首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Auditory motion aftereffects of approaching and withdrawing sound sources were investigated in the free field. The approaching and withdrawing of a sound source were simulated by means of differently directed changes in the amplitude of impulses of broadband noise (from 20 Hz to 20 kHz) through two loudspeakers placed 1.1 and 4.5 m away from the listener. Presentation of the adapting approaching and withdrawing stimuli changed the perception of test signals following them: a stationary test signal was perceived by listeners as moving in the direction opposite to one of the movement of the adapting stimulus, whereas a test stimulus slowly moving in same direction as the adapting signal was perceived as stationary. The specific features of the auditory aftereffect of signals moving in a radial direction were similar to those of sound sources moving in a horizontal plane.  相似文献   

2.
Human long-latency auditory evoked potentials were studied during simulation with variable-amplitude pulse sequences from a sound source moving to and from the subject. The N1 peak parameters were shown to depend on an accurate estimate of the direction of the change in the distance to the sound source. Differences in the processing of signals that simulated the approaching and/or distancing of the sound source were found in the N1 and P2 component parameters of on- and off-responses as was a more pronounced long negative potential shift in the evoked response to the approaching source as compared to the distancing source.  相似文献   

3.
Auditory aftereffects were evaluated after short adaptation to radial sound source motion with different velocities. Approach and withdrawal of the sound source were simulated by means of rhythmical noise (from 20 Hz to 20 kHz) impulse sequences with an arising or diminishing amplitude. They were presented to an anechoic chamber through two loudspeakers placed at 1.1 and 4.5 m from the listener. The adapting stimulus velocities were 0.68, 3.43, 6.92, and 9.97 m/s with an adaptation duration of 5 s. At all motion velocities, the aftereffect manifested itself in divergence of psychometric functions upon approaching and withdrawing of adaptors. The direction of function displacements was opposite to that of the adaptor motion. Three parameters reflecting alteration of perception after motion adaptation were determined and compared with control data: the evaluation of stationary test stimuli; the velocity of moving test signal at the point of subjective equality (perceptually unmoving point); and the percentage of responses after averaging over all test signals. These parameters of auditory radial motion aftereffect similarly changed with the adaptor velocity. They demonstrated a significant effect at slow motion (0.68 and 3.43 m/s) and a small effect at a quick motion (6.92 and 9.97 m/s).  相似文献   

4.
Adaptation of listeners to approaching or receding sound stimuli continued for 5 s under free-field conditions. Motion of the adaptive and test sound stimuli was simulated by means of oppositely directed linear changes in the amplitude of the low- and high-frequency noises (0.05–1 and 3–20 kHz, respectively) from two stationary loudspeakers. In a group of eight subjects with normal hearing, the auditory motion after-effect of the approaching and receding sound stimuli was evaluated by integrated indices that characterized the shift of the psychometric curves in response to the test stimuli under various conditions of listening. The aftereffect occurs in the case when the spectral composition of the adaptive and test stimuli coincides. In response to the high-frequency stimuli, the effect of adaptation to both the approaching and receding sound stimuli was observed, while in response to the low-frequency stimuli, only the approach of stimuli caused an aftereffect. There was no radial motion aftereffect in the case of mismatching the spectral bands of the adaptive and test stimuli. Thus, the frequency selectivity was characteristic of the auditory aftereffect of adaptation to the approaching and receding sound stimuli.  相似文献   

5.
We experimentally demonstrated that tonal acoustic signals with a carrier frequency of 140–200 Hz had a repellent effect on male mosquitoes (Culicidae). Swarming males of Aedes diantaeus were concentrated in a small space near the auxiliary attracting sound source which simulated the flight sound of conspecific females (carrier frequency 280–320 Hz). Then, the resulting cluster of attracted mosquitoes was stimulated with test signals of variable amplitude and carrier frequency from a second loudspeaker. The direction of mosquito flight from the source of test sounds and a decrease in their number above the attracting sound source were used as the criteria of behavioral response. Pronounced avoidance responses (negative phonotaxis) of swarming mosquitoes were observed in the range of 140–200 Hz. Most of the mosquitoes left the area above the attracting sound source within one second after the onset of the test signal. Mosquitoes mostly flew up, sideways, and backwards in relation to the test acoustic vector. We presume that mosquitoes develop defensive behavior against attacking predatory insects based on analysis of auditory information. The range of negative phonotaxis is limited at higher frequencies by the spectrum of the flight sounds of conspecific females, and in the low frequency range, by the increasing level of atmospheric noise.  相似文献   

6.
7.
The work presents experimental data on certain changes in electrical responses of the auditory system's midbrain centre in a contraphasic binaural presentation of sound impulse series. Neuronal cortical activity is selective in respect to dynamic interaural changes of signals' phasic spectre which may serve as a basis for the mechanisms of localising a moving source of sound. Human auditory evoked potentials reveal a manifestation of memorizing the auditory image movement direction as shown by appearance of stimuli deviant from standard mismatch negativity.  相似文献   

8.
We investigated the acoustical information present in the field of arbitrary sound sources which may provide direction and distance to the source from a local reading of the sound field parameters. If the effects of reflections are negligible, the particle acceleration is directed radially at the instant of sound pressure nulls. The spectral relation between the radial component of the particle aceleration and the sound pressure is characterized by a critical frequency where a sharp transition occurs in the amplitude ratio and the phase relation of these variables. The critical frequency depends on the distance to the source and depends little on the source type (mono-, di- or quadrupole). Thus, a local reading of the particle acceleration and the sound pressure is in principle sufficient to localize the sound source in three dimensions. Fish might use this kind of information for acoustic orientation.  相似文献   

9.
The existence of space-specific differences in auditory aftereffects has been tested under short-term (5 s) exposure to broadband noise (20–20000 Hz). Adapting stimuli were emitted as constant-amplitude noise sequences. Test stimuli could be of constant and changing amplitude: increasing amplitude of noise pulses in a sequence mimicked an approaching sound source, whereas a decrease in amplitude was perceived as withdrawal. The experiments were done in an anechoic chamber. Auditory aftereffects were assessed under the following conditions: (a) adapting and test stimuli were emitted through a loudspeaker mounted at a distance of 1.1 m from the listener (i.e., subjectively near); (b) both stimuli were emitted from a distance of 4.5 m (subjectively far); (c) adapting and test stimuli were emitted from different distances. The results showed that the characteristics of perception of the imitated sound source motion were similar in proximity and remoteness, which was observed both in the control (without adaptation) and after adaptation to noise. In the absence of adaptation, the psychophysical curves were asymmetrical: the listeners reported approaching of test stimuli more often for both spatial domains. However, the overestimation of test stimuli as drawing closer was more pronounced when they were emitted from the distance of 1.1 m, i.e., from near the listener. After the adaptation to noise, the aftereffects showed spatial specificity and were observed only when adapting and test stimuli belonged to the same spatial domain. These aftereffects were similar in their pattern and strength both in proximity and remoteness: the listeners reported withdrawal of test stimuli more frequently as compared with the control. As a result of these aftereffects, the symmetry of psychometric curves was restored, and the estimation of the direction of sound source motion in the experiment became equiprobable.  相似文献   

10.
The auditory organs of the cricket which are situated in the front legs are joined together by a large transverse trachea which decisively influences their directional characteristics. The transverse trachea is medially divided by a septum. The importance of this septum for the localization of a sound source was tested by means of behavioural experiments in which the phonotactic movements of intact Gryllus bimaculatus females were compared quantitatively with those of the same specimen after perforation of the septum. The septal perforation does not noticeably influence locomotion in the absence of acoustic stimuli but selectively changes essential characteristics of phono taxis: 1) The animals walk in less straight lines. The oscillations around the mean course, typical of phonotaxis, are increased in amplitude, while the frequency decreases. 2) Course deviations from the direction of the sound source become more pronounced. 3) The threshold for phonotaxis is raised by about 10 dB. 4) Both the speed at which the animals walk and the proportion of time during which they are mobile are reduced. The results are discussed in relation to the role of the septum in the mechanism of sound localization, and with regard to its possible importance for the recognition of acoustic patterns.  相似文献   

11.
In everyday life, we need a capacity to flexibly shift attention between alternative sound sources. However, relatively little work has been done to elucidate the mechanisms of attention shifting in the auditory domain. Here, we used a mixed event-related/sparse-sampling fMRI approach to investigate this essential cognitive function. In each 10-sec trial, subjects were instructed to wait for an auditory "cue" signaling the location where a subsequent "target" sound was likely to be presented. The target was occasionally replaced by an unexpected "novel" sound in the uncued ear, to trigger involuntary attention shifting. To maximize the attention effects, cues, targets, and novels were embedded within dichotic 800-Hz vs. 1500-Hz pure-tone "standard" trains. The sound of clustered fMRI acquisition (starting at t?=?7.82 sec) served as a controlled trial-end signal. Our approach revealed notable activation differences between the conditions. Cued voluntary attention shifting activated the superior intra--parietal sulcus (IPS), whereas novelty-triggered involuntary orienting activated the inferior IPS and certain subareas of the precuneus. Clearly more widespread activations were observed during voluntary than involuntary orienting in the premotor cortex, including the frontal eye fields. Moreover, we found -evidence for a frontoinsular-cingular attentional control network, consisting of the anterior insula, inferior frontal cortex, and medial frontal cortices, which were activated during both target discrimination and voluntary attention shifting. Finally, novels and targets activated much wider areas of superior temporal auditory cortices than shifting cues.  相似文献   

12.
Perception of approaching and withdrawing sound sources and their action on auditory aftereffects were studied in the free field. Motion of adapting stimuli was mimicked in two ways: (1) simultaneous opposite changes of amplitude of broadband noise impulses at two loudspeakers placed at 1.1 and 4.5 m from the listener; (2) an increase or a decrease of amplitude of broadband noise impulses in only one loudspeaker, the nearer or the remote one. Motion of test stimuli was mimicked in the former way. Listeners determined direction of the test stimuli motion without any adaptation (control) or after adaptation to stationary, slowly moving (with an amplitude change of 2 dB) and rapidly moving (amplitude change of 12 dB) stimuli. Percentages of “withdrawal” reports were used for construction of psychometric curves. Three phenomena of auditory perception were observed. In the absence of adaptation, a growing-louder effect was revealed, i.e., listeners reported more frequently the test sounds as the approaching ones. Once adapted to stationary or slowly moving stimuli, listeners showed a location-dependent aftereffect. Test stimuli were reported as withdrawing more often as compared with control. The effect was associated with the previous one and was weaker when the distance to the loudspeaker producing adapting stimuli was greater. After adaptation to rapidly moving stimuli, a motion aftereffect was revealed. In this case, listeners reported a direction of test stimuli motion as being opposite to that of adapting stimuli. The motion aftereffect was more pronounced when the adapting stimuli motion was mimicked in the former way, as this method allows estimation of their trajectory. There was no relationship between the motion aftereffect and the growing-louder effect, whichever way the adapting stimuli were produced. There was observed a tendency for reduction of aftereffects of approaching and for intensification of aftereffects of withdrawal with growing distance from source of adapting stimuli.  相似文献   

13.
Acoustic harassment and deterrent devices have become increasingly popular mitigation tools for negotiating the impacts of marine mammals on fisheries. The rationale for their variable effectiveness remains unexplained, but high variability in the surrounding acoustic field may be relevant. In the present study, the sound fields of one acoustic harassment device and three acoustic deterrent devices were measured at three study sites along the Scandinavian coast. Superimposed onto an overall trend of decreasing sound exposure levels with increasing range were large local variations in the sound level for all sources in each of the environments. This variability was likely caused by source directionality, inter-ping source level variation and multipath interference. Rapid and unpredictable variations in the sound level as a function of range deviated from expectations derived from spherical and cylindrical spreading models and conflicted with the classic concept of concentric zones of increasing disturbance with decreasing range. Under such conditions, animals may encounter difficulties when trying to determine the direction to and location of a sound source, which may complicate or jeopardize avoidance responses.  相似文献   

14.
The formation of a defensive conditioned reflex to sound has been studied in rabbits raised from birth up to 30 days of life in dark. It was shown that, as compared with control animals of the same age, elaboration of reflex to sound takes place in them in shorter times periods and with less pairings. This corresponds to changes in electrographic manifestations of conditioning: increased amplitude and reduced peak latency of evoked potentials to acoustic stimuli in the auditory and sensorimotor cortical zones. The data obtained testify to enahcned functional activity of the auditory cortex, apparently due to a compensatory enhancement of impulse activity coming from the intact receptors of the auditory apparatus. It has been assumed that the observed functional changes appearing in the cortical end of the signal analyser (auditory zone); in response to sound, following visual deprivation, are a consequence of an early nature training of synaptic structures with regard to perceptionof impulses of acoustic modality.  相似文献   

15.
The significant role of inertial features of the auditory system in localization of moving sound sources is presented. This inertia requires some time of observation about the movement in space of the auditory signals. Slower time of estimation of the localizing features of the moving sound source (as comparing with the unmoving one) is followed by the appearance of new important possibility of the auditory system, namely in perception and analysis of the parameters of moving sound sources. Meanwhile these inertial features are manifested by another significant change in the activity of the auditory system. After the cessation of the auditory signals we observed some aftereffects which manifested that the subjective perceived trajectory of the moving sound source is lengthening toward the direction of the existing movement even when the auditory signal is switched off. Stated above can serve as an evidence of presence in the auditory system some predictive possibilities concerning the direction of moving sound sources. This feature is essential from biological point of view for an adequate organisation of behaviour of the subjects.  相似文献   

16.
Although the use of sounds in spatial orientation is widespread among animals, only a few groups advanced such specific adaptations as echolocation. In contrast, practically all animals and night-active species in particular, must occasionally orient themselves relative to invisible but audible objects such as a hidden rival or predator. In this study, I would like to determine the impact of locating which involves the use of acoustic parameters of sender’s vocalisations by receivers and changes of positions and triangulation of sender’s vocalisations by receivers in estimating the distance to the sender during night-time territorial interactions of the corncrake (Crex crex). Males were subjected to two kinds of stimuli: approaching one, imitating the change of the distance of the calling intruder toward the focal male while keeping the direction constant, or stationary stimuli, involving acoustic stimulation with no motion. Although males subjected to approaching stimulation moved longer distances, in both stimuli groups, males moved predominantly toward or out of the playback speaker, and only occasionally made sideway movements. However, the results gave no evidence of corncrakes moving specifically in order to locate the source of the sound; they suggest that males moved toward or away from the already located sound. The fact that males moved longer distances in response to approaching than stationary stimuli indicates that they were able to perceive the change of the distance to the playback speaker based only on structural parameters or amplitude of the calls played.  相似文献   

17.
Spike timing is precise in the auditory system and it has been argued that it conveys information about auditory stimuli, in particular about the location of a sound source. However, beyond simple time differences, the way in which neurons might extract this information is unclear and the potential computational advantages are unknown. The computational difficulty of this task for an animal is to locate the source of an unexpected sound from two monaural signals that are highly dependent on the unknown source signal. In neuron models consisting of spectro-temporal filtering and spiking nonlinearity, we found that the binaural structure induced by spatialized sounds is mapped to synchrony patterns that depend on source location rather than on source signal. Location-specific synchrony patterns would then result in the activation of location-specific assemblies of postsynaptic neurons. We designed a spiking neuron model which exploited this principle to locate a variety of sound sources in a virtual acoustic environment using measured human head-related transfer functions. The model was able to accurately estimate the location of previously unknown sounds in both azimuth and elevation (including front/back discrimination) in a known acoustic environment. We found that multiple representations of different acoustic environments could coexist as sets of overlapping neural assemblies which could be associated with spatial locations by Hebbian learning. The model demonstrates the computational relevance of relative spike timing to extract spatial information about sources independently of the source signal.  相似文献   

18.
The representation of alternative conspecific acoustic signals in the responses of a pair of local interneurons of the bushcricket Tettigonia viridissima was studied with variation in intensity and the direction of sound signals. The results suggest that the auditory world of the bushcricket is rather sharply divided into two azimuthal hemispheres, with signals arriving from any direction within one hemisphere being predominantly represented in the discharge of neurons of this side of the auditory pathway. In addition, each pathway also selects for the most intense of several alternative sounds. A low-intensity signal at 45 dB sound pressure level is quite effective when presented alone, but completely suppressed when given simultaneously with another signal at 60 dB sound pressure level. In a series of intracellular experiments the synaptic nature of the intensity-dependent suppression of competitive signals was investigated in a number of interneurons. The underlying synaptic mechanism is based on a membrane hyperpolarisation with a time-constant in the order of 5–10 s. The significance of this mechanism for hearing in choruses, and for the evolution of acoustic signals and signalling behaviour is discussed. Accepted: 20 November 1999  相似文献   

19.

Background

Singing in songbirds is a complex, learned behavior which shares many parallels with human speech. The avian vocal organ (syrinx) has two potential sound sources, and each sound generator is under unilateral, ipsilateral neural control. Different songbird species vary in their use of bilateral or unilateral phonation (lateralized sound production) and rapid switching between left and right sound generation (interhemispheric switching of motor control). Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata domestica) have received considerable attention, because they rapidly modify their song in response to manipulations of auditory feedback. However, how the left and right sides of the syrinx contribute to acoustic control of song has not been studied.

Methodology

Three manipulations of lateralized syringeal control of sound production were conducted. First, unilateral syringeal muscular control was eliminated by resection of the left or right tracheosyringeal portion of the hypoglossal nerve, which provides neuromuscular innervation of the syrinx. Spectral and temporal features of song were compared before and after lateralized nerve injury. In a second experiment, either the left or right sound source was devoiced to confirm the role of each sound generator in the control of acoustic phonology. Third, air pressure was recorded before and after unilateral denervation to enable quantification of acoustic change within individual syllables following lateralized nerve resection.

Significance

These experiments demonstrate that the left sound source produces louder, higher frequency, lower entropy sounds, and the right sound generator produces lower amplitude, lower frequency, higher entropy sounds. The bilateral division of labor is complex and the frequency specialization is the opposite pattern observed in most songbirds. Further, there is evidence for rapid interhemispheric switching during song production. Lateralized control of song production in Bengalese finches may enhance acoustic complexity of song and facilitate the rapid modification of sound production following manipulations of auditory feedback.  相似文献   

20.
Selective attention is the mechanism that allows focusing one’s attention on a particular stimulus while filtering out a range of other stimuli, for instance, on a single conversation in a noisy room. Attending to one sound source rather than another changes activity in the human auditory cortex, but it is unclear whether attention to different acoustic features, such as voice pitch and speaker location, modulates subcortical activity. Studies using a dichotic listening paradigm indicated that auditory brainstem processing may be modulated by the direction of attention. We investigated whether endogenous selective attention to one of two speech signals affects amplitude and phase locking in auditory brainstem responses when the signals were either discriminable by frequency content alone, or by frequency content and spatial location. Frequency-following responses to the speech sounds were significantly modulated in both conditions. The modulation was specific to the task-relevant frequency band. The effect was stronger when both frequency and spatial information were available. Patterns of response were variable between participants, and were correlated with psychophysical discriminability of the stimuli, suggesting that the modulation was biologically relevant. Our results demonstrate that auditory brainstem responses are susceptible to efferent modulation related to behavioral goals. Furthermore they suggest that mechanisms of selective attention actively shape activity at early subcortical processing stages according to task relevance and based on frequency and spatial cues.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号