首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Hysterectomized-ovariectomized virgin rats were tested for maternal behavior following treatment with 100 μg/kg EB immediately at surgery and either oil, 0.5 or 5.0 mg progesterone either 0, 24 or 44 hr following surgery. Stimulus pups were presented 48 hr postoperatively which is counted as Day 0 of testing. EB + oil-treated females displayed short-latency maternal behavior beginning on Day 0. The injection of 5.0 mg progesterone at 0, 24, or 44 hr significantly inhibited the onset of maternal care while the effect of the lower dose of progesterone depended upon the timing of its administration in relation to that of EB. At a dose of 0.5 mg, progesterone given 24 hr following EB, inhibited the appearance of maternal behavior but had no effect given at 44 hr, and resulted in only a partial delay when given at the same time as the EB. Possible mechanisms by which progesterone interfered with the display of maternal behavior were discussed.  相似文献   

2.
Two ovarian hormone regimens reported to induce rapid-onset maternal behavior (MB) in maternally naive virgin, ovariectomized Sprague-Dawley (SD) albino rats (R. S. Bridges, 1984, Endocrinology 114, 930-940; A. L. Giordano, 1987, Doctoral Dissertation, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ) were assessed in Long-Evans (LE) hooded rats, a strain which tends to be less maternally responsive in various situations dissociated from parturition. The combination of sufficiently high and long-lasting treatments with estradiol (E, 10-mm Silastic capsule, sc, on Day 1) and progesterone (P, 3 x 30-mm Silastic capsules, Days 3-13) resulted in a mean MB onset latency (after pup presentation on Day 14) of 1.8 days. In contrast, no-hormone or P-only controls had latencies of about 5.5 days. However, the E + P combination was completely ineffective if the E capsule was withdrawn along with the P capsules, unlike the case for SD rats. Also in contrast to the albinos, E alone was ineffective, while E treatment following P withdrawal was only partially effective. The most efficacious regimen, which included a P treatment (injections of 4 mg/day, Days 3-12 or 3-15) known to maintain pregnancy in ovariectomized rats, resulted in mean latencies of less than or equal to 1 day; 39% overall displayed MB rapidly, i.e., retrieval within 15 min of exposure to pups and crouching by 3 hr, and 89% became maternal by the next day. With this regimen, neither duration of 4 mg/day P treatment (10 or 13 days) nor hysterectomy 2 days before testing affected MB latencies. Thus, the essential features of the previously reported ovarian hormone regimens for induction of short-latency MB are efficacious in LE rats, but the hormonal requirements in this strain seem to be more precise. Factors which might contribute to an even higher percentage maternal on the first day of pup exposure are considered.  相似文献   

3.
The ability to care for the young is innate and readily displayed by postpartum females after delivery to ensure offspring survival. Upon pup exposure, rodent virgin (nulliparous) females also develop parental behavior that over time becomes displayed at levels equivalent to parenting mothers. Although maternal behavior in postpartum females and the associated neurocircuits are well characterized, the neural mechanisms underlying the acquisition of maternal behavior without prior experience remain poorly understood. Here, we show that the development of maternal care behavior in response to first‐time pup exposure in virgin females is initiated by the activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). ACC activity is dependent on feedback excitation by Vglut2+/Galanin+ neurons of the centrolateral nucleus of the thalamus (CL), with their activity sufficient to display parenting behaviors. Accordingly, acute bidirectional chemogenetic manipulation of neuronal activity in the ACC facilitates or impairs the attainment of maternal behavior, exclusively in virgin females. These results reveal an ACC‐CL neurocircuit as an accessory loop in virgin females for the initiation of maternal care upon first‐time exposure to pups.  相似文献   

4.
Prolonged exposure to prolactin (Prl) or to ectopic pituitary grafts that secrete Prl has been shown to stimulate maternal behavior in steroid-treated, hypophysectomized female rats. Since Prl levels in the blood of pregnant rats increase beginning 2-3 days prepartum, it was of interest to determine whether acute Prl priming prior to exposure to rat young would also stimulate full maternal behavior. Hypophysectomized, ovariectomized nulliparous rats were assigned to one of three treatments: Group 1, prolonged Prl; Group 2, acute Prl; or Group 3, controls/no Prl. All groups were implanted with 3 X 30 mm progesterone (P)-filled Silastic capsules s.c. at the time of ovariectomy (ovx) on Treatment Day 1. After ovx, Group 1 rats (prolonged Prl) were injected twice daily with 0.5 mg Ovine (o) Prl throughout the course of the study. Group 2 (acute Prl) and 3 (controls/no Prl) females were injected with vehicle alone or noninjected from Day 1-10. On Day 11 of Treatment, P implants were removed from all rats and each female was given a 2 mm estradiol-17 beta (E2)-filled Silastic implant. Starting on Day 11, Group 2 females were injected twice daily with oPrl. Group 3 rats continued to receive vehicle only. Behavioral testing began on Day 12 and was conducted daily through Day 22. Prolonged Prl priming (Group 1) stimulated a rapid onset of all aspects of maternal behavior (latencies less than 1 day, all p less than 0.05-0.001 vs. Group 3 controls). Control latencies ranged from 4-10 days.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

5.
Maternal behavior of the sensitized virgin rat is affected by approach-avoidance systems as well as by hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which is also activated during stress. The present experiments investigated the effects of adrenalectomy and of varying corticosterone concentrations on the onset and expression of maternal behavior in sensitized virgin rats. In the first experiment, latency to onset of maternal behavior and time spent licking once maternal were positively related to endogenous levels of corticosterone. However, few rats showed licking. In the second experiment, virgin rats were adrenalectomized or given sham surgeries before being sensitized and being given 0, 25, 100, 300, or 500 microg/mL of corticosterone in their drinking water. In the third experiment, virgin rats were adrenalectomized or given sham surgeries and given either control or corticosterone time-release pellets after being sensitized. Maternal behavior was then tested. Adrenalectomy increased licking in the second experiment and time over pups in the third experiment. Corticosterone replacement reduced licking in the second experiment and both licking and time over pups in the third experiment. In conclusion, exogenous corticosterone had an inhibitory effect on the expression of maternal behavior in the sensitized virgin rat, unlike the facilitatory effect previously found in the postpartum rat.  相似文献   

6.
The present study investigated the effects of isolation rearing, through the artificial rearing paradigm (AR), on the hormonal induction of maternal behavior (MB) in female Sprague-Dawley rats. Between postnatal days (PND) 4 and 18, rat pups were raised either with their mothers (MR) or artificially, without their mothers (AR). As well, some of the AR pups were provided with additional maternal-like licking stimulation (AR-MAX) while the others were not given any additional stimulation (AR-MIN). At PND 60-100, AR (n = 28) and MR (n = 25) animals were ovariectomized (OVX). One week after the surgery, rats were either treated with a 2-week estrogen (E2) and progesterone (P) hormonal regimen or not treated with the hormone replacement. Maternal behavior testing with foster pups commenced 24 h following the removal of P treatment. Results demonstrated that MR animals showed increased pup licking and hover-crouching in comparison to AR animals and that hormonally primed groups became maternal more quickly than non-primed groups, regardless of the rearing history. There was also a significant interaction between the rearing condition (MR vs. AR) and hormonal treatment on the quality of maternal behavior exhibited. The highest level of licking and crouching was shown by the hormone-treated MR group. Mechanisms for these effects are discussed.  相似文献   

7.
《Hormones and behavior》2006,49(5):528-536
The present study investigated the effects of isolation rearing, through the artificial rearing paradigm (AR), on the hormonal induction of maternal behavior (MB) in female Sprague–Dawley rats. Between postnatal days (PND) 4 and 18, rat pups were raised either with their mothers (MR) or artificially, without their mothers (AR). As well, some of the AR pups were provided with additional maternal-like licking stimulation (AR-MAX) while the others were not given any additional stimulation (AR-MIN). At PND 60–100, AR (n = 28) and MR (n = 25) animals were ovariectomized (OVX). One week after the surgery, rats were either treated with a 2-week estrogen (E2) and progesterone (P) hormonal regimen (Bridges, R.S., 1984. A quantitative analysis of the roles of dosage, sequence, and duration of estradiol and progesterone exposure in the regulation of maternal behavior in the rat. Endocrinology 114, 930–940) or not treated with the hormone replacement. Maternal behavior testing with foster pups commenced 24 h following the removal of P treatment. Results demonstrated that MR animals showed increased pup licking and hover-crouching in comparison to AR animals and that hormonally primed groups became maternal more quickly than non-primed groups, regardless of the rearing history. There was also a significant interaction between the rearing condition (MR vs. AR) and hormonal treatment on the quality of maternal behavior exhibited. The highest level of licking and crouching was shown by the hormone-treated MR group. Mechanisms for these effects are discussed.  相似文献   

8.
Female rats subjected both to Caesarean section and intraperitoneal injections of saline either failed to behave maternally or showed abnormally long latencies to the onset of this behavior. This interference with the initiation of pup care was reversed either by adrenalectomy or the injection of dexamethasone, indicating adrenal mediation. Radioimmunossay of plasma progesterone revealed that progesterone was not the adrenal agent involved in the observed behavioral disruption.  相似文献   

9.
10.
The role of prolactin in maintaining the maternal behavior of the rat was investigated. Ergocornine, a drug that interferes with the release of prolactin from the anterior pituitary, was injected daily into postpartum females for a period of 21 days. Those receiving ergocornine did not differ significantly from control females on any measure of maternal behavior, the control females having received either ergocornine plus prolactin or simply the vehicle. Since both lactation and the decidual cell response to uterine traumatization were inhibited after ergocornine, and since each were at least partially reversed by exogenous prolactin, it was evident that ergocornine interfered successfully with the postpartum release of prolactin. That at the same time maternal behavior was unaffected, supports the conclusion that prolactin is not essential in maintaining the maternal responsivity of the postpartum female rat.  相似文献   

11.
Maternal behavior is flexible and programs offspring development. Using a novel manipulation, we demonstrate that rat maternal behavior is sensitive to ecologically relevant stimuli. Long-Evans hooded rat dams (F0) and pups were exposed to a predator condition (cat odor) or a control condition (no odor) for 1 h on the day of parturition. Predator-exposed F0 dams displayed significantly more maternal behavior (licking/grooming, arched-back nursing) relative to control-exposed dams across five subsequent observation days. Female offspring (F1) were raised to adulthood, bred and maternal behavior was observed. F1 dams reared by a predator-exposed F0 dam displayed significantly higher maternal behavior relative to F1 dams reared by a control-exposed F0 dam across 5 days of observation. Increased levels of maternal behavior in predator-reared (PR) F1 dams were evident even in F1 females that had been cross-fostered (CF) from a control-exposed F0 dam, suggesting a non-genomic transmission of increased levels of maternal behavior. Lactating PR F1 dams had significantly elevated estrogen receptor alpha and beta mRNA in the medial preoptic area relative to control-reared (CR) F1 dams. Furthermore, among CR F1 dams, there was no significant difference between those dams that had been CF from predator-exposed F0 dams and those that had been sham CF. These results support the hypothesis that flexible rat maternal behavior can shape offspring development according to current environmental conditions. The results also suggest that estrogen signaling may be part of an epigenetic mechanism by which changes in maternal behavior are passed from F0 to F1 dams.  相似文献   

12.
The duration of the effectiveness of estradiol benzoate (EB) on the latency to the onset of maternal behavior was measured in 16-day pregnant rats that were hysterectomized-ovariectomized (HO). Eight groups of HO animals were treated with either a single SC injection of 5 μg/kg of EB or oil at surgery and were initially presented with foster pups at either 24, 48, 72, or 96 hr postoperatively. Compared to their respective controls, EB-treated animals showed singificantly shorter latencies when testing began at 48 and 72 hr but not 24 or 96 hr. In the second experiment, 16-day HO rats were treated with 5 μg/kg of EB at surgery and either oil or 0.5 mg of progesterone at 0, 24, or 44 hr postoperatively. Additional groups received either progesterone or oil at surgery (instead of EB) and a second injection of oil 44 hr later. Testing began 48 hr following surgery for all groups, and the results showed that only the groups injected with EB alone or EB plus progesterone at 44 hr displayed short-latency maternal behavior. It was concluded that a significant reduction in the latency to the onset of maternal behavior can be obtained between 24 and 72 hr after EB treatment and that progesterone when injected concurrently or 24 hr later can inhibit the effectiveness of EB.  相似文献   

13.
14.
Ovariectomized and intact adult virgin female mice presented with two 1-day-old mouse pups built larger nests and more nests rated “maternal” than did intact animals not proffered young. Virgin females presented with live 1-day olds behind a wire partition in their homecages also constructed larger nests and more nests rated “maternal” as compared to animals presented with freshly killed 1-day olds, 19-day olds, or with no pups behind the partition.  相似文献   

15.
The present series of experiments investigated the role of progesterone in inhibiting the onset of maternal behavior in the rat. Female rats hysterectomized and ovariectomized on Day 16 of pregnancy and injected subcutaneously with 20 μg/kg of estradiol benzoate (EB) show a short latency to onset of maternal behavior when presented with test pups 48 hr later. A subcutaneous injection of either 1 or 5 mg of progesterone on Day 16 of pregnancy and again 24 hr later inhibited this EB-induced short-latency onset of maternal behavior. The central neural site at which progesterone might act to produce this inhibitory effect was explored. Famale rats, hysterectomized and ovariectomized on Day 16 of pregnancy and injected subcutaneously with EB, received implants of crystalline progesterone on Day 16 of pregnancy into either the medial preoptic area, ventromedial hypothalamus, midbrain tegmentum, dorsal raphe nucleus, or median raphe nucleus. No inhibitory effects were found and all females showed a short-latency onset of maternal behavior. Several possible explanations for this lack of inhibitory effect of intracerebral implantation of progesterone are discussed.  相似文献   

16.
Sexually experienced female musk shrews (Suncus murinus) lack an ovarian, vaginal, and behavioral estrous cycle. Females, once induced by their initial contact with a male, are able to display copulatory behavior whenever a male is present (Rissman, Silveira, and Bronson, 1988). Based on plasma levels of steroids, and on hormone replacement studies conducted after ovariectomy (OVX), we have shown that testosterone (T) plays an essential role in the regulation of female sexual behavior (Rissman and Crews, 1988; Rissman, Clendenon, and Krohmer, 1990a; Rissman, 1991). To date we have only examined the potential contribution of adrenal steroids to female sexual behavior in a preliminary manner. After adrenalectomy, gonadally intact females display significantly lower levels of sexual behavior than controls (Rissman and Bronson, 1987). The following experiments were conducted to examine the role the adrenal steroids (in contrast to the medullary hormones) play in the induction of female sexual behavior in the musk shrew. In the first experiment gonadally intact females were treated with dexamethasone (DEX) to reduce the secretion of adrenal steroids. Significantly fewer females receiving DEX demonstrated sexual behavior as compared with controls. In the second study, OVX females received T-filled Silastic implants. When DEX was administered to OVX + T females at a dose that dropped circulating T levels to those found in ovary and adrenal intact females, no effect on sexual behavior was noted. The data show that the adrenals are a behaviorally important source of T and contribute toward the hormonal control of sexual behavior in these female mammals.  相似文献   

17.
The repeated findings that levels of various male-typical behaviors (e.g., copulatory behavior and intermale aggression) are reduced in prenatally stressed (P-S) males, coupled with reports of effects on female physiology and behavior, prompted us to examine the maternal behaviors of P-S animals toward young. Sprague-Dawley female rats were timed-mated (+ sperm = Day 1). From Gestation Days 15 to 22 experimental females were subjected to heat and restraint stress. Control females remained undisturbed throughout pregnancy. The offspring, as adults, were assessed for maternal behavior. P-S males exhibited a significantly shorter latency (in days) to show full maternal behavior (FMB) than Control males, median = 5.0 vs 8.0, respectively. P-S females, on the other hand, exhibited a significantly longer latency than Control females to show FMB (7.0 vs 3.0, respectively). as well as longer latencies to retrieve one, two, or three pups, to begin to crouch over pups, and to build nests in response to young. Sex differences were apparent between Control males and Control females (females were more responsive to young). In contrast, P-S males and Control females exhibited similar latencies to show components of FMB (3-5 days), as did P-S females and Control males (7-9 days). These data demonstrate, therefore, that prenatal stress eliminates the sex difference normally observed in pup-induced maternal behavior. Moreover, the data suggest that prenatal stress renders the male's responsiveness to young more "female-like," while conversely rendering the response of the female more "male-like."  相似文献   

18.
In a series of experiments the development of sexual behavior was studied in female rats. Lordosis behavior in response to manual stimulation was induced in 100% of 19-day old females by treatment with 10 μgm estradiol benzoate (EB) and 0.5 mgm progesterone (P) and earwiggling was displayed at earlier ages. During normal development, vaginal opening preceded the display of the first receptivity in most cases, the first two behavioral sex cycles tended to be prolonged and irregular, but the subsequent cycles were of regular 4 or 5 days duration. Although treatment of immature (18-, 23- or 28-day old) females with EB (10 μgm) and P (0.5 mgm) or with EB (0.025, 0.25 or 2.5 μgm until vaginal opening occurred) resulted in precocious vaginal opening and display of sexual receptivity, the treatment did not advance the development of behavioral cyclicity. Progesterone [0.25 mgm/100 gm body weight (bw)] facilitated the display of sexual receptivity in EB-primed (0.5 or 2.5 μgm/100 gm bw) ovariectomized immature and adult female rats. Evidence was presented that behavioral sensitivity to estrogen increases with age.  相似文献   

19.
In order to clarify the role of the median (MRN) and dorsal (DRN) raphe nuclei in regulating maternal care (retrieving and licking behavior), radiofrequency lesions or microknife cuts were made in postpartum rats on the day after delivery. Animals were housed individually without pups after the operation. One week after the surgery, maternal behavioral test was carried out daily for 3 days using pups of 2-6 days age. The results demonstrated that rats with MRN lesions or ventral horizontal cuts of the MRN showed extremely low incidence of the maternal behavior, as compared to those in control and sham-operated groups. DRN-lesions or dorsal cuts of the MRN had no effect. In locomotor activities measured by the infrared sensor system, there was no difference between the groups. This suggest that the MRN but not DRN plays a critical role in regulating retrieving and licking behaviors and ventral outputs are involved in this function in postpartum rats.  相似文献   

20.
J A Ramaley 《Steroids》1973,22(5):597-608
The following study was designed to test whether the change in the amplitude of the adrenal rhythm and the response to stress seen at puberty in female rats is dependent upon stimulation of the adrenal system by gonadal steroids. Rats were gonadectomized either at two days of age or at 21 days of age and periodic blood samples were taken by cardiac puncture and assayed for corticosterone (B) by a fluorometric procedure. Ovariectomy at weaning age (21 days) had no effect on adrenal function until the time of normal puberty onset. In intact animals, a rise in resting levels of corticosterone and an increase in the incremental response to ether stress was noted at 35 days of age. (Puberty in females was 35.3 ± 1.2 days as indicated by vaginal opening). Ovulation occurred the following day in 9/10 rats. Gonadectomy at two days of age had essentially the same effect as later gonadectomy in females. By 70 days of age, resting corticosterone values and stress responses in gonadectomized females reached levels similar to intact females. It is concluded that an independent adrenarche can occur in females but that gonadal steroids present at the time of puberty modulate the timing of this process.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号