  相似文献   

9.
Several terrestrial animals and delphinids manipulate objects in a tactile manner, using parts of their bodies, such as their mouths or hands. In this paper, we report that bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) manipulate objects not by direct bodily contact, but by spontaneous water flow. Three of four dolphins at Suma Aqualife Park performed object manipulation with food. The typical sequence of object manipulation consisted of a three step procedure. First, the dolphins released the object from the sides of their mouths while assuming a head-down posture near the floor. They then manipulated the object around their mouths and caught it. Finally, they ceased to engage in their head-down posture and started to swim. When the dolphins moved the object, they used the water current in the pool or moved their head. These results showed that dolphins manipulate objects using movements that do not directly involve contact between a body part and the object. In the event the dolphins dropped the object on the floor, they lifted it by making water flow in one of three methods: opening and closing their mouths repeatedly, moving their heads lengthwise, or making circular head motions. This result suggests that bottlenose dolphins spontaneously change their environment to manipulate objects. The reason why aquatic animals like dolphins do object manipulation by changing their environment but terrestrial animals do not may be that the viscosity of the aquatic environment is much higher than it is in terrestrial environments. This is the first report thus far of any non-human mammal engaging in object manipulation using several methods to change their environment.  相似文献   

10.
A common method for testing preference for objects is to determine which of a pair of objects is approached first in a paired-choice paradigm. In comparison, many studies of preference for environmental enrichment (EE) devices have used paradigms in which total time spent with each of a pair of objects is used to determine preference. While each of these paradigms gives a specific measure of the preference for one object in comparison to another, neither method allows comparisons between multiple objects simultaneously. Since it is possible that several EE objects would be placed in a cage together to improve animal welfare, it is important to determine measures for rats’ preferences in conditions that mimic this potential home cage environment. While it would be predicted that each type of measure would produce similar rankings of objects, this has never been tested empirically. In this study, we compared two paradigms: EE objects were either presented in pairs (paired-choice comparison) or four objects were presented simultaneously (simultaneous presentation comparison). We used frequency of first interaction and time spent with each object to rank the objects in the paired-choice experiment, and time spent with each object to rank the objects in the simultaneous presentation experiment. We also considered the behaviours elicited by the objects to determine if these might be contributing to object preference. We demonstrated that object ranking based on time spent with objects from the paired-choice experiment predicted object ranking in the simultaneous presentation experiment. Additionally, we confirmed that behaviours elicited were an important determinant of time spent with an object. This provides convergent evidence that both paired choice and simultaneous comparisons provide valid measures of preference for EE objects in rats.  相似文献   

11.
Analyzing cerebral asymmetries in various species helps in understanding brain organization. The left and right sides of the brain (lateralization) are involved in different cognitive and sensory functions. This study focuses on dolphin visual lateralization as expressed by spontaneous eye preference when performing a complex cognitive task; we examine lateralization when processing different visual stimuli displayed on an underwater touch-screen (two-dimensional figures, three-dimensional figures and dolphin/human video sequences). Three female bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) were submitted to a 2-, 3- or 4-, choice visual/auditory discrimination problem, without any food reward: the subjects had to correctly match visual and acoustic stimuli together. In order to visualize and to touch the underwater target, the dolphins had to come close to the touch-screen and to position themselves using monocular vision (left or right eye) and/or binocular naso-ventral vision. The results showed an ability to associate simple visual forms and auditory information using an underwater touch-screen. Moreover, the subjects showed a spontaneous tendency to use monocular vision. Contrary to previous findings, our results did not clearly demonstrate right eye preference in spontaneous choice. However, the individuals' scores of correct answers were correlated with right eye vision, demonstrating the advantage of this visual field in visual information processing and suggesting a left hemispheric dominance. We also demonstrated that the nature of the presented visual stimulus does not seem to have any influence on the animals' monocular vision choice.  相似文献   

12.
Two bottlenosed dolphins taught to classify pairs of three-dimensional objects as either same or different were tested with novel stimulus sets to determine how well their classification abilities would generalize. Both dolphins were immediately able to classify novel pairs of planar objects, differing only in shape, as same or different. When tested on sets of three objects consisting of either all different objects or of two identical objects and one different object, both dolphins proved to be able to classify 'all different' sets as different and 'not all different' sets as same, at levels significantly above chance. These data suggest that dolphins can use knowledge about similarity-based classification strategies gained from previous training to perform successfully in a variety of novel same-different classification tasks. Visual classificatory abilities of dolphins appear to be comparable to those that have been demonstrated in primates.  相似文献   

13.
Most experimental paradigms to study visual cognition in humans and non-human species are based on discrimination tasks involving the choice between two or more visual stimuli. To this end, different types of stimuli and procedures for stimuli presentation are used, which highlights the necessity to compare data obtained with different methods. The present study assessed whether, and to what extent, capuchin monkeys’ ability to solve a size discrimination problem is influenced by the type of procedure used to present the problem. Capuchins’ ability to generalise knowledge across different tasks was also evaluated. We trained eight adult tufted capuchin monkeys to select the larger of two stimuli of the same shape and different sizes by using pairs of food items (Experiment 1), computer images (Experiment 1) and objects (Experiment 2). Our results indicated that monkeys achieved the learning criterion faster with food stimuli compared to both images and objects. They also required consistently fewer trials with objects than with images. Moreover, female capuchins had higher levels of acquisition accuracy with food stimuli than with images. Finally, capuchins did not immediately transfer the solution of the problem acquired in one task condition to the other conditions. Overall, these findings suggest that – even in relatively simple visual discrimination problems where a single perceptual dimension (i.e., size) has to be judged – learning speed strongly depends on the mode of presentation.  相似文献   

14.
The semiwild beach-feeding bottlenose dolphins ( Tursiops aduncus ) of Monkey Mia, Western Australia, provide an unparalleled opportunity to examine prey preference of this species. In a series of binary-choice feeding experiments, we took advantage of the animals' willingness to be fed by hand, to explore their preferences for fish species, size, and state (freshly caught or previously frozen). At the end of each beach visit, each dolphin was provided with a pair of fish but allowed to eat only the first one chosen. The dolphins appeared indifferent among the three species of fish offered to them (yellowtail trumpeter, Amniataba caudovittatus ; striped trumpeter, Pelates sexlineatus ; and western butterfish, Pentapodus vitta ), which were of similar body form and matched for mass. Overall, the dolphins showed a slight preference for the larger of two yellowtail trumpeter offered, suggesting the capability for rational choice when there was a basis for it (most likely energy in this case), although there was considerable individual variation. The dolphins did not distinguish between freshly caught and previously frozen yellowtail. The methodology we describe can be used to generate data of potential value for understanding food and habitat selection of wild dolphins, and for modifying management practices for semiwild dolphins at Monkey Mia and elsewhere.  相似文献   

15.
Heart rate, EEG, and motor responses were recorded following presentation of a series of 6–10 sound stimuli (2.5-s tones of 1000, 4000, and 250 Hz, 70 dB, interstimulus intervals 18–25 s) in neonates aged 9 to 22 weeks during stage 2–3 sleep. The infants (17 of 19) revealed heart rate (HR) changes in response to tone stimuli that consisted in an expanded form of three phases: (1) short-latency (at 1 s after tone presentation) HR deceleration, (2) HR acceleration with a maximum at 3–5 s, and (3) late HR deceleration at 6–9 s of the poststimulus interval. The occurrence rate of the first two phases of cardiac response is relatively constant during a series of stimuli, whereas the likelihood of late HR deceleration is the highest following the first tone presentation and decreases significantly when the stimulus is repeated. Differences in the dynamics and statistical analysis allow a relative independence of all the three response phases to be suggested. The HR acceleration phase is dramatically enhanced in association with the motor response elicited by the sound stimulus. The late HR deceleration phase occurs not only after the first presentation of stimuli, but also when they are repeated if they evoke EEG reaction (vertex potentials) in response to both the beginning and end of the tone sound. Possible mechanisms of the three phases of poststimulus HR changes are: the vagal cardiac reflex associated with the acoustic (adaptive) reflex, activation of sympathetic efferents in combination with the startle reflex, and secondary vagal deceleration of sinus rhythm likely to be associated with the processes of perception (detection) of a “novel” stimulus and to serve as an indirect sign of an orienting reaction.  相似文献   

16.
Pattern of discharges and interhemispheric interaction of parietal neurons (somatosensory presentation of the ear) were compared in interstimulus intervals before active shake-off and passive freezing reactions at stimuli after elaboration defensive avoid reflex. Before passive reactions in contrast to active right-side influences on left-side neurons were reinforced with delay to 100 ms, it result in appearence of asymmetry in interhemispheric interaction with right-side dominance. Before passive reactions intensity of delta-frequencies was increased in impulsation of single neurons and in interaction of cells pair. On this evidence the pattern of interhemispheric asymmetry before stimuli can be one of a number of factors, determinant active or passive form of behavioral reactions and also reflective of activation level of neocortex.  相似文献   

17.
Multi-unit sympathetic activity was recorded at elbow level from median nerve fascicles supplying glabrous skin of the left hand in five healthy subjects. The resultant vasomotor responses accompanying the neural activity were monitored by simultaneous recordings of skin blood flow using the laser doppler method and skin temperature in the innervation zones. No significant change in sympathetic activity was observed during handgrip exercise of the right hand under a constant gripping force of 2 kg. Subjects maintained the same gripping force of the right hand during exposure in random order to local vibration and/or noise, each type of exposure lasting 5 min with intervals of 20 min. A two-peaked significant increase in outflow from sympathetic nerves was observed during local exposure of the right hand to vibration with a frequency of 60 Hz and an acceleration of 50 m.s-2, followed by a postexposure period which revealed a relative suppression of sympathetic nerve activity and a significant increase in blood flow. Noise at 100 dB(A) showed only an initial effect on skin sympathetic nerve activity (SSA), whereas when combined with local vibration at 60 Hz, a pronounced increase in neural activity was noticed, indicating a combined effect of vibration and noise. These results from direct recordings of SSA suggest a sympathetic vasomotor reflex mechanism triggered by local vibration stimuli to the hand.  相似文献   

18.
Recognition of shape of natural objects was studied during lateralized tachistoscopic presentation and different degree of noise-like ("rain drops") masking in 15 healthy subjects. Two sets of figures were used: halftone and contour ones. In all masking conditions, the mean group data showed a significantly better recognition of contour images by the left hemisphere as compared to the right hemisphere. The probability of correct response decreased with increase in the degree of masking. Contour figures were recognized significantly better than halftone figures. Gender differences in recognition were revealed. Male subjects displayed no hemispheric preference in recognition of both types of stimuli in both masking conditions. Possible neurophysiological mechanisms and functional significance of the findings are discussed.  相似文献   

19.
Healthy subjects (n = 88) were asked to passively visualize positive and passive emotiogenic visual stimuli and also stimuli with a neutral emotional content. Images of the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) were used. Amplitude/time characteristics of the components of evoked EEG potentials (EPs), P1, N1, P2, N2, and P3 and topographic distribution of the latter components were analyzed. The latencies, amplitudes, and topography of the EP waves induced by presentation of positive and negative stimuli were found to be different from the respective values for the EPs induced by neutral stimuli. The level and pattern of these differences typical of different EP components were dissimilar and depended on the sign of the emotions. Specificities related to the valency of an identified stimulus were observed within nearly all stages of processing of visual signals, for the negative stimuli, beginning from an early stage of sensory analysis corresponding to the development of wave Р1. The latencies of components Р1 in the case of presentation of emotiogenic negative stimuli and those of components N1, N2, and Р3 in the case of presentation of the stimuli of both valencies were shorter than the latencies observed at neutral stimuli. The amplitude of component N2 at perception of positive stimuli was, on average, lower, while the Р3 amplitude at perception of all emotiogenic stimuli was higher than in the case of presentation of neutral stimuli. The time dynamics of topographic peculiarities of processing of emotiogenic information were complicated. Activation of the left hemisphere was observed during the earliest stages of perception, while the right hemisphere was activated within the intermediate stages. Generalized activation of the cortex after the action of negative signals and dominance of the left hemisphere under conditions of presentation of positive stimuli were observed only within the final stages. As is supposed, emotiogenic stimuli possess a greater biological significance than neutral ones, and this is why the former attract visual attention first; they more intensely activate the respective cortical zones, and the corresponding visual information is processed more rapidly. The observed effects were more clearly expressed in the case of action of negative stimuli; these effects involved more extensive cortical zones. These facts are indicative of the higher intensity of activating influences of negative emotiogenic stimuli on neutral systems of the higher CNS structures.  相似文献   

20.
In a two-choice discrimination paradigm, a bottlenose dolphin discriminated relational dimensions between visual numerosity stimuli under monocular viewing conditions. After prior binocular acquisition of the task, two monocular test series with different number stimuli were conducted. In accordance with recent studies on visual lateralization in the bottlenose dolphin, our results revealed an overall advantage of the right visual field. Due to the complete decussation of the optic nerve fibers, this suggests a specialization of the left hemisphere for analysing relational features between stimuli as required in tests for numerical abilities. These processes are typically right hemisphere-based in other mammals (including humans) and birds. The present data provide further evidence for a general right visual field advantage in bottlenose dolphins for visual information processing. It is thus assumed that dolphins possess a unique functional architecture of their cerebral asymmetries.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号