共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Social context affects risk taking by a satellite species in a mixed-species foraging group 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
Mixed-species flocks of birds form during winter in the easterndeciduousforests of North America. These flocks consist oftwo flock-leading nuclearspecies, tufted titmouse (Baeolophusbicolor) and Carolina chickadee(Poecile carolinensis), andseveral follower, or satellite, species,including downy woodpecker(Picoides pubescens) and white-breastednuthatch (Sitta carolinensis).Hypotheses explaining the adaptivenessof participation in suchmixed-species foraging groups have focused onincreased foragingsuccess and/or decreased predation risk. We tested thepredictionthat if nuthatches join nuclear species to reduce predationrisk,they should be more reluctant to visit an exposed feederin the absence oftitmice than in their presence. When the feederwas positioned 16 m fromforest cover, latency to visit thefeeder was greater for both male and femalenuthatches whentitmice were absent. Removal of titmice had no effect onlatencyat 8 m. In the absence of titmice, nuthatches visited the feederlessfrequently at both distances. These results indicate thatreduced predationrisk is a benefit that satellite species gainby flocking with nuclearspecies. 相似文献
4.
Vigilance is amongst the most universal of anti‐predator strategies and commonly declines with increasing group size. We experimentally manipulated predation risk in a system with a known relationship between group size and vigilance levels to explore whether this relationship changes in response to elevated predation risk. We investigated the vigilance levels of Egyptian geese Alopochen aegyptiaca at eight golf courses in the western Cape, South Africa, to assess the perception of and reaction to predation risk. We manipulated predation risk by introducing trained Harris's hawks Parabuteo unicintus where avian predation was otherwise low or absent. The study confirmed the typical reduction in vigilance with group size on control sites, where the risk of predation is low. However, at experimental sites with elevated predation risk, a positive relationship between vigilance and group size was observed. We hypothesize that the mechanism for this relationship might be linked to social information transfer via copying behaviour and manipulation to induce vigilance. Thus, larger groups will have a higher probability of containing individuals with experience of elevated predation risk and their increased vigilance behaviour is copied by naïve individuals. This prediction is based on the intended outcome of introducing avian predation to make the geese feel less safe and to eventually leave the site as a management tool for controlling nuisance geese. 相似文献
5.
6.
Olivier Pays Alice Ekori Hervé Fritz 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》2014,120(12):1207-1216
Prey can obtain valuable benefits from associating with other species if heterospecifics help to detect predators or locate good food patches. In mixed‐species groups, how species respond to the presence of other species remains a poorly explored question although it might give crucial insights into mechanisms underlying the interspecific coexistence. We studied temporary mixed‐species groups of large herbivores in Hwange National Park (Zimbabwe) between the common impala (Aepyceros melampus), the focal species here, and bigger species including the plains zebra (Equus quagga), the greater kudu (Tragelaphus strepsiceros) or the blue wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus). In the Hwange savanna, the focal and smaller species are exposed to a larger range of predators than the associated species. In this context, we investigated how impalas adjusted their vigilance with group size comparing impala‐only and mixed‐species groups and whether the identity of heterospecifics affected vigilance of impalas. Our study showed that the time impalas spent in vigilance significantly decreased with group size when they formed impala‐only groups, whereas it did not significantly vary with group size in mixed‐species groups. Moreover, in mixed‐species groups, impalas did not adjust their time spent in vigilance with the proportion of conspecifics and the identity of the associated species. Thus, the mechanism underlying the difference of impalas' behavioural adjustment of vigilance with group size between single‐ and mixed‐species groups seemed to be related to the presence but not to the number and the identity of heteropecifics. Finally, we discuss the concept that larger and dominant heterospecifics were likely to increase competition for food access, thereby forcing higher vigilance of impalas, outweighing any reduction from collective vigilance. 相似文献
7.
Antipredatory vigilance usually decreases in groups. The generallyaccepted \"collective detection\" explanation implies that becausethere are more eyes to scan the surroundings for predators,individuals in a group can lower their personal investment invigilance without increasing their predation risk. The roleof other factors, such as numerical risk dilution caused bythe mere presence of companions, has been neglected. In a model,we explore a dilution game when foragers in groups have accessto protective cover. We show that foragers can take advantageof risk dilution and that this leads to changes in vigilancewith group size without the need to invoke collective detection.We identify a cost to maintaining high levels of vigilance asless vigilant foragers gather food faster and so depart thegroup sooner (to reach cover) leaving more vulnerable stragglersbehind. In groups, there is a scramble to reach safe sites thatcan induce a reduction in vigilance levels. Such a mechanismoperates less forcefully in large groups because individualsin these groups are less vulnerable to the departure of an individual.We also demonstrate that individuals should adopt lower levelsof vigilance, to reach safe sites sooner, when predator evasionis compromised or when the rate of food intake is high. Themodel provides new insights into the mechanisms underlying changesin vigilance with group size in animals. 相似文献
8.
9.
10.
Nobuyuki Kutsukake 《International journal of primatology》2007,28(4):907-918
Diurnal primates rely on visual monitoring behavior to collect various kinds of ecological and social information. Vigilance
behavior is monitoring specifically to detect external threats. Previous studies of vigilance behavior were focused mainly
on the influence of predation threats, whereas the influences of conspecific factors, such as intragroup threats, have been
relatively unstudied. Individual vigilance is predicted to be inversely related to the group size or the number of individuals
nearby if the main target of the vigilance is a predation threat and positively related if the main target of the vigilance
is a conspecific threat. I studied wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, and measured the vigilance duration when they are resting on the ground via
2-min focal observation. In both males and females, vigilance duration increased as the number of individuals nearby increased.
This result agrees with the idea that the chimpanzees are vigilant toward other group members. In addition, maternal vigilance
monitors and protects the safety of dependent offspring as the duration of maternal vigilance was longer when a dependent
infant was separated from its mother than when the offspring was in contact with its mother. The results indicate that the
vigilance behavior in wild chimpanzees was affected by conspecific factors. 相似文献
11.
Mixed-species primate groups in the kibale forest: Ecological constraints on association 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Five species of diurnal primates in the Kibale Forest of western Uganda— red colobus (Colobus badius),black- and- white colobus (Colobus guereza),redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius),blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis),mangabeys (Cercocebus albigena)-often associate in mixed- species groups that vary in size and composition from day to day. Across this range of species,
we found no consistent effect of association on feeding rate. In addition, there is no systematic difference between the species-
specific individual feeding rates when animals were in mixed- species groups feeding in a specific tree on one day and when
individuals of one of the same species were feeding in the same individual tree on a subsequent day. If associating in a mixed-
species group lowers the risk of predation, one might expect that the number of vigilant events would decrease in mixed- species
groups. However, the only species to exhibit a consistent decrease in vigilant behavior when in association was the red colobus.
Redtail monkeys were more vigilant when in association. We predicted that the density and distribution of food resources would
both constrain the frequency of association and the size of mixed- species groups. Based on 22 months of data on food resources
and bimonthly censuses, we found no relationship between the frequency of association (except mangabeys) or mean mixed- species
group size and the density and distribution of food resources for all species. Finally, we examined the behavior of the monkeys
in and out of association before and after the playback of a crowned hawk eagle call (Spizaetus coronatus),a known predator. When more species were in association, the amount of time they spent being vigilant following the playback
was greater and the response more intense than when fewer species were in association or when the group was alone. The results
of this study illustrate that the nature of the costs and benefits of polyspecific associations for these different monkey
species are complex and vary from species to species. 相似文献
12.
Because "odd" individuals often suffer disproportionately highrates of predation, solitary individuals should join groupswhose members are most similar to themselves in appearance.We examined group-choice decisions by individuals in armoredand nonarmored species and predicted that either (1) the oddityeffect would result in preference for conspecific groups forsolitary individuals of both species, or (2) individuals inthe armored species would prefer to associate with groups containingindividuals of the more vulnerable species. Armored brook sticklebacks(Culaea inconstans) and nonarmored fathead minnows (Pimephalespromelas) have the same predators and often occur together instreams. In mixed-species shoals, yellow perch (Perca flavescens)attacked minnows earlier and more often than sticklebacks. Wetested whether solitary minnows and sticklebacks preferred toassociate with conspecific or heterospecific shoals under conditionsof both low and high predation risk. When predation risk washigh, minnows preferred to associate with conspecifics overheterospecifics, as predicted by the oddity effect. In contrast,sticklebacks preferentially associated with groups of minnowsover groups of conspecifics when predation risk was high. Whenpredation risk was low, solitary individuals of both speciespreferentially associated with conspecific over heterospecificshoals. Stickleback shoal choices under low-risk conditionsmay have been influenced by interspecific competition for food.In feeding experiments, minnows were more efficient foragersthan sticklebacks, so it should benefit sticklebacks to avoidminnows unless predation risk is high. Therefore, for armoredprey, the benefits of associating with more vulnerable preyappear to override the costs of both the oddity effect and foodcompetition when predation risk is high. 相似文献
13.
By forming larger sizes of groups, individuals benefit from a decrease in vigilance, but the collective vigilance of the group as a whole is not compromised. We examined whether this group size effect is apparent in mixed-species groups of red-bellied tamarins (S. labiatus) and saddleback tamarins (S. fuscicollis) which form stable and permanent associations in the wild. We studied general vigilance and responses to hidden threatening stimuli in five captive groups of each species, while they were housed in single- and mixed-species groups. For vigilance, the individual rate was lower in the larger mixed-species groups than in the smaller single-species groups. In addition, the amount of time when at least one individual was vigilant was higher in mixed-species groups. This suggests that the tamarins alter their vigilance behavior in the presence of the other species. In response to hidden threats, both species performed brief vigilance checks and frequencies of checking did not differ in single- and mixed-species groups. However, both species had a significant reduction in the mean duration per check, and there was a reduced total amount of time spent vigilance checking in the mixed-species groups compared to the single-species groups, demonstrating the group size effect. Overall the mixed-species groups had a higher number and mean duration of checking than the smaller single-species groups. Given that the two species share a common set of predators, and respond to each other's alarm calls, these findings provide strong evidence that individuals of both tamarin species may be able to benefit from forming mixed-species groups via improved vigilance and monitoring of threats. 相似文献
14.
This study investigated the costs and benefits of flocking in white-fronted geese Anser albifrons foraging on rice grains in Japan. The time budgets of focal geese were recorded, and the effects of flock size on the proportions of time spent in vigilant and agonistic behaviour were tested. The results showed that the decline in vigilance level and consequent increase in foraging time were beneficial results of flocking whereas agonistic interactions, a potential cost of flocking, did not increase with increasing flock size. However, seasonal variation in flock size suggested that exploitative competition could be a cost of flocking; the sizes of flocks in spring, when resource depletion had progressed, were significantly reduced compared with those in autumn. An experimental increase in rice density resulted in a significant increase in flock size. We conclude that the flock size of foraging white-fronted geese is a result of compromise between a constant benefit of flocking (i.e. decline in vigilance level) and a cost of flocking varying with food abundance (i.e. exploitative competition). 相似文献
15.
16.
17.
18.
Christine Errard 《Journal of Insect Behavior》1994,7(1):83-99
The purpose of this study was to determine the role of early social experience on the ontogeny of kin and nestmate recognition in ants by means of both behavioral and chemical analysis. Workers of two ant species,Manica rubida (Myrmicinae) andFormica selysi (Formicinae), were reared in homospecific groups (control) or in artificial heterospecific groups (mixed), created less than 5 h after their emergence. Recognition was evaluated between unfamiliar individuals of different species reared in control and mixed groups for periods of 5, 12, 24, 48, and 72 h and 8, 15, 30, 60, or 90 days after emergence. Heterospecifically reared individuals of both species gradually became tolerant of allospecific individuals from control groups. Moreover, homospecifically reared individuals did not aggress allospecific individuals reared in mixed groups. During the course of familiarization between the species, there were modifications of the chemical recognition signals. In mixed groups, hydrocarbon profiles of both species acquire gradually some of the components characteristic of their heterospecific nestmates. These experiments showed that allospecific recognition required the acquisition of a minimal quantity of allospecific cues. The phenomenon provided an another example of the relationship between tolerance and the chemical cues displayed by both species. The results suggested that the individual recognized the allospecific cues borne on each individual's body surface and/or that each individual learned and memorized allospecific cues during its early life. Therefore, each individual might develop a template encoding the allospecific and the conspecific cues to characterize nestmates. 相似文献
19.