共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 78 毫秒
1.
Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains,Tanzania 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
This study investigated post-conflict (PC) behavior among wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the M-group in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, and examined what types of behavior characterize the PC situation in this group, and the factors that influence the occurrence of PC affiliation between opponents soon after the end of an aggressive conflict (i.e., reconciliation). We found that the opponents affiliated selectively soon after the end of aggression, suggesting that reconciliation occurred in this group. The mean individual corrected conciliatory tendency (CCT) (Veenema et al. 1994 in Behav Proc 31:29–38) was 14.4%, which is similar to or lower than frequencies observed in studies of captive and wild chimpanzees. The valuable relationship hypothesis predicts that the CCT is higher among individuals who share valuable relationships (e.g., males or affiliative dyads) than among individuals who do not (e.g., females or less-associative dyads). However, the analysis based on data for aggression between unrelated individuals (including one incident between an adult and non-adult) and aggression between unrelated adults, did not uncover this difference. Affiliation by a previously uninvolved individual with the victim (consolation) and with the aggressor (appeasement) occurred more frequently following aggression than in the control condition. The results are compared with previous studies of captive and wild chimpanzees. 相似文献
2.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1988,29(2):147-161
Association partners of young chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park were analyzed. Juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees
associated frequently with their mothers, although mother-offspring association decreased as the offspring grew up. Males
tended to leave their mothers and associate with adult males, while females remained frequently associating with their mothers
in early adolescence. In late adolescence and young adulthood, males usually associated with adult males and cycling adult
females. Females may transfer into neighboring unit-groups in this stage. Although an immigrant female tended to be alone
when her estrous cycle stopped, she associated with many individuals, in particular with adult males, when she resumed cycling.
Some orphans were observed to associate frequently with particular adults. The findings were discussed in relation to the
unique characteristics of chimpanzee social system. 相似文献
3.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(4):343-360
Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, was studied, by analyzing processes of play and interindividual relationships
in play. The results are discussed in relation to communication mechanisms. Play was initiated in several ways. Communication
about play seems to depend on the receiver's interpretation: They can interpret the sender's behavior as play, referring to
(1) play signals accompanied by the behavior, (2) transformation of the behavior in timing, strength, or rhythm, and (3) situation
of the occurrence. Initiation attempts sometimes failed because one hesitated in playing with the other. Although the stronger
often reduced his/her activity during play, play tended to escalate in activity. Players may enjoy such escalation. Play also
had a mechanism not to escalate into fighting. Play was influenced by individuals other than the players. The third party's
movement often affected the players' interaction. The term play does not indicate a behavior itself but the context of the
behavior: The players interpret their behaviors in their play context. 相似文献
4.
The M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, began to feed on three agricultural fruit species, guava, mango and lemon.
It took them 7–8 years until they began to taste these fruits since the villagers left the park area in 1974. Although adult
chimpanzees are conservative in their feeding habits, they are capable of rapidly acquiring new feeding habits, or new traditions,
once they notice that the food is suitable. 相似文献
5.
Feces of wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, were inspected for intestinal parasites under a compound microscope.
Eggs or larvae ofOesophagostomum, Strongyloides, Trichuris, Prosthenorchis, andBertiella were found. Intestinal nematodes significantly increased in the mid-rainy season. This finding supports (or, at least, is
not in conflict with) the hypothesis thatAspilia leaves which are occasionally swallowed by chimpanzees may function as a vermicide, since ingestion of such leaves also increases
significantly in the mid-rainy season. 相似文献
6.
Michael A. Huffman Shunji Gotoh Linda A. Turner Miya Hamai Kozo Yoshida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1997,38(2):111-125
A longitudinal study of nematode infection in chimpanzees was conducted between 1989 and 1994 on the M group chimpanzees of
the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania during two annual dry and rainy season periods and a third rainy season. Chemical
and physical antiparasite properties of medicinal plant use against the strongyle nematodeOesophagostomum stephanostomum have recently been reported at Mahale. Here, the incidence of nematode infections were analyzed for seasonal trends to elucidate
the possible influence of parasite infection on previously reported seasonality of medicinal plant use and to test the hypothesis
that the use of these plants is stimulated byO. stephanostomum. The number of chimpanzees infected byO. stephanostomum was significantly higher in the rainy season than in the dry season of both 1989–1990 and 1991–1992. However, the incidence
ofTrichuris trichura andStrongyloides fuelleborni showed no seasonality. Reinfection of individuals byO. stephanostomum occurred in synchrony with annual variation in rainfall: there was a sharp rise in the occurrence of new infections per individual
within one to two months after the beginning of the first heavy rains of the season. This pattern coincides with the reproductive
cycle of this nematode species.O. stephanostomum (95%) infections were associated significantly more frequently with medicinal plant use than eitherT. trichiura (50%) orS. fuelleborni (40%) infections. These observations are consistent with previous reports for the increased use of these plants during the
rainy season and are consistent the hypothesis that medicinal plant use is stimulated byO. stephanostomum infection. 相似文献
7.
Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania: A preliminary study
Hitoshige Hayaki Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(2):187-197
Dominance relationships among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were analyzed. Although all
adolescent males were unequivocally subordinate to all adult males, dominance relationships within the age classes were much
less clear. Especially among adolescent males, few pant-grunts or agonistic interactions occurred. While adolescent males
frequently pant-grunted at adult males, these latter males, except the alpha and the youngest, rarely pant-grunted to one
another. This suggests that a difference of social status exists between adolescent and adult males. Adult males rarely display
overt dominance to one another probably because the presence of other males affects their interactions. Moreover, they seem
to try to keep their dominance relationship ambiguous when making it overt is not advantageous to them. This may be a political
way for males to coexist with one another in a unit-group. 相似文献
8.
Shigeo Uehara 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(1):1-13
Sex differences in animal prey intake were revealed by fecal analysis among wild chimpanzees of the large-sized M-group (ca.
100 members) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania: prime adult or old males feed more on vertebrates, while adult
females more onCamponotus ants. By contrast, such differences were not obvious in the neighboring, small-sized K-group (ca. 20–30 members), despite
the similar environment in which the two unit-groups lived. Such sex and group differences may be explained in terms of various
factors, either ecological or social, or both, but social factors seem most responsible in particular for the group differences.
It seems likely that increased capture rate of vertebrates per unit-group in the larger-sized M-group results in increased
per capita intake of meat among prime adult or old males. Also, the more frequent interactions among prime adult or old males
of M-group appear to reduce the frequency of theirCamponotus ant-fishing behavior. 相似文献
9.
Opportunistic and restrictive matings among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains,Tanzania 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
The mating patterns of free-ranging chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, were studied. Opportunistic mating (non-competitive and temporary mating) was frequently
observed in a large-sized unit-group, among young, low-ranking males, and among young, newcomer, non-ovulating females. Restrictive
mating (a continuous sexual relationship between a particular pair which includes possessiveness and consortship) was frequently
observed in a small-sized unit-group, among middle- and old-aged, high-ranking males, and among old, resident, ovulating females.
Relations between those characteristics, such as group size and composition, ages of the individuals of both sexes, female
estrous stages, and life history, and the 2 mating patterns are discussed. 相似文献
10.
Detailed observations on the consumption ofVernonia amygdalina (Del.), a naturally occurring plant of known ethnomedicinal value, by an adult female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of M-group in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania were made. Chewing the pith of several shoots, she sucked out and swallowed
the astringent, bitter tasting juice; spitting out the fibrous remains. The female was followed during this period for 11
hr, over two consecutive days, and was recognized to be in irregular health at the time of consumption, exhibiting signs of
lethargy, lack of appetite, and irregularity of bodily excretions. The low frequency and lack of seasonality in the usage
of this plant suggest that it is sought after for reasons other than as a food source. These factors suggest that for chimpanzees,
the consumption of this plant is primarily medicinal. The symptoms displayed by the female are the same as those experienced
by people throughout tropical Africa who utilize this plant as a medicinal treatment for them. Interactions between the female
and others suggest that they too were aware of her condition and coordinated their activities with the female and her infant. 相似文献
11.
Kenji Kawanaka 《International journal of primatology》1984,5(5):411-434
In order to characterize the social unit in chimpanzees, about which several conflicting views have been proposed, the proximity
matrix among 55 recognized chimpanzees and the range covered by each of them are examined, on the basis of data obtained at
the Mahale Mountains during 12 months in 1978–1979. It is shown once again that chimpanzees have a bisexual social unit (unit-group).
Two such unit-groups were detected in the study area. All animals belonged to one of the two unit-groups except a few cycling
females (and a juvenile male accompanied by his cycling mother) which were seen to associate alternately with members of two
neighboring unit-groups, covering a whole range of one or even two unit-groups. The problem of such females is discussed in
relation to the spatial relationships between the two unit-groups. Reexamining the membership of a unit-group, it is demonstrated
that a unit-group was most likely patrilineal. While nulliparous females transferred between unit-groups, parous females tended
to remain in a unit-group where they first gave birth to infants and to have several offspring therein. This appeared significant
for ensuring recruitment of members of the next generation to a patrilineal unit-group. Although some adult males left their
natal unit-group, they never joined the other. Male departure from a unit-group seemed to be forced by the other males and
to be the sociological equivalent of going into exile, which is unique in nonhuman primates. 相似文献
12.
This study, based on 687 hr of focal observations, aims to describe overall patterns of the sexual behavior of the adult male
chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, to compare the results with previous reports, and to explain the variations between studies.
Genital inspection of cycling females by adult males was eight times as frequent as that of lactating females, and twice as
frequent as that of pregnant females. Inspection of the genitals of cycling females increased dramatically 7–10 days before
the onset of maximal swelling and gradually decreased as the day of ovulation approached. Adult males likely obtained information
on the attractivity of females by inspecting their genitals. Mating was usually initiated by male courtship and followed by
pelvic thrusts in a dorsoventral posture, performed on, rather than above, the ground, which continued for 7 s. on average,
and was typically followed by female squeaking and darting from the male, or by the male grooming the female. Higher-ranking
males mated with females in the peri-ovulatory period more frequently than did lower-ranking males. In particular, two alpha
males mated with such females more often than did any other adult males. A male who interfered with a mating pair was dominant
over the mating male in other agonistic contexts. The duration of intromission was correlated with neither dominance rank
nor age. However, when an adult male declined in rank from alpha in 1991 to third in 1992, he showed a significantly shorter
duration of intromission. This indicates that for a particular male, the alpha rank guaranteed longer duration of intromission.
Allies of alpha males tended to mate with peri-ovulatory females more frequently than expected from their low dominance ranks.
The number of mating partners was not correlated with male dominance rank, but was sometimes negatively correlated with male
age. Females were significantly more likely to emit a copulatory squeak when mating with younger, rather than older, adult
males. Male dominance rank and the rate of female copulatory squeaking were not correlated. Weaning infants regularly interfered
with their mothers' mating. Occasionally, unrelated adolescent males and rarely females pushed themselves in between copulating
adults. Female choice was indicated when they performed a “penis erection check” or took the initiative in courtship, or on
the other hand showed strong reluctance to mate with particular males. Young adult males more often received erection checks
than did prime males, while none of the three old adult males did. Courtship initiated by estrous females was not directed
to two of the oldest males, the exception of which was the alpha male. The oldest males, except for the alpha, were consistently
avoided by many estrous females, both young and old. In response to female reluctance, males behaved violently, however, this
was not effective, because other more dominant males came to rescue the female. Neither courtship nor mating was seen between
mature sons and their mothers, nor between brothers and sisters. 相似文献
13.
Food transfer between mother and infant chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
We studied food transfer between chimpanzee mothers and infants in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. The rate
of infant solicitation for food dramatically increased in the second year of life, then gradually decreased and, in the seventh
year, virtually disappeared. The pattern of the ontogeny of food sharing precisely followed that of solicitation because mothers
shared food only when requested to do so by their infants. The success rate of solicitation, however, did not display extreme
changes across ages. Food that was difficult to process was shared more frequently because it was more likely to be demanded
by infants. We defined food retrieval as an infant’s recovery of leftovers discarded by its mother. Food types retrieved were
often those that are difficult to process and were also likely to be shared by mothers. However, infants tended to solicit
small, difficult food types for sharing while they often retrieved the remains of large, difficult food types. The function
of food sharing and food retrieval lies in an infant’s learning food types that it cannot easily obtain or process by itself.
The level of competition for food between mothers and infants remained low throughout infancy. We noted no particular characteristic
about foods from which infants were displaced by mothers. As infants grew older, they increased the distance between themselves
and mothers that became more aggressive. 相似文献
14.
Grips and hand movements of chimpanzees during feeding in Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania 下载免费PDF全文
Mary W. Marzke Linda F. Marchant William C. McGrew Sandra P. Reece 《American journal of physical anthropology》2015,156(3):317-326
It has long been assumed that stone tool making was a major factor in the evolution of derived hominin hand morphology. However, stresses on the hand associated with food retrieval and processing also have been recognized as relevant early hominin behaviors that should be investigated. To this end, chimpanzee food manipulation was videotaped in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Grips and hand movements by 39 chimpanzees were analyzed for arboreal and terrestrial feeding involving 10 food‐types and associated vegetation. It was predicted that (1) new grips would be found that had not been observed in captivity, (2) forceful precision grips would be absent from the repertoire, as in captivity, and (3) precision handling would be observed. New grips involving the full thumb and buttressed index finger, and a new integrated pattern of grips and forceful hand movements were discovered, associated with feeding on large fruits and meat. Participation of the full thumb in these grips, rather than the distal thumb and fingers, throws light on feeding behaviors that may have become increasingly significant factors in the evolution of derived hominin thumb morphology. The proximal thumb stabilizes food with the flexed index finger against the pull of the teeth and provides leverage in breaking food into portions. Isolated qualitative observations of possibly forceful pinch by the thumb and side of the index finger highlight the need for comparative quantitative data to test whether humans are unique in forceful precision gripping capability. Precision handling was not seen. Am J Phys Anthropol 156:317–326, 2015. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 相似文献
15.
Nadia Corp Hitoshige Hayaki Takahisa Matsusaka Shiho Fujita Kazuhiko Hosaka Nobuyuki Kutsukake Michio Nakamura Miho Nakamura Hitonaru Nishie Masaki Shimada Koichiro Zamma William Wallauer Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》2009,50(2):184-189
In 1998, four chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were observed wiping their mouths with non-detached
leaves or stalks of grass, or rubbing their mouths with a tree trunk or branch, especially while eating lemons. The number
of mouth-wiping/rubbing individuals increased to 18 in 1999. By 2005, 29 chimpanzees were documented wiping/rubbing their
muzzles in this way. Although it is difficult to determine whether the chimpanzees acquired this behavior as a result of trial
and error or social learning, the fact that chimpanzees at other sites perform this behavior with detached leaves or leafy
twigs much more often than with intact items suggests the possibility that cleaning with intact plant parts at Mahale spread
via social learning. 相似文献
16.
A 12-year-old female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of M-Group in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania was seen to rouse, capture, and eat a squirrel hiding in the
narrow hole of a tree. The kill was aided by the use of a sturdy tool modified from a branch of the same tree. This appears
to be the first reported case for chimpanzees, or any other nonhuman primate, of tool-use that directly led to the capture
of a mammalian prey species. This behavior is discussed in relation to possible factors contributing to the occurrence of
tool-use in small mammal predation especially by females to exploit a low competition meat source. 相似文献
17.
Koichi Koshimizu Hajime Ohigashi Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida Hiroyuki Takasaki 《International journal of primatology》1993,14(2):345-356
Potential medicinal plants for wild chimpanzees have been studied in order to discover their physiologically active compounds. Tests of the physiological activity of 3 plant species—Vernonia amygdalina, Aspilia mossambicensis, andFicus exasperata—indicate that they contain a variety of active compounds. From one species,V. amygdalina, an antitumor agent and 2 possible antitumor promoters are identified. Furthermore, steroid glucosides were isolated as the bitter substances. These structurally new compounds are expected to exhibit a number of significant physiological activities. The chemical investigation of possible medicinal plants used by chimpanzees should be helpful in recovering naturally occurring compounds of medicinal significance for human use. 相似文献
18.
Hiroyuki Takasaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(2):121-129
The mating behavior of receptive females in the M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains, western Tanzania, was studied by intensive sampling over a period of 5 months. Restrictive matings
were observed only between parous females mostly in the ovulatory stage and prime adult males, in particular the alpha. Young,
nulliparous and/or non-ovulatory females tended to mate with immature or low-ranking adult males. By contrast, older, parous
and/or ovulatory females tended to mate with adult and higher-ranking males. These confirmed the results of a previous extensive
study of the same group. In addition, gradual shift from opportunistic to restrictive matings after inter-unit-group transfer
was revealed for 2 ex-K-group parous females. The findings were discussed in relation to the life history of female chimpanzees.
A National Park since 1984. 相似文献
19.
Ten male and nine female habituated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Kasoje area of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were weighed intermittently between December 1973 and March 1980 by luring them up a rope hung on a spring balance: six adult males averaged 42.0 kg and eight adult females 35.2 kg. Seasonal change in body weight was recognized at least partially; body weights tended to decrease in the later part of the wet season presumably because of food shortage in the middle of the wet season. Comparison of body weight among three populations of the same subspecies suggests that adult female chimpanzees of Mahale appear to be heavier than those of the Gombe National Park, Tanzania, and that they seem to be similar to the forest-living counterparts of eastern Zaire. On the other hand, body weights of adult male chimpanzees from the three populations do not show significant differences. Perhaps feeding competition among adult females in a small, isolated habitat is more severe than that among adult males, which may result in the body weight reduction among adult female chimpanzees at Gombe. 相似文献
20.
Kenji Kawanaka 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(3):285-305
The behavior and social interactions of young male chimpanzees were studied in relation to their age change. The data were
obtained at the Mahale Mountains National Park, during a four-month period in 1986. Early adolescent males, becoming independent
of their mothers, spend a long time near adults of both sexes. Late adolescent males are not tolerated by the senior males.
Although such animals do not stop traveling together with their seniors, they are separated from the other members including
the males of their own age class, and each of them lives a relatively lonely life. Where seniors are not nearby, they perform
charging displays in front of estrous females. Young adult males tend to remain in the proximity of the alpha male, and can
associate with their seniors without pant-grunting. Although some young adult males dominate over some senior males, increasingly
performing charging displays, they do not appear to be permitted to associate intimately with their seniors; they are not
yet considered to have attained social maturity. Prime and senior males are strongly bonded with one another, being able to
associate intimately with those of their own or senior age classes including the alpha male. A young adult male's rise in
rank is not connected with joining the “adult male-cluster,” nor does a senior male's decline necessarily means his dropping
out from the cluster: the social position of male chimpanzees cannot be understood solely from their agonistic dominance rank.
The alpha male plays a leading part in integrating the males of the unit-group. Young adult males and their seniors tend to
associate most frequently with him, and all the males of the early adolescent or senior age classes pay attention to his movements. 相似文献