共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 17 毫秒
1.
Genichi Idani 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(4):441-447
The role in seed dispersal played by the pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) inhabiting Wamba, Republic of Zaïre, was studied. Germination was tested for seeds of 17 plant species recovered from the feces of pygmy chimpanzees at Wamba. The fecal seeds of 13 species germinated, and in six of the species the germination rate for the fecal seeds was higher than that of control seeds. Although five other species showed a higher germination rate in the control seeds than in the fecal seeds, the remaining two species revealed no difference in germination rate between the fecal and control seeds. There was no great difference in germination velocity between the fecal and control seeds of the same species. For comparison, seeds of four plant species collected from the feces of common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and gibbons (Hylobates lar) in captivity in Okinawa were tested for their germinability. In this test, although the seeds had passed through the digestive tract, their germinability demonstrated little change. Based on the behavioral characteristics of the pygmy chimpanzee at Wamba and observations of the captive primates on Okinawa, it seems that pygmy chimpanzees may play an important role in the seed dispersal of fruit plant species at Wamba. 相似文献
2.
The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Takayoshi Kano 《Primates; journal of primatology》1982,23(2):171-188
This is a sociological study of the pygmy chimpanzees of Wamba which have been provisioned sufficiently since 1977. The society
of the pygmy chimpanzee is basically similar to that of the common chimpanzee in the flexibility in forming parties and in
the existence of a definite social unit, i.e.,unit group. It also resembles in that young females play an important role in exchanging members among unit groups, thus, as transmitters
of genes from group to group, while males do in integrating groups, delimiting the social boundaries of unit groups through
their inter-group antagonism. Pygmy chimpanzees differ from common chimpanzees in fission and fusion pattern of parties. The
former generally forms parties larger in size and more homogeneous in composition than the latter. Any party of the former
has both reproductive and nursing functions, while the parties of the latter are categorized into various types according
to functions and compositions. This uniformity in pygmy chimpanzee parties seems to be well maintained by the females' high
sexual ability to manifest semi-continual estrus. 相似文献
3.
A L Zihlman 《Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology》1987,49(3-4):127-136
Hand deformities in 2 wild-born pygmy chimpanzees were investigated through dissection, linear measurements, bone mineral analysis and X-rays. Weight of tissue (skin, muscle, bone) was determined for each body segment (hand, forearm, upper arm), permitting comparison between the abnormal and normal upper limbs. In one individual, the right and left forearms and hands differed in tissue weights and bone mineral content. In the other, the hands differed in tissue composition, but not the forearms. 相似文献
4.
Thomas Patterson 《Primates; journal of primatology》1979,20(3):341-354
A group of captive pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) was studied in the San Diego Zoological Gardens. The behavior patterns that these animals exhibit are described. Each of
these behavior patterns is compared to those described for wild and captive common chimpanzees (P. troglodytes). Differences in behavior between these two species are attributed to specialization of the pygmy chimpanzee to a rain forest
habitat and to a monogamous social system. 相似文献
5.
Hiroshi Ihobe 《Primates; journal of primatology》1990,31(1):109-112
Interspecific interactions accompanied by physical contacts between wild pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) and red colobus (Colobus badius) were observed on three occasions at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. In all cases, the red colobus initiated the interactions by
approaching the pygmy chimpanzees. Most of the pygmy chimpanzees, which were within 5 m of the red colobus, were juveniles
or infants but the adult male pygmy chimpanzees never showed any interest in the red colobus. The red colobus groomed the
chimpanzees in two cases, but the latter never groomed the former. No true aggressive interactions were observed between the
two species. The lack of any evidence of hunting of red colobus through longitudinal studies of the pygmy chimpanzees of Wamba,
together with the present observations, suggests that red colobus are probably not targets of hunting by the pygmy chimpanzees. 相似文献
6.
Skeletal dimensions of pygmy (Pan paniscus) and common (Pan troglodytes) chimpanzees were compared. Significant differences were found in the clavicles, scapulae, pelvises, and in the humerus/femur and femur head/length ratios. No significant differences were observed in long bone lengths or talar breadths. There is extensive overlap in body weights, so that the observed differences cannot be accounted for by body size alone. We conclude that pygmy and common chimpanzees are morphologically distinct. Implications for hominoid evolution are discussed. 相似文献
7.
A parasitological examination was carried out on the feces of pygmy chimpanzees,Pan paniscus, in Zaïre, Africa. Of a total of 390 feces, 99.0% containedTroglodytella sp. trophozoites, 45.1% dicrocoeliid trematode eggs, 21.0%Capillaria sp. eggs, 3.3%Trichuris sp. eggs, 52.9%Strongyloides sp. eggs, 17.9%Oesophagostomum sp. eggs, 21.0% hookworm-like nematode eggs and 6.2% oxyurid eggs. No zoonotic protozoan was found. This is the first survey on the parasites of the pygmy chimpanzees in their natural habitat. 相似文献
8.
Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1972,13(4):415-425
Local information was collected on the ecology of pygmy chimpanzees in the Lac Tumba Region, Zaire. Population density very small; habitat, secondary swamp forest with occasional trespass of esobe grassland; locomotion, quadrupedal walking on the ground; diet, primarily frugivorous and vegetarian, but insects, honey and fish consumed. Many kinds of cultivated plants also eaten; sleeping nest built in a tree in the same fashion as ordinary chimpanzees; group size fairly large, from 15–20 to 40 head; human persecution very severe. It proved possible to make a field study of the species in this region.The research was financially supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo. 相似文献
9.
Hiroshi Ihobe 《Primates; journal of primatology》1992,33(2):247-250
Meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) was witnessed on two occasions at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. Only flying squirrels were observed to be eaten by the bonobos.
Several bonobos gathered around the possessor of the meat and showed interest in the meat on all occasions. Begging behavior
was noted on one of the two occasions, but the possessor of the meat ignored it. No sharing of meat was seen on either occasion.
The exclusive targets of hunting by bonobos are apparently small mammals, such as flying squirrels and infant duikers, since
evidence of meat eating by wild bonobos, which have been studied for more than fifteen years, has been restricted to these
mammals. The bonobos at Wamba may have a specialized “prey image”, as in the case of the chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Tai forest, and certain medium-sized or small mammals may not conform to this image. 相似文献
10.
Gen’ichi Idani 《Primates; journal of primatology》1995,36(3):377-383
Peering behavior (prolonged gazing within 30 cm by an animal toward another) in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Zaire, was studied. A total of 230 peering episodes were observed in various social contexts. Peering behavior
was often directed from younger animals toward older ones. In particular, adult females were most frequently involved in peering,
with individuals of all age-sex classes. On the other hand, male bonobos seldom took part in peering behavior. Four types
of behavior patterns followed the peering behavior: (1) the peerer left; (2) the peeree left; (3) both peerer and peeree stayed
but had no further social interaction; and (4) some other social interaction followed. Type (1) was the most frequent. Peering
usually led to tolerance by older (dominant) animals of a younger (subordinate) animal’s subsequent actions directed towards
the former. Peering was thus concluded to be a unilateral action for initiating affinitive interactions by the peerer. 相似文献
11.
The dichotomy between the two Pan species, the bonobo (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) has been strongly emphasized until very recently. Given that most studies were primarily based on adult individuals, we shifted the “continuity versus discontinuity” discussion to the infant and juvenile stage. Our aim was to test quantitatively, some conflicting statements made in literature considering species differences between immature bonobos and chimpanzees. On one hand it is suggested that infant bonobos show retardation in motor and social development when compared with chimpanzees. Additionally it is expected that the weaning process is more traumatic to chimpanzee than bonobo infants. But on the other hand the development of behaviors is expected to be very similar in both species. We observed eight mother–infant pairs of each species in several European zoos. Our preliminary research partially confirms that immature chimpanzees seem spatially more independent, spending more time at a larger distance from their mother than immature bonobos. However, the other data do not seem to support the hypothesis that bonobo infants show retardation of motor or social development. The development of solitary play, environmental exploration, social play, non-copulatory mounts and aggressive interactions do not differ between the species. Bonobo infants in general even groom other group members more than chimpanzee infants. We also found that older bonobo infants have more nipple contact than same aged chimpanzees and that the weaning process seems to end later for bonobos than for immature chimpanzee. Additionally, although immature bonobos show in general more signs of distress, our data suggest that the weaning period itself is more traumatic for chimpanzees. 相似文献
12.
Hiroshi Ihobe 《Primates; journal of primatology》1992,33(2):163-179
Male-male relationships among wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) in two adjacent unitgroups (E1 and E2 groups), which were formed by division of the E group, were studied at Wamba, in the
Central Zaire Basin, by analyzing the proximity and social interactions among males. Dominant-subordinate relationships between
a male-male dyad were easily recognized from the directions of individual agonistic interactions. Male bonobos rarely joined
forces in aggression. Clear differences in social status existed between adult and adolescent male bonobos in both groups,
as reported in the case of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). The presence of mothers in the unit-group greatly influenced the dominant-subordinate relationships among males through
strong mother-son bonds in both groups. However, the extent of the mother-son bonds differed between the groups. Males in
the E2 group participated more frequently in agonistic or affinitive interactions than did males in the E1 group. Males in
the E1 group were divided spatially into several clusters, while there were cohesive relationships among the adult males in
the E2 group. The difference in intensities of mother-son bonds between the groups may be explained by the distribution of
males at the time of the division of the E group. Differences in male-male relationships between bonobos and chimpanzees seem
to be related to differences in intra- and inter-unit-group competition among males between the two species. Male chimpanzees
may achieve coexistence by manipulating ambivalent relationships that are caused by intra- and inter-unit-group competition
among them, while male bonobos may achieve coexistence by decreasing intra- and inter-unit-group competition among them. 相似文献
13.
Takayoshi Kano 《American journal of physical anthropology》1984,63(1):1-11
The wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) of Wamba, Zaire have an extremely high frequency of external abnormal traits. Out of 96 animals in a sample comprising all age and sex classes from two different natural groups, 46 individuals had at least one limb defect. The frequency of abnormalities was greater in males than in females, and increased directly with age. Almost all infants and juveniles had completely normal limbs, but virtually all males and over half the females which had reached their prime were found to have one or more limb defects. These findings indicate that congenital factors contribute little to the frequency of abnormalities in this sample. Survival rate is the same in individuals with defects as in those without defects. It appears that individual social status is far more important in procuring foods than locomotor ability. Thus, the operation of such abnormalities as a selective factor in intraspecific competition is minimized in the network of their social relations. 相似文献
14.
Suehisa Kuroda 《Journal of Ethology》1984,2(2):127-137
The pygmy chimpanzee rocks its upper body in a variety of contexts, while the common chimpanzee rocks in only 3 situations;
courtship, charging display and frustration. In the pygmy chimpanzee, behaviors involving rocking except for charging display
are intentional communicative behaviors which bring about various body contact interactions. Each interaction pattern corresponds
to each information set consisting of participant age-sex combination, additional signal and group situation. Rocking without
any other gestures functions as a sign of tolerance or appeasement to access. This suggests that communicative behaviors involving
rocking have a kind of segmentive structure, i.e., rocking represents access while other additional information sets represent
the interaction pattern, the character of which is analogous to linguistic communication. Rocking in most interactions possibly
derived from rocking in courtship behavior which appears to be a ritualized form of frustrated rocking, while other forms
of rocking may possibly be of different origins. 相似文献
15.
Furuichi Takeshi Idani Gen'ichi Ihobe Hiroshi Kuroda Suehisa Kitamura Koji Mori Akio Enomoto Tomoo Okayasu Naobi Hashimoto Chie Kano Takayoshi 《International journal of primatology》1998,19(6):1029-1043
We analyzed population dynamics and birth seasonality of wild bonobos at Wamba, Democratic Republic of the Congo, based on 20 years of observations (1976–1996). Wamba Bonobo infant mortality is much lower than that reported for chimpanzees. This seemes to be related to several socioecological characteristics of bonobos: the use of abundant fruit and herbaceous foods, larger food patch size, female feeding priority, and the absence of infanticide. The mean interval between live births of 4.8 years is shorter than those reported for chimpanzees, and some females simultaneously carried and nursed two successive offspring. Mother–offspring conflicts, such as refusal of suckling attempts and interference with mothers' copulation, which are common in chimpanzees, are rare in Wamba bonobos. A birth peak seems to occur during the light rainy season from March to May, just after the season with the least rainfall. This timing of births is similar to those reported for chimpanzee populations, and might benefit both mother and offspring by maximizing the amount of time before the next dry season. 相似文献
16.
17.
Vera Walraven Linda van Elsacker Rudolf Verheyen 《Primates; journal of primatology》1995,36(1):145-150
A group of seven pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) was tested for their mirror-image reactions during a ten-day experiment. The time spent viewing the mirror waned quickly.
Little social responses directed towards the mirror were observed. Self-directed behaviors were shown from testday one on.
It was concluded that four out of seven animals could correctly identify their mirror-image, one infant was not (yet) able
to do so, and for two other individuals the results were inconclusive. 相似文献
18.
G Idani 《Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology》1991,57(2):83-95
Social relations and behaviours of adolescent female pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) that migrated between unit groups were studied at Wamba, Za?re. Each immigrant female selected one particular resident female, a 'specific senior female' (SSF) and frequently approached and followed her. Affinitive behaviours (e.g. grooming, genitogenital rubbing) were frequently performed between the immigrant and her SSF. Most affinitive behaviours were more likely to be initiated by the immigrant than by her SSF. The immigrant's relationship with her SSF seemed to facilitate the immigrant's integration into the new unit group. Resident males actively approached and mated with the immigrant females. Copulation also appeared to further the immigrant's stable relationship with the resident males. 相似文献
19.
Takayoshi Kano 《International journal of primatology》1983,4(1):1-31
The pygmy chimpanzees of Yalosidi, near the southeastern limit of the range of this species, differ in group size, food habit, nesting behavior, response to humans, etc., from those of other localities (Wamba, Lomako, and Lake Tumba). It is inferred that the pygmy chimpanzee has a wide range of ecological variation. A comprehensive use of the habitat was characteristic of the Yalosidi pygmy chimpanzees. Every stratum of every vegetation type in their habitat was used by them for feeding, resting, or sleeping. The wide ecological niche of pygmy chimpanzees probably permits their survival in competition with other nonhuman primates of Yalosidi, which are more agile but confined to more limited niches. 相似文献
20.
Suehisa Kuroda 《Primates; journal of primatology》1980,21(2):181-197
The affinitive interactions among the pygmy chimps seem to be the most frequent in the male/female combination, next in the female/female, and the least frequent in the male/male. The differences of frequencies among the combinations, however, are little, and it may be said that the pygmy chimps are not so much biased by sex in the affinitive relations. Their aggressive behavior is mild, and their behavior in general shows high tolerance, especially between different sexes and among females. Similar tendency is also seen in grouping (Kuroda, 1979). Therefore, the pygmy chimp’s society may be said to be integrated by the male/female affinity, little differentiated by sex in the grouping and the affinitive relations, and with high inter-individual tolerance and coherence. 相似文献