首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
A group of seven pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) was tested for their mirror-image reactions during a ten-day experiment. The time spent viewing the mirror waned quickly. Little social responses directed towards the mirror were observed. Self-directed behaviors were shown from testday one on. It was concluded that four out of seven animals could correctly identify their mirror-image, one infant was not (yet) able to do so, and for two other individuals the results were inconclusive.  相似文献   

2.
Two male Japanese monkeys used a mirror to inspect an object attached to their bodies but not directly visible. These monkeys had been trained previously to use a mirror to guide their hand to a target. In Experiment 1 their behavior in the presence of a mirror was observed. In Experiment 2 the monkeys used the mirror to locate a picture projected on a screen to the left or right rear side of the cage. In Experiment 3 the monkeys used a mirror to observe and finally grasp an object attached behind their heads. Two monkeys who were not trained to use a mirror to obtain an otherwise hidden object did not show such behavior.  相似文献   

3.
Studies of non-human primate self-recognition in mirrors demonstrate variation both within and between species. This study applied a rigorous methodology that took into account habituation of subjects to the mirror as an object and to the experimental situation. The species observed in our study was Miopithecus talapoin, which has been little studied in the wild or in captivity. Although this species shows several interesting characteristics, including complex social organisation and a high encephalization index, the talapoin monkeys in the study did not pass the mark test; however, they showed a prerequisite for self-recognition, namely comparing their body parts to the image of these in the mirror.  相似文献   

4.
All animals must acquire food and mates by approaching them despite possibilities of accompanying risks and thus are frequently encountered with approach-avoidance conflicts in daily lives. Behavioral individual differences in such situations may be considered as one of the most biologically fundamental personality trains. “Partitioned raisin test” was devised to assess this trait with macaque monkeys. It involved throwing raisins into groups of monkeys and observing the preferred distance of each from the human feeder, a source of possible harm. The test was administered to 4 groups of Japanese monkeys (30 total) and 3 groups of rhesus monkeys (19 total), all l-yr-old and matched in history. Individual differences in the preferred proximity to the feeder, as expressed by the Proximity Index (PI), were found in both species.PI was not correlated with a measure of dominance over the raisins. Individual differences inPI were also not due to territorial effects unrelated to the location of the feeder.PI was stable in five of the six monkeys re-tested after one year of interval in a newly organized group, where there supposedly had been a change in their social structure. Partitioned raisin test was shown to be capable of depicting individual differences related to differential approach bias in an approach-avoidance conflict situation. Although possible confounding effects by social factors need to be delineated in the following studies, the method may provide a handy and widely applicable way for the assessment of this trait with monkeys.  相似文献   

5.
6.
I studied long- tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis)feeding behavior and ecology as part of a larger behavioral ecological study at the Natai Lengkuas Station, Tanjung Puting National Park, Kalimantan Tengah, Indonesia. I collected data on feeding behavior via scan sampling of all visible individuals in the focal group (approximately 800 observation hours). I established vegetational plots and monitored them monthly to determine food availability and abundance. I found long- tailed macaques to be primarily frugivorous;leaves, flowers, insects and bark provided the remainder of the diet. They used at least 33 plant species as food sources, but >60% of the diet was provided by only 5 species. Based on previous vegetational analyses, these tree species were among the highest in relative density. However, selection ratios for 19 food species indicate that 13 of them were selected more often than expected. Long- tailed macaques appear to be selective feeders but can exploit a variety of food sources during periods of food scarcity.  相似文献   

7.
The daily change in both percentage and absolute number of circulating major lymphocyte subset was determined with young Japanese monkeys and rhesus monkeys. The blood sample was collected at four hour-intervals beginning at 16:00 for 24 hours under the condition of applying tethering system by which blood samples could be collected without restraint. During the dark period (from 20:00 to 08:00), the number of peripheral lymphocytes increased and that of granulocytes decreased, resulting in no significant change in the number of total peripheral white blood cells. The absolute number of CD4 + T, CD8 + T, and CD20 + B cells showed the significant daily change similar to that in number of peripheral lymphocytes, indicating no proportional change in these subsets. The typical proportional change was observed in CD16 + natural killer (NK) cells and the percentage of CD16 + cells decreased during dark period (from 20:00 to 04:00) and increased in the morning (from 08:00 to 12:00). The NK activity determined by killing K562 target cells showed the same changing pattern as that of percentage in CD16+ NK cells. The changing pattern of both percentage and activity of NK cells was consistent with that of plasma cortisol levels. In addition, the intravenous injection of 300 μg/kg of cortisol induced increase in plasma cortisol levels and decrease in percentage of CD16 + NK cells during the first 60 min after cortisol injection. These results strongly suggest that the levels of peripheral functional CD16 + NK cells might be directly regulated by plasma cortisol level in macaque monkeys.  相似文献   

8.
The specific aim of this study was to determine the effects of gonadotropins in vitro upon the incidence of and precise time interval to germinal vesicle breakdown (GVB) and extrusion of the first polar body (PB1) in oocytes from nonstimulated rhesus monkeys. Cumulus-enclod germinal vesicle (GV) stage oocytes from 10 normal, cycling rhesus monkeys in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle were cultured with either: (1) 1.0 μg/ml human follicle-stimulating hormone (hFSH), (2) 10 μg/ml human luteinizing hormone (hLH), (3) 1.0 μg/ml hFSH and 10 μg/ml hLH, or (4) no gonadotropins (controls). Oocytes (n = 234) were examined at 3-hr intervals from 0 to 21 hr and at 4-hr intervals from 24 to 52 hr for GVB and PB1. Neither the incidence of GVB (hFSH: 63.5%; hLH: 56.1%; both gonadotropins: 63.1%; no gonadotropins: 53.6%) nor extrusion of PB1 (hFSH: 41.3%; hLH: 36.4%; both gonadotropins: 36.9%; no gonadotropins; 31.9%) differed (P > 0.05) among treatments. The time to GVB was accelerated (P < 0.05) by gonadotropins (hFSH: 10.8 ± 1.7 hr; hLH: 10.1 ± 1.8 hr; both gonadotropins: 8.8 ± 1.1 hr) when compared to controls (17.4 ± 2.0 hr). However, the time interval to extrusion of PB1 did not differ (P > 0.05) among treatments (hFSH: 32.3 ± 1.2 hr; hLH: 35.1 ± 1.4 hr; both gonadotropins: 35.2 ± 1.3 hr; no gonadotropins: 34.1 ± 1.2 hr). The mean interval to extrusion of PB1 was 34.1 ± 0.6 hr. In conclusion, GVB and PB1 extrusions appear to be, in part, independently regulated events in macaque oocytes matured in vitro since the timing of PB1 extrusion is not tightly coupled with the onset of GVB. Although the developmental potential of oocytes may be enhanced by gonadotropins, alternative approaches must be developed to improve the poor competence of oocytes from nonstimulated monkeys to mature in vitro. © 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

9.
10.
Learning and imitation were examined in animals selected from two groups of sixteen pigtail monkeys. There were significant differences in performance on a cued-alternation task as a function of both social status within the stable group, and prior exposure to a social model. High status animals responded more frequently, but were less successful in acquiring appropriate response delay. Exposure to the model improved response latencies and acquisition of response delay for all subjects. However, model exposure did not improve alternation performance. Results are discussed in terms of prior social experience of the subjects, general learning strategies, and differential sensitivity to multiple reinforcement contingencies. Findings are related to ethological concepts of imitation, and field reports on primate social learning.  相似文献   

11.
12.
Summary Part of the beta-globin genes ofMacaca cynomolgus andGorilla gorilla has been cloned and sequenced. Ten putatively neutral nucleotide polymorphisms have been described at the beta-globin locus in humans. They are associated in seven combinations, which define seven different haplotypes of the beta-globin gene: four major frameworks—1, 2, 3, and 3*—and three minor frameworks, which we term KI1, KA1, and OR1. The nucleotide sequences of these frameworks are compared with those of homologous sequences in chimpanzee, colobus, macaque, and gorilla. This comparison provides strong evidence that framework 2 was the earliest framework in the human lineage. From framework 2, a rooted parsimonious tree for the six other frameworks is constructed. This phylogenetic tree is discussed in terms of the evolution of nucleotide polymorphisms as well as in terms of genetic affinities between human populations.For each position at which there is base difference in comparing human, gorilla, and chimpanzee beta-globin genes, the phyletic lineage where the corresponding substitution occurred has been identified using the maximum parsimony procedure. The data provide evidence that polymorphisms may represent a significant component of differences between closely related species. If so, nucleotide polymorphisms may strongly bias estimates of small evolutionary distances.  相似文献   

13.
Several examples have been documented of novel behaviours which have apparently arisen spontaneously in primate groups and then spread through the group by learning. Here we describe the first recorded instance of such an acquired behaviour in a prosimian. The behaviour, consisting of immersing the tail in water and then drinking from the wet tail, was observed in a group of semi free-ranging ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Seventeen of 28 animals showed the behaviour, including adult males. Several animals which did not show the behaviour were observed watching and sometimes sharing the wet tail of animals who did. Several incomplete sequences, notably of non-stimulus directed elements, were also seen in non-performers. It is likely that stimulus enhancement is the mechanism of spread of this behaviour through the group, although the presence of the incomplete sequences suggests that imitation is also a possibility.  相似文献   

14.
Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This chapter reviews results of clinical and functional imaging studies which investigated the time-course of cortical and subcortical activation during the acquisition of motor a skill. During the early phases of learning by trial and error, activation in prefrontal areas, especially in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, is has been reported. The role of these areas is presumably related to explicit working memory and the establishment of a novel association between visual cues and motor commands. Furthermore, motor associated areas of the right hemisphere and distributed cerebellar areas reveal strong activation during the early motor learning. Activation in superior-posterior parietal cortex presumably arises from visuospatial processes, while sensory feedback is coded in the anterior-inferior parietal cortex and the neocerebellar structures. With practice, motor associated areas of the left-hemisphere reveal increased activity. This shift to the left hemisphere has been observed regardless of the hand used during training, indicating a left-hemispheric dominance in the storage of visuomotor skills. Concerning frontal areas, learned actions of sequential character are represented in the caudal part of the supplementary motor area (SMA proper), whereas the lateral premotor cortex appears to be responsible for the coding of the association between visuo-spatial information and motor commands. Functional imaging studies which investigated the activation patterns of motor learning under implicit conditions identified for the first, a motor circuit which includes lateral premotor cortex and SMA proper of the left hemisphere and primary motor cortex, for the second, a cognitive loop which consists of basal ganglia structures of the right hemisphere. Finally, activity patterns of intermanual transfer are discussed. After right-handed training, activity in motor associated areas maintains during performance of the mirror version, but is increased during the performance of the original-oriented version with the left hand. In contrary, increased activity during the mirror reversed action, but not during the original-oriented performance of the untrained right hand is observed after left-handed training. These results indicate the transfer of acquired right-handed information which reflects the mirror symmetry of the body, whereas spatial information is mainly transferred after left-handed training. Taken together, a combined approach of clinical lesion studies and functional imaging is a promising tool for identifying the cerebral regions involved in the process of motor learning and provides insight into the mechanisms underlying the generalisation of actions.  相似文献   

15.
Abstract: This study reports on the endothelial expression of vascular cell adhesion molecule-1 (VCAM-1) in the central nervous system (CNS) early after experimental infection of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with pathogenic and nonpathogenic simian immunodeficiency virus (SIV). Diffuse endothelial expression of VCAM-1 was observed in the CNS in all animals receiving pathogenic SIV These findings demonstrate the rapidity with which pathogenic SIV is able to enter the CNS and induce endothelial cell activation.  相似文献   

16.
17.
Cryopreservation of peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMC) from animal model studies and clinical trials is utilized as a primary method for long-term storage of PBMC for future in vitro and in vivo applications. The objective of this study was to define the mechanistic pathways involved in cryopreservation-induced apoptosis of CD4+ T-cells in PBMC, and to evaluate a cytokine treatment of the cryopreserved samples to rescue apoptosis for the potential future use of the cryopreserved PBMC. Using cryopreserved PBMC samples isolated from na?ve and Simian immunodeficiency virus (SIV)-infected rhesus macaques as a model, frozen PBMC showed significantly increased levels of apoptosis-induced CD4+ T-cell death compared to fresh PBMC over a 5-day culture period as detected by Annexin V/PI and trypan blue staining. Mechanistic studies using a broad-spectrum caspase inhibitor z-VAD demonstrated a crucial involvement of caspases in cryopreservation-induced apoptosis of CD4+ T-cells. Furthermore, the ability of z-VAD to inhibit both mitochondrial membrane perturbation and apoptotic cell death implicated the involvement of caspase-mediated mitochondrial membrane damage in cryopreservation-induced apoptosis of CD4+ T-cells. Due to their known properties to promote T-cell survival and inhibit apoptosis, we evaluated the ability of IL-2, IL-4, and IL-7 combination cytokine treatment of the cryopreserved cells to rescue apoptosis of the CD4+ T-cells. The cytokine treatment resulted in a significant inhibition (p<0.01) of apoptosis-induced cell death and rescued CD4+ T-cell survival (p<0.01) in the cryopreserved cells. Efficient rescue of cryopreserved CD4+ T-cells has clinical significance in immune function analysis of longitudinal samples and in various long-term protocols requiring cryopreservation, including bone marrow and stem cell transplantation.  相似文献   

18.
Background, Aim and Scope  Quite often there is need for precise and representative parameters in LCA studies. Probably the most relevant have direct influence on the functional unit, whose definition is crucial in the conduct of any LCA. Changes in the functional unit show directly in LCI and LCIA results. In comparative assertions, a bias in the functional unit may lead to a bias in the overall conclusions. Since quantitative data for the functional unit, such as geometric dimensions and specific weight, often vary, the question arises how to determine the functional unit, especially if a comparative assertion shall be representative for a region or market. Aim and scope of the study is to develop and apply methods for obtaining precise and representative estimates for the functional unit as one important parameter in an LCA study. Materials and Methods  Statistical sampling is applied in order to get empirical estimates for the weight of yoghurt cups, as a typical parameter for the functional unit. We used a two-stage sampling design, with stratified sampling in the first stage and three different sampling designs in the second stage, namely stratified, clustered, and a posteriori sampling. Sampling designs are motivated and described. In a case study, they are each used to determined a representative weight for 150 g yoghurt cups in Berlin, at the point of sale and within a specific time. In the first sampling stage, food markets are randomly selected, while in the second stage, yoghurt cups in these food markets are sampled. The sampling methods are applicable due to newly available internet data. These data sources and their shortcomings are described. Results  The random sampling procedure yields representative estimates, which are compared to figures for market leaders, i.e. yoghurt cups with very high occurrence in the supermarkets. While single types of yoghurt cups showed moderate uncertainty, representative estimates were highly precise. Discussion results show, for one, the performance of the applied statistical estimation procedures, and they show further that adding more information in the estimation procedure (on the shape of the cup, on the type of plastic, on the specific brand) helps reducing uncertainty. Conclusions  As conclusions, estimates and their uncertainty depend on the measurement procedure in a sensitive manner; any uncertainty information should be coupled with information on the measurement procedure, and it is recommended to use statistical sampling in order to reduce uncertainty for important parameters of an LCA study. Recommendations and Perspectives  Results for market leaders differed considerably from representative estimates. This implies to not use market leader data, or data with a high market share, as substitute for representative data in LCA studies. Statistical sampling has been barely used for Life Cycle Assessment. It turned out to be a feasible means for obtaining highly precise and representative estimates for the weight of yoghurt cups in the case study, based on empirical analysis. Further research is recommended in order to detect which parameters should best be investigated in LCA case studies; which data sources are available and recommended, and which sampling designs are appropriate for different application cases. ESS-Submission Editor: Seungdo Kim. PhD (kimseun@msu.edu)  相似文献   

19.
Development of tool use in a macaque and a gorilla   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
The development of the capacity to use a stick as a tool was tested in a macaque (Macaca fuscata) and a gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) infants that had previously shown to be able to use strings and supports as dragging tools. Subjects were tested between 15 and 38 months of age. Different levels of competence between the subjects emerged over testing. The macaque developed a stereotyped strategy to cope with the problem, only getting random successes, whilst the gorilla developed a flexible strategy and revealed to be able to mentally represent the solution of the problem. In fact, when not successful using the stick, the gorilla thought out an alternative strategy, choosing and adapting a new object to use it as a tool.  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号