共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Emmanuelle Tastard Christophe Andalo Martin Giurfa Monique Burrus Christophe Thébaud 《Arthropod-Plant Interactions》2008,2(4):237-246
To assess if pollinators’ behaviour could explain the maintenance of hybrid zones between different flower colour morphs,
we analyzed flower colour variation in an Antirrhinum hybrid zone using spectrometry and a model of bee perception. Some colours generated by hybridization were not observed in
any Antirrhinum species and even appeared to be rare among angiosperms. Variation in flower colours within the hybrid zone was continuous;
the most similar colours were predicted not to be discriminated from one another in natural foraging situations. However,
when compared at a scale corresponding to bees’ foraging range, some flower colours could be discriminated from all colours
displayed by neighbouring plants. This could affect pollinator behaviour and explain lower visitation rates within the centre
of the hybrid zone. Behavioural studies involving bumblebees and plant mixtures of parental and hybrid flower colours carefully
characterized with appropriate visual models will be necessary to test this hypothesis.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Handling editor: Lars Chittka 相似文献
2.
G. A. Horridge 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1997,181(3):267-277
The discrimination of pattern disruption in freely flying honeybees (Apis mellifera) was examined. Bees were trained to discriminate at a fixed distance between a regularly repeated black/white pattern and the same pattern at a different magnification in targets of the same angular size. The locations of areas of black were regularly shuffled to make them useless as cues. The results of the experiments indicate that the bees discriminate the disruption of the pattern as a whole, irrespective of the actual pattern. Bees trained to prefer a larger period transfer to an even larger period, when given a forced choice with a pair of patterns of differing disruption from those they were trained on, as if their spontaneous preference has not been overcome. Bees trained to prefer a smaller period, however, prefer the former negative pattern rather than transfer to an even smaller period. These results show that the bees do not rely solely on learning the absolute period of a pattern nor the relative disruption of two patterns, and they are confused when these two cues conflict in tests with unfamiliar targets. Bees can discriminate between fields of view that differ in average disruption as a generalized cue, irrespective of pattern. Accepted: 7 April 1997 相似文献
3.
Adrian G Dyer Christa Neumeyer 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2005,191(6):547-557
The colour discrimination of individual free-flying honeybees (Apis mellifera) was tested with simultaneous and successive viewing conditions for a variety of broadband reflectance stimuli. For simultaneous viewing bees used form vision to discriminate patterned target stimuli from homogeneous coloured distractor stimuli, and for successive discrimination bees were required to discriminate between homogeneously coloured stimuli. Bees were significantly better at a simultaneous discrimination task, and we suggest this is explained by the inefficiency with which the bees brain can code and retrieve colour information from memory when viewing stimuli successively. Using simultaneous viewing conditions bees discriminated between the test stimuli at a level equivalent to 1 just-noticeable-difference for human colour vision. Discrimination of colours by bees with simultaneous viewing conditions exceeded previous estimates of what is possible considering models of photoreceptor noise measured in bees, which suggests spatial and/or temporal summation of colour signals for fine discrimination tasks. The results show that when behavioural experiments are used to collect data about the mechanisms facilitating colour discrimination in animals, it is important to consider the effects of the stimulus viewing conditions on results. 相似文献
4.
K. Lunau E. J. Maier 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1995,177(1):1-19
Freshly emerged flower visitors exhibit colour preferences prior to individual experience with flowers. The understanding of innate colour preferences in flower visitors requires a detailed analysis, as, on the one hand, colour is a multiple-signal stimulus, and, on the other hand, flower visits include a sequence of behavioural reactions each of which can be driven by a preferential behaviour. Behavioural reactions, such as the distant approach, the close-range orientation, the landing, and the extension of mouthparts can be triggered by colour stimuli. The physiological limitations of spectral sensitivity, the neuro-sensory filters, and the animals' different abilities to make use of visual information such as brightness perception, wavelength-specific behaviour and colour vision shape colour preferences. Besides these receiverbased factors, there are restrictions of flower colouration due to sender-based factors such as the absorption properties of floral pigments and the dual function of flower colours triggering both innate and learned behaviour. Recordings of the spectral reflection of coloured objects, which trigger innate colour preferences, provide an objective measure of the colour stimuli. Weighting the spectral reflection of coloured objects by the spectral composition of the ambient light and the spectral sensitivity of the flower visitors' photoreceptors allows the calculation of the effective stimuli. Perceptual dimensions are known for only a few taxa of flower visitors. 相似文献
5.
B. Tatler D. C. O'Carroll S. B. Laughlin 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2000,186(4):399-407
A hot head gives an insect a clearer view of a moving world because warming reduces motion blur by accelerating photoreceptor
responses. Over a natural temperature range, 19–34 °C, the speed of response of blowfly (Calliphora vicina) photoreceptors more than doubles, to produce the fastest functional responses recorded from an ocular photoreceptor. This
acceleration increases temporal resolving power, as indicated by the corner frequency of the response power spectrum. When
light adapted, the corner frequency increases from 53 Hz to 119 Hz with a Q
10 of 1.9, and when dark adapted from 8 Hz to 32 Hz with a Q
10 of 3.0. Temperature sensitivity originates in the phototransduction cascade, and is associated with signal amplification.
The temperature sensitivity of photoreceptors must be taken into account when studying the mechanisms, function and ecology
of vision, and gives a distinct advantage to insects that thermoregulate.
Accepted: 2 February 2000 相似文献
6.
We used a population biological approach to assist our understanding of the evolution of behaviour, with island bumblebees as our model system. The widespread European species Bombus terrestris occurs on all major Mediterranean, and some Atlantic islands. Bees from different populations differ in a variety of behavioural traits, including floral colour preferences, flower detection, and learning behaviour. We attempted to correlate these behavioural differences with each populations environment, but could not find straightforward adaptive explanations. We also performed reciprocal transplant studies to compare nectar foraging performance of bees from three different populations, but found that non-native bees consistently outcompeted native bees. Thus, we consider genetic drift, exaptation, and pleiotropy as possible alternative explanations to a strictly adaptive explanation for between population behavioural differences in bumblebees. 相似文献
7.
8.
Visual discrimination of black bars by honeybees was studied in a Y-choice apparatus with fixed vertical patterns at constant range. The problem is to discover how bees remember different degrees of complexity of the orientation cue. Previous conclusions with parallel gratings and single bars disagree. With broad bars versus orthogonal bars, the bees learn the orientation cue if the bars are centred at the same place, but they learn the position cue in the vertical direction when the bars are at different places on the two targets. With several bars on each target, the bees learn their orientation and positions. As fixed patterns increase in complexity, the bees follow a simple rule, to look only at the range of places where the cues were displayed. The frame of reference is disrupted when a black spot is added to the training pattern. There is abundant evidence that the bees do not re-assemble the pattern or learn shapes. The filters that detect the position and orientation cues are coarsely tuned, so that they respond in a graded way, but the memory of the range of directions of the cue, as seen from the point of choice, is more exact. 相似文献
9.
10.
K. Arikawa H. Uchiyama 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1996,178(1):55-61
1. | The ommatidia of the butterfly Papilio have a fused and tiered rhabdom. The distal tier of the rhabdom is made up of four distal photoreceptors (R1–4), whereas the proximal tier is made up of four proximal (R5–8) and one basal photoreceptor cell (R9). |
2. | We first confirmed by light microscopy that the ommatidia of Papilio are not twisted, i.e. have the same spatial organization all about the longitudinal axis. The polarization method, previously applied to the distal tier, hence is applicable to identify the photoreceptor location from the peak angle of the polarization sensitivity. |
3. | We determined the polarization and spectral sensitivity of in total 109 proximal and basal photoreceptors in the lateral looking eye region. All of the photoreceptors were either green or red type, most of which fall into three classes as judged by the peak angles of the polarization sensitivity: around 40°, 150°, and 180° (= 0°) with respect to the dorso-ventral axis. The first two classes are formed by the proximal photoreceptors with straight microvilli oriented at the average angle of 39° (R6, 8) and 144° (R5, 7) respectively, and the third is formed by the basal photoreceptors R9 with straight microvilli oriented at 180° (= 0°). The mean polarization sensitivity (PS = maximal sensitivity/minimal sensitivity) was about 2. |
4. | 75% of the proximal and 48% of the basal photoreceptors were of the red type. |
5. | A single ommatidium of Papilio appears to contain two to four types of spectral receptors. |
11.
B. Walz B. Zimmermann S. Seidl 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1994,174(4):421-431
We have measured Cai at rest and upon light stimulation in the photoreceptors of the honeybee drone microfluorometrically with the fluorescent Ca2+ indicator dyes fura-2, fluo-3 and Ca-green 5N.In darkness, Cai was 90 nM after 5 min of dark adaptation. A saturating light step caused Cai to rise in the bulk cytoplasm to 750 nM within 1 s. Our measurements with the low affinity dye Ca-green 5N showed that bright 1-s light flashes cause a rapid increase in Cai which was graded with stimulus intensity. Ca-green 5N fluorescence reached a peak in about 200 ms, and then decayed to a slightly lower sustained plateau. The fluorescence signal peaked, when the receptor potential was repolarizing from its peak to the plateau. This observation is in agreement with the proposal that the peak-to-plateau transition of the receptor potential is caused by the rise in Cai
From our Fluo-3 measurements it appears that the latency of the Ca2+ increase is by 3–4 ms longer than the latency of the receptor potential elicited by bright 100-ms light flashes. This result provides no support for the proposal that Ca2+ mediates the opening of those membrane channels responsible for the upstroke of the receptor potential.Abbreviations ER
endoplasmic reticulum
- IP3
Inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate
- SMC
submicrovillar cisternae 相似文献
12.
13.
P.J.G.M. Van Breugel P.H.M. Bovee-Geurts S.L. Bonting F.J.M. Daemen 《生物化学与生物物理学报:生物膜》1979,557(1):188-198
The late photointermediates of rhodopsin photolysis have been analyzed spectrally and chemically in bovine rod outer segment membrane suspension at 25°C and pH 6.5. The decay of metarhodopsin II follows two spectrally distinct routes, resulting 40 min after illumination in a stable mixture of photoproducts with absorbance maxima around 380 and 452 nm, free retinal and metarhodopsin III, respectively. Chemical analysis shows that three different products are involved: free retinal (approx. 34%), protein-bound retinal (approx. 51%) and lipid-bound retinal (approx. 15%). The latter fraction consists of retinylidene-phosphatidylethanolamine exclusively.Photolysis of membranes reconstituted with various phospholipids gives a qualitatively normal spectral picture, but the production of metarhodopsin III may vary with the phospholipid composition, i.e. with the percent of phosphatidylethanolamine present. Chemical analysis shows that with increasing phosphatidylethanolamine content of the membrane, the retinylidene phosphatidylethanolamine fraction increases proportionally at the expense of free retinal, while the fraction of protein-bound retinal remains unaffected.The results indicate that under these conditions metarhodopsin III (defined as a long wavelength product of metahrodopsin II decay) is composed of two chemically distinct components: opsin-bound retinal and retinylidene phosphatidylethanolamine. 相似文献
14.
15.
Lisa J. Oliver Michael Salmon Jeanette Wyneken Robert Hueter Thomas W. Cronin 《Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology》2013,45(4):233-248
The eyes of three species of sea turtle hatchlings (loggerheads, green turtles, and leatherbacks) possess visual streaks, areas of densely packed ganglion cells running along the antero‐posterior retinal axis. These probably function to provide heightened visual acuity along the horizon. The vertical extent and absolute concentration of cells within the streak, compared to the rest of the retina, differ among the species. Leatherbacks have an additional specialized region (area temporalis) that might enhance their ability to detect prey below them in the water column. Green turtles and loggerheads, but not leatherbacks, show compensatory eye reflexes that keep the visual streak horizontal. Species differences in retinal structure and eye reflexes probably reflect their unique specializations in visual ecology and behaviour. 相似文献
16.
C. R. L. Reilly S. H. Thompson 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2007,193(9):943-953
The absolute low-light sensitivity of four congeneric species of rockfish (genus Sebastes) was studied from analysis of electroretinograms measured in living fish. The purpose was: (1) to determine if temperature sensitive noise in rod photoreceptors affects the absolute limit to low-light sensitivity at environmentally realistic temperatures and light levels, and (2) to examine whether interspecific variations in habitat utilization within rockfish communities correlate with differences in visual sensitivity. It was found that the low-light sensitivity of individual retinae is inversely dependent on temperature, decreasing tenfold with a 10°C increase in temperature. While in all four species, temperature had a similar effect on sensitivity, the absolute sensitivity levels were different. The four species could be divided into two groups based on measured sensitivity. Kelp and olive rockfish form a high-sensitivity group capable of responding to light levels approximately 50-fold lower than blue and black rockfish. The sensitivity groups correlated with reported diel activity patterns; the high-sensitivity group forages nocturnally, whereas members of the low-sensitivity group are quiescent during twilight and night and forage during the day. 相似文献
17.
M. Lehrer 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1999,184(2):195-206
Colour discrimination performance of honeybees was examined by training bees to a two-coloured disc presented on a vertical plane. Access to the food reward was through the centre of the disc. In one experiment, the upper half of the disc was yellow, and the lower half blue. In another experiment, this was vice versa. In either case, the learned disc was tested against each of a series of ten discs whose colour differed in either the upper or the lower half. A comparison between the results obtained in the two experiments reveals that colour discrimination is significantly better in the lower half of the frontal eye region than it is in the upper half. The results, similar to earlier results obtained in pattern discrimination tasks, cannot be explained by peripheral eye-region-specific specializations. It is proposed that the functional significance of the lower frontal visual field is based on more central neural mechanisms that might constitute an adaptation to the forager's natural needs. Accepted: 2 November 1998 相似文献
18.
We investigate effects of random perturbations on the dynamics of one-dimensional maps (single species difference equations) and of finite dimensional flows (differential equations for n species). In particular, we study the effects of noise on the invariant measure, on the correlation dimension of the attractor, and on the possibility of detecting the nonlinear deterministic component by applying reconstruction techniques to the time series of population abundances. We conclude that adding noise to maps with a stable fixed-point obscures the underlying determinism. This turns out not to be the case for systems exhibiting complex periodic or chaotic motion, whose essential properties are more robust. In some cases, adding noise reveals deterministic structure which otherwise could not be observed. Simulations suggest that similar results hold for flows whose attractor is almost two-dimensional. 相似文献
19.
Peter Skorupski Thomas F. Döring Lars Chittka 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2007,193(5):485-494
Most species of flower-visiting Hymenoptera are trichromatic, with photoreceptor spectral sensitivity peaks in the UV, blue and green regions of the spectrum. Red flowers, therefore, should be relatively difficult to detect for such insects. Nevertheless, in population biological studies in the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris, the Sardinian island population (B. t. sassaricus) displayed significantly higher responses to red artificial flowers (in tests of innate colour choice and detectability) than several mainland populations of the same species (Chittka et al. in Cognitive ecology of pollination, pp 106–126, 2001; Popul Ecol 46:243–251, 2004). Since there is relatively little physiological data on population differences in sensory systems, we used intracellular recording to compare photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in B. t. sassaricus and the southern European and Mediterranean population, B. t. dalmatinus. The results show both populations to be UV–blue–green trichromats, but with a small but significant increase in long-wave sensitivity in island bees. Spectral peaks were estimated at 348, 435 and 533 nm (B. t. dalmatinus) and 347, 436 and 538 nm (B. t. sassaricus) for UV, blue and green receptors, respectively. There were no significant differences in UV and blue receptor sensitivities. We found no photoreceptors maximally sensitive to red spectral light in the Sardinian population and model calculations indicate that the behavioural population differences in colour responses cannot be directly explained by receptor population differences. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. 相似文献
20.
Summary Acetylcholinesterase (AChE) activity was demonstrated histochemically at the electron microscopic level in the compound eye of the worker bee (Apis mellifica L.) by use of the method of Lewis and Shute (1969).All photoreceptor axons (short and long visual fibres) display AChE activity. The reaction product is located in the axoplasm and at the plasma membrane. Substantial amounts of the reaction product can be detected in the intercellular spaces between the visual fibres. Along the visual fibres, the enzyme activity is unevenly distributed. High AChE activity is present in the distal parts of the axons, in contrast to lower enzyme levels in the lamina. However, AChE is also present in the proximal terminals of the visual fibres as well as in the intercellular spaces between visual fibre terminals and the postsynaptic neurones (monopolar cells). Intracellular enzyme activity is almost absent in the monopolars.The authors assume the high AChE activity in the visual fibres to be indicative of acetylcholine as the transmitter at the first synapse of the compound eye. This hypothesis is discussed in view of the results of autoradiographic, electrophysiological and pharmacological investigations of the compound eye and of the ocellus. Our data are at variance with results of studies on the eyes of Diptera. 相似文献