共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
V. P. Bingman A. Gagliardo P. Ioalé 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1996,179(5):695-702
The orientation of phase-shifted control and hippocampal lesioned homing pigeons with previous homing experience was examined to investigate the possible participation of the hippocampal formation in sun compass orientation. Hippocampal lesioned pigeons displayed appropriate shifts in orientation indicating that such birds possess a functional sun compass that is used for orientation. However, their shift in orientation was consistently larger than in control pigeons revealing a difference in orientation never observed in pigeons that have not undergone a phase shift. Although alternative interpretations exist, the data suggest the intriguing possibility that following a change in the light-dark cycle, the hippocampal formation participates in the re-entrainment of a circadian rhythm that regulates sun compass orientation. 相似文献
2.
Experiments were performed to test whether melatonin plays a role in sun-compass orientation of homing pigeons. Birds were kept for a period of time in dim continuous light (LL) or in artificial light-dark (LD) cycles and then released under the sun from unfamiliar sites. Control birds in dim LL were oriented homeward in all cases. Birds with melatonin implants in LD were capable of a correct use of the sun compass at release. Birds with melatonin implants in dim LL, on the contrary, performed very poorly in orientation. The present results demonstrate for the first time that melatonin is involved in the control of the circadian rhythms underlying sun-compass orientation in birds. 相似文献
3.
Summary The sun navigation hypothesis ofMatthews is briefly reviewed with special reference to his sun occlusion experiment which supports this hypothesis. The results of a similar experiment are presented in which 13 young pigeons were kept from sight of the sun during a seven day period including the fall equinox while the sun height at noon decreased 3° 6.7. When the birds were released from a point 93 km. south of their loft they were exposed to a noon sun height equal to that of a point north of their home loft eight days earlier. Nevertheless they showed a northward orientation as did the 10 control birds with no difference in homing performance between the experimental and control groups. These results are not in agreement with those ofMatthews and do not support his sun navigation hypothesis.
Zusammenfassung Die Sonnennavigations-Hypothese vonMatthews wird kurz besprochen, besonders im Hinblick auf den Warte-Versuch (Sun occlusion experiment), der unter anderem zur Prüfung der Hypothese angestellt wurde. Die Ergebnisse eines im Wilhelmshavener Gebiet angestellten Warteversuchs werden mitgeteilt: 13 Tauben wurden um die Herbst-Tagundnachtgleiche für 7 Tage ohne Sicht der Sonne gehalten, deren Mittagshöhe in dieser Zeit um 3° 6,7 abnahm. Als die Vögel 93 km südlich ihres Heimatorts aufgelassen wurden, befanden sie sich im Gebiet einer Mittags-Sonnenhöhe gleich der, welche vor der Warteperiode etwa 2° nördlich vom Heimatort herrschte. Trotzdem flogen sie gleich den Kontrollen nordwärts ab. Auch im Heimkehrerfolg bestand kein Unterschied zwischen Versuchs-und Kontrolltauben. Diese Ergebnisse stimmen mit den vonMatthews berichteten nicht überein und stützen seine Sonnen-Navigations-Hypothese nicht.相似文献
4.
5.
Francesca Odetti 《Animal behaviour》2003,66(6):1093-1099
Experiments have shown that pigeons, Columba livia, are able to develop navigational abilities even if reared and kept confined in an aviary, provided that they are exposed to the natural winds. In contrast, pigeons reared in a wind-screened aviary do not learn to navigate. Pigeons kept confined in a screened aviary when young do not learn to navigate even if, once they become adult, the screens are removed and the birds are exposed to natural winds for a period sufficiently long for map learning. In all of these experiments, pigeons were kept confined until the day of test release. In the present study we found that pigeons reared in a wind-screened aviary developed navigational abilities if, once adult, they were allowed to perform spontaneous flights around the loft. Nevertheless, their navigational performance never reached the level of controls.Copyright 2003 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 相似文献
6.
7.
Dybus A Pijanka J Cheng YH Sheen F Grzesiak W Muszyńska M 《Journal of applied genetics》2006,47(1):63-66
A total of 445 domestic pigeons were genotyped for the lactate dehydrogenase (LDHA) gene. Crude DNA was isolated from blood samples and feathers. Two polymorphic sites were identified in intron 6: one near the splice donor site GT is called site H and the other near the splice acceptor site is called site B. Interestingly, the nucleotide changes of both these sites associate perfectly with the A and B alleles of HaeIII polymorphism: the A allele with nucleotide A of site H and nucleotide T of site B; while the B allele with nucleotide G of site H and nucleotide G of site B. In this study, we have identified the molecular difference between alleles A and B of the pigeon LDHA gene. The difference at site H in intron 6 explains the HaeIII polymorphism. The frequencies of LDHAAB and LDHABB genotypes between the analysed groups differ significantly (P < 0.001); the LDHAA allele was more frequent in the groups of pigeons with elevated homing performance (P < 0.001). The functional difference may be due to the change at site B, the potential splice branch site. Since LDHA activity is associated with the homing ability, it is possible that during the process of selection for the homing ability, the LDHAA allele has been selected, and is more prevalent in the top-racing groups. 相似文献
8.
R. Bordel E. Haase 《Journal of comparative physiology. B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology》1993,163(3):219-224
The influence of flight and flight duration on 13 blood parameters was studied in homing pigeons which returned after 2–22 h of flight from release sites 113–620 km away. The haematocrit value decreased from 54.4% in controls to 51.0% in the flown birds. A lowered haematocrit overproportionately improves blood flow. The plasma concentrations of glucose and l(+)-lactate did not differ between experimental and control birds. This is compatible with the idea that carbohydrates are utilized as fuel mainly in the initial phase of flight. Plasma free fatty acid levels were significantly increased during flight and triglyceride concentrations gradually decreased with progressive flight duration. These findings support the view that lipids are the main energy source during flight. Plasma uric acid concentrations were increased two- to fourfold in flown birds. Urea levels gradually rose with flight duration to 400% of controls. Plasma protein concentration was lowered in flown pigeons. These results hint to an increased protein degradation during flight. Na+, K+, Ca2+, and Mg2+ levels in the plasma of the flown pigeons were not significantly different from control values. This finding together with the urea/uric acid ratio indicates that no severe dehydration occurred in our pigeons during free-range flight.Abbreviations FA
fatty acids
- FFA
free fatty acids 相似文献
9.
We adapted a technique to explore the social transmission of spatial information in homing pigeons Columba livia. Five demonstrator pigeons were first trained to find a food goal within an indoor arena. This arena consisted of nine lidded cups laid out within a 12x12 grid on the floor. The task was to find the goal cup and flip the lid to obtain the food hidden within. Once the demonstrators had reached criterion the experiment proper began. During stage 1 of the experiment, 10 target birds, which had not previously been trained to find the goal, were introduced to the spatial task either in isolation or paired with a demonstrator. We measured how long they took to complete the task, the number of squares crossed on the grid, and the number of incorrect lids flipped. In stage 2, the target birds were introduced to the arena a second time, by themselves, and we compared the performance of the birds in the two treatments. The pigeons that had been introduced to the task with a demonstrator in stage 1 walked further and made more incorrect choices when searching for the food goal in stage 2 than the pigeons that were introduced to the task alone. This indicates that pigeons learn a spatial, food-finding task more effectively when performing the task alone than when accompanied by a knowledgeable conspecific. We discuss possible reasons for this in the light of previous experiments. Copyright 1999 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. 相似文献
10.
Gagliardo A Odetti F Ioalè P 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2001,268(1480):2065-2070
Whether pigeons use visual landmarks for orientation from familiar locations has been a subject of debate. By recording the directional choices of both anosmic and control pigeons while exiting from a circular arena we were able to assess the relevance of olfactory and visual cues for orientation from familiar sites. When the birds could see the surroundings, both anosmic and control pigeons were homeward oriented. When the view of the landscape was prevented by screens that surrounded the arena, the control pigeons exited from the arena approximately in the home direction, while the anosmic pigeons' distribution was not different from random. Our data suggest that olfactory and visual cues play a critical, but interchangeable, role for orientation at familiar sites. 相似文献
11.
12.
We carried out magnetic and nonmagnetic experiments on fresh, upper-beak skin tissue samples isolated from six pairs of homing pigeons to test whether the tissue contains magnetite particles. Results of (1) room-temperature isothermal remanent magnetization (IRM) acquisition and alternating field (AF) demagnetization, (2) low-temperature demagnetization of saturation IRM acquired at 5 K in a field of 5 tesla (T) (SIRM5 K) after zero-field cooled (ZFC) and field cooled (FC) treatments, and (3) cycling of the saturation IRM acquired at 300 K in a field of 5 T (SIRM300 K) between 5 and 300 K, indicate the presence of magnetite in the measured samples. A significant loss of SIRM5 K below 20 K suggests the dominance of superparamagnetic (SPM) particles. The SIRM acquisition capacity of the female pigeon is stronger than that of the male pigeon in all four measured pairs, suggesting for the first time that the magnetite concentration is probably sex dependent. Light microscopic observation on the histological sections stained with Prussian Blue detected the presence of some tiny, dotted, dark-blue staining Fe3+ aggregates (size 1–4 μm) located directly beneath the subcutis within strands of connective tissue, nearby the rim of the regions full of red nuclei. The results of this study support the idea that homing pigeons may have a magnetite-based receptor, which potentially could be used for sensing the Earth’s magnetic field during navigation. 相似文献
13.
Experimental resection of the olfactory nerve in the homing pigeon induces a total degeneration of the nerve and olfactory epithelium. The orthograde degenerative process starts before the retrograde one. Ten days after resection, new neurons begin to differentiate from the basal cells. The axon forms earlier than the distal dendritic process, and the speed of growth increases slowly. The regenerated axons only reach the bulb in the 5th month. Two months after resection the olfactory epithelium is similar to that of the intact control side. The ultrastructural features of the mucosa and olfactory axons are similar to those of normal ones. 相似文献
14.
15.
R. Schwilch L. Jenni S. Jenni-Eiermann 《Journal of comparative physiology. B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology》1996,166(2):77-87
This study examines metabolic changes occurring during short to endurance flights and during subsequent recovery in free-flying pigeons, in particular the change towards lipid utilization with increasing flight duration, lipid supply to the flight muscles, protein utilization and the time needed to metabolically recover. Eight plasma metabolite concentrations were measured in homing pigeons released from sites 20–200 km from the loft (0.3–4.8 h flight duration) just after landing and after keeping birds fasting at rest for 30 and 60 min, respectively, after their return. Birds kept in the loft fasting at rest were used as controls. Plasma free fatty acid and glycerol concentrations increased rapidly with flight duration and leveled off after about 1.5 h. This indicates a marked change towards a high and stable lipid utilization from adipose tissues within 1–2 h of flight. Plasma triglyceride levels and very-low-density lipoproteins were decreased after short flights, but subsequently regained or surpassed fasting levels at rest. This indicates that re-esterification of free fatty acids and delivery as very-low-density lipoproteins to the flight muscles to circumvent constraints of fatty acid supply, as described previously for small passerines, is not as significant in the pigeon which has a much lower mass-specific energy rate. An initial increase in plasma glucose levels and a transient decrease to fasting levels at rest was observed and may reflect the initial use and subsequent exhaustion of glycogen stores. Contrary to other birds and mammals, -hydroxy-butyrate levels increased markedly with flight duration. This may suggest a more important sparing of carbohydrates and protein as gluconeogenic precursors in the pigeon than in other species. Plasma uric acid levels increased linearly up to about 4 h flight duration. This indicates an accelerated protein breakdown during flight which may primarily serve to deliver amino acids as glucogenic precursors and citrate cycle intermediates. With increasing flight duration, the energy sources change from an initial phase based primarily on carbohydrates to a lipid-based endurance phase. It is discussed whether this metabolic change depends on the level of power output or the performed work (energy spent) since the start of flight. During the first hour of recovery, most metabolites reached or approached fasting levels at rest, indicating a marked reduction in lipolysis and protein breakdown. -hydroxy-butyrate levels remained at flight levels and glucose levels increased slightly, indicating a restoration of glycogen stores.Abbreviations
VLDL
very-low-density lipoproteins
-
FFA
free fatty acids 相似文献
16.
17.
18.
The magnetic map of homing pigeons: an evergreen phantom 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Wallraff HG 《Journal of theoretical biology》1999,197(2):265-269
19.
We describe an experiment that uses the grouping tendencies and navigational abilities of the homing pigeon (Columba livia) to investigate the possibility of socially mediated information transfer in a field setting. By varying the composition of paired-release types, we allowed some naive birds to receive an accurate demonstration of the home route whilst others were paired with similarly naive conspecifics. After this 'paired phase', we predicted that if any learning of spatial information occurred then naive members of the former pairs would outperform their untutored conspecifics when re-released individually during the subsequent 'single phase' of the experiment. This prediction was not confirmed. Neither homing speed nor initial orientation was superior in individually released tutored versus untutored birds, despite the fact that both performance measures were better in the earlier 'paired phase' with experienced demonstrators. Our results suggest that although naive homing pigeons clearly interact with their experienced partners, they are unable to transfer any individually useful spatial information to subsequent homing flights. 相似文献
20.
Hans G. Wallraff 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》1986,159(2):143-159
Summary 1,324 vanishing bearings of individual pigeons, completely inexperienced in homing, were recorded during 100 releases at 36 sites symmetrically distributed around their home loft near Würzburg at distances ranging from 7 to 180 km. Two directional components could be derived from the data: (a) a weak but significant homeward component pointing to the loft site and (b) a more pronounced compass component pointing to the northwest (Fig. 1). With the latter kind of tendency the pigeons clearly demonstrated what earlier had been described as preferred compass direction (PCD).Homeward directedness at the 36 differently situated release sites is negatively correlated with angular divergence between PCD and homeward direction (Fig. 2). Compass preferences derived from different quartets of symmetrically arranged sites were similar. Over a period of 11 years the PCD varied significantly in time but did not change fundamentally (Figs. 3, 4). Interrelations between various parameters of initial-orientation data were investigated by correlation analyses (Table 3).PCDs have not only been found in the Würzburg area but around many other loft sites as well. They are, however, differently pronounced, and in experienced pigeons they are generally weaker than in first-flight birds. Various reasons (partly evident, partly thought possible) for variability of PCDs, and for their absence in some series of experiments, are discussed.As a PCD persists, and even tends to become clearer, after elimination of homeward orientation by olfactory deprivation, it is concluded that it reflects directional tendencies which are independent of the process of site localization. A great deal of what is called release-site biases, i.e., site-specific deviations from the beeline course towards home, can be understood on a PCD basis. The functional background of the PCD is unknown. Hypothetical advantages for the pigeons homing strategy are considered.Abbreviations
RSB
release-site bias
-
PCD
preferred compass direction 相似文献