首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 796 毫秒
1.
空气污染作为一种有害的环境因素,对人类及动物的生理、心理均有影响.在鸟类中,信鸽(Columba livia)是研究空气污染影响的理想模型.为探究空气污染的行为学效应,通过收集并筛选2018和2019年成都市信鸽协会春秋两个季节举办的64场赛事共285羽参赛5场及以上的信鸽不同空距等级下的归巢速度,利用混合线性模型分析...  相似文献   

2.
Brieftauben     
Homing pigeons Homing pigeons are well known for their excellent homing abilities which allow them to return to their lofts from unknown releasing sites more than hundreds of kilometres away. Several orientation mechanisms – sun compass, earth's magnetic field, olfactory cues, visual cues – are known to be involved in homing performance as well as parameters such as motivation and experience. New technology give an insight in their homing behaviour and track preferences and it is shown that homing pigeons physiology and neurobiology seem to be functionally adapted to homing. Pigeons races are still common and it is shown how the pigeon breeder tries to maximize the success of his pigeons.  相似文献   

3.
Since birds use the earth's magnetic field for compass orientation when astronomical cues are lacking and it has recently been suggested that the pineal body is part of their magnetic compass, test releases have been performed in overcast conditions with pigeons deprived of the pineal body. On the whole, both experimental and control birds were capable of homeward orientation, though the bearings of experimental were rather more scattered. No differences in homing speed or success were recorded. Thus, the pineal body does not appear to play an important role in the homing of pigeons.  相似文献   

4.
BACKGROUND: Anecdotal observations and early airplane and helicopter tracking studies suggest that pigeons sometimes follow large roads and use landmarks as turning points during their homeward journey. However, technical limitations in tracking pigeon routes have prevented proof. RESULTS: Here, we present experimental and statistical evidence for this strategy from the analysis of 216 GPS-recorded pigeon tracks over distances up to 50 km. Experienced pigeons released from familiar sites during 3 years around Rome, Italy, were significantly attracted to highways and a railway track running toward home, in many cases without anything forcing them to follow such guide-rails. Birds often broke off from the highways when these veered away from home, but many continued their flight along the highway until a major junction, even when the detour added substantially to their journey. The degree of road following increased with repeated releases but not flight length. Significant road following (in 40%-50% of the tracks) was mainly observed from release sites along northwest-southeast axis. CONCLUSIONS: Our data demonstrate the existence of a learned road-following homing strategy of pigeons and the use of particular topographical points for final navigation to the loft. Apparently, the better-directed early stages of the flight compensated the added final detour. During early and middle stages of the flight, following large and distinct roads is likely to reflect stabilization of a compass course rather than the presence of a mental roadmap. A cognitive (roadmap) component manifested by repeated crossing of preferred topographical points, including highway exits, is more likely when pigeons approach the loft area. However, it might only be expected in pigeons raised in an area characterized by navigationally relevant highway systems.  相似文献   

5.
To test whether the sun is an essential factor for the development of a functioning orientation system in birds, a group of young pigeons was raised as ‘No-Sun’-birds. They were not allowed to see the sun, and they were released to fly around their loft only under total overcast. The control group had an equal number of opportunities to fly under overcast plus additional flights under sun. When released as untrained birds under solid cloud cover, the ‘No-Sun’-birds were significantly oriented, whereas the controls were not. Small magnets glued between the wings (north toward the head) reversed the ‘No-Sun’-birds' orientation, indicating they used a magnetic compass. These findings show that the orientation system can develop without information from the sun. Differences in the orientation behavior of the ‘No-Sun’-birds and normally raised young pigeons are discussed.  相似文献   

6.
Several workers have investigated the effect of anosmia on pigeon navigation in different geographical locations because it has been suggested that homing behavior is based on different cues, such as olfactory cues, the Earth's magnetic field or infrasound, and that in the absence of one cue another would be used. In this situation, no cue is universally indispensable, including olfactory ones. In order to extend such observations to a novel biome, we observed the behaviour of 192 young inexperienced birds raised in southeastern Brazil, a tropical area where olfactory tests had never been run before. The birds were released from eight symmetrically distributed sites 17 to 44 km from the loft. Half of these birds (experimentals) had been made temporarily anosmic by washing their olfactory mucosae with 4% solution of ZnSO4 the day before release, while controls were treated with Ringer solution. The results of release tests showed that anosmia totally impaired the navigational performance of experimental birds, which were unable to home from sites at relatively short distances from home (34-44 km) and whose pooled initial bearings produced a (negative) homeward component not significantly different from 0. Homing performance of controls was significantly better, and their pooled vanishing bearings had a significant homeward component, in spite of much scatter in individual releases. We conclude that pigeon homing in the study area depends on olfactory information, even though local environmental conditions in the interior of the State of Sao Paulo, as in several other parts of the world, do not appear to be as favorable as Italy for the development of efficient olfactory navigation.  相似文献   

7.
Summary In order to test whether stimuli perceived during passive displacement are important for the subsequent homing, pigeons were transported in an apparatus designed to prevent them from receiving relevant information: The experimental birds were continuously rotated quite rapidly (1.5 cps, radial acceleration about 4 g); in addition, they were exposed to an artificial magnetic field and supplied with bottled air. Control birds were transported in open-air cages on top of the van with free view to all sides.Five pairs of releases from equal distances in opposite directions were conducted. Experienced birds were released at distances of about 15, 90, and 300 km from the loft, inexperienced birds at distances of about 180km. In each pair of releases both groups of pigeons were significantly homeward oriented. Neither in initial orientation nor in homing performance nor in the distribution of recoveries were the experimental birds inferior to the controls or in any perceptable way different from them.It is concluded that homing of passively displaced pigeons is not primarily based on information gathered during the outward journey.Abbreviations EP experimental pigeon(s) - CP control pigeon(s) The possibility to maintain our pigeon loft in a building that belongs to the Zoological Institute (Prof. M. Lindauer) of the University of Würzburg is gratefully acknowledged.  相似文献   

8.
Photovoltaic solar panels represent one of the most promising renewable energy sources, but are strong reflectors of horizontally polarized light. Polarized light pollution (PLP) associated with solar panels causes aquatic insects to prefer to oviposit on panels over natural water bodies, with potential to negatively impact their global populations as solar energy expands. We evaluate the hypothesis that anti-reflective coatings (ARCs) used to increase the energy efficiency of solar panels will reduce the amount of PLP they reflect, and their attractiveness to aquatic insects. We created artificial test surfaces that mimicked the optical properties of coated and uncoated solar panels and exposed them to wild populations of polarotactic mayflies (Ephemeroptera), horseflies (Tabanidae) and non-biting midges (Chironomidae) used as indicators of PLP. We evaluated the reflection-polarization properties of test surfaces from four different angles of view and under sunny and overcast skies in the visible and ultraviolet parts of the spectrum. Matte (i.e. ARC-coated) sunlit solar panels were strong sources of horizontally polarized light only when the sun was afront and behind, in contrast to uncoated panels which exceeded common polarization-sensitivity thresholds for aquatic insects from all four viewing directions. As predicted by these polarization patterns, horsefly numbers and water-seeking behaviors were significantly reduced by ARCs. Under overcast skies, both matte and shiny (i.e. uncoated) panels were insect-detectible sources of PLP. Matteness modestly reduced the degree of polarization of reflected light, but not sufficiently such that fewer chironomids were attracted to them. Mayflies actually preferred matte panels under overcast skies. ARCs are most likely to reduce PLP and benefit aquatic insects under sunny skies and when used in conjunction with white non-polarizing gridding, but may actually exacerbate the severity of their negative effects under overcast conditions. Consequently, even current ARC technology has a role to play in aquatic insect conservation, but strategic deployment of solar panels away from water bodies and temperate regions may trump these benefits.  相似文献   

9.
Snowy Owls Bubo scandiacus often face the sun when sitting on the ground or when perched. Such sun‐orienting has been suggested to represent a visual display to conspecifics but other explanations have not been thoroughly examined. We observed the orientation of wintering Snowy Owls to both the sun and the wind, and their perching behaviour during two winters in central Saskatchewan, Canada. We proposed three new explanations for sun orientation in addition to the display hypothesis: thermoregulation, hunting and defence against predators. On sunny days, 44% of 710 Owls faced the sun; this was non‐random because few did so on overcast days. Sun‐ as opposed to wind‐orienting was strongly associated with weather conditions. Logistic regressions indicated that at temperatures below –13 °C and at wind speeds greater than about 18 km/h, Owls tended to orient to the wind rather than to the sun. The likelihood of wind‐orienting increased if the Owl perched above the ground, whereas the likelihood of sun‐orientating increased slightly when the Owl was sitting on the ground. There was no difference between the sexes in orienting behaviour. Snowy Owls seemed to prioritize wind‐orienting for thermoregulation but the results are also consistent with the idea that sun‐orientation can reduce heat loss. Facing into the sun did not support the hunting explanation because the birds would have been blinded and not able to see prey, but was consistent with the protection explanation if it helps to increase vigilance against enemies. Although we cannot completely rule out the display explanation, the spatial context of sunning Owls and a lack of a sex effect makes it unlikely that this is the main function. Instead, Owls seem to trade‐off wind‐ vs. sun‐orienting according to the prevailing weather conditions and do so mainly to thermoregulate and perhaps to maintain vigilance.  相似文献   

10.
Tracking radar and visual observation techniques were used to observe the orientation of free-flying passerine nocturnal migrants in situations in which potentially usable directional cues were absent or gave conflicting information. When migrants had seen the sun near the time of sunset and/or the stars, they oriented in appropriate migratory directions even when winds were opposed. Under solid overcast skies that prevented a view of both sun and stars, the birds headed downwind in opposing winds and thus moved in seasonally inappropriate directions. The data point to the primacy of visual cues over wind direction, with either sun or stars being sufficient to allow the birds to determine the appropriate migration direction.  相似文献   

11.
Experiments were performed to test whether melatonin plays a role in sun-compass orientation of homing pigeons. Birds were kept for a period of time in dim continuous light (LL) or in artificial light-dark (LD) cycles and then released under the sun from unfamiliar sites. Control birds in dim LL were oriented homeward in all cases. Birds with melatonin implants in LD were capable of a correct use of the sun compass at release. Birds with melatonin implants in dim LL, on the contrary, performed very poorly in orientation. The present results demonstrate for the first time that melatonin is involved in the control of the circadian rhythms underlying sun-compass orientation in birds.  相似文献   

12.
The importance of visual landmarks during homing in pigeons (Columba livia) remains a contentious issue. Three experiments which explore the role of visual landmarks at release sites are reported here. The effects of releasing homing pigeons after a 5-minute period in either a clear or an opaque sided release box were investigated. In the clear sided box pigeons were able to observe local surroundings at a release site, but this view was obstructed in the opaque sided box. In experiment 1 pigeons were released from familiar locations close to home (between 2 and 5.6 km): being unable to view landmarks prior to release significantly slowed homing speeds. In experiment 2 pigeons were released at familiar locations further from home (between 8.4 and 10 km): being unable to view landmarks prior to release did not significantly affect homing speeds. In experiment 3 pigeons were trained to home from distant release sites but were tested at closer, unfamiliar sites located on the likely homing routes used by the pigeons in training. No significant difference in homing speeds were observed when pigeons were released from either the clear or opaque sided box. The significance of these results for understanding the role of visual landmarks within a pigeon's familiar area is discussed.  相似文献   

13.
We examined the relative importance of climatic factors and population density to territorial start time ofVanessa atalanta males. Start time varies with solar altitude and therefore with seasons. We removed seasonal effects by converting start times to corresponding solar altitudes. Start time solar altitude correlates primarily with ambient temperature (T a) and secondarily with substrate temperature (T s), regardless of cloud cover. Overcast cloud cover resulted in later not earlier start times as expected from reduced solar radiation (R) levels. R may affect start time indirectly by affectingT s and later start times under overcast skies may be a result ofT s. Start times under solid overcast but not under broken overcast were different than under clear skies, suggesting thatV. atalanta males can use dim sun or blue patches in broken overcast as a start time cue. Early interaction frequency is correlated withT a and wind direction, but not with start time itself, suggesting that male population density is unimportant compared with climatic factors. We conclude thatV. atalanta has a climate-dependent start time but, also, that maintaining a relatively fixed daily schedule is more important to males than is achieving an optimal body temperature while perching.  相似文献   

14.
The mechanisms used by homing pigeons (Columba livia) to navigate homeward from distant sites have been well studied, yet the mechanisms underlying navigation within, and mapping of, the local familiar area have been largely neglected. In the local area pigeons pote ntially have access to a powerful navigational aid--a memorized landscape map. Current opinion suggests that landmarks are used only to recognize a familiar start position and that the goalward route is then achieved solely using compass orientation. We used high-resolution global positioning system (GPS) loggers to track homing pigeons as they became progressively familiar with a local homing task. Here, we demonstrate that birds develop highly stereotyped yet individually distinctive routes over the landscape, which remain substantially inefficient. Precise aerial route recapitulation implies close control by localized geocentric cues. Magnetic cues are unlikely to have been used, since recapitulation remains despite magnetic disruption treatment, and olfactory cues would have been positionally unstable under the variable wind conditions, making visual landmarks the most likely cues used.  相似文献   

15.
《Behavioural processes》1997,39(2):137-147
A new device, the direction-recorder, offered the possibility to extend earlier studies of homing behaviour of pigeons when relevant topographical elements (mountains and large tracts of water) interpose between the release site and the home loft. Three series of experiments were carried out at three different sites to investigate intraindividual and interindividual variability in subsequent tosses from the same locality. Two release sites were chosen behind a mountain chain with respect to home; at the third site homeward directed route crosses the sea. From our results it turns out that homing pigeons may adopt different strategies. Moreover, a wide intraindividual variability was observed in repeated tosses at the same site; some pigeons remained faithful to the first route, whereas other birds tried successive new routes which, in most cases, were significantly shorter than previous ones. This result indicates that pigeons try, and are actually able, to improve their performance in subsequent releases from the same site.  相似文献   

16.
Previous studies have shown that migrating palmate newts (Lissotriton helveticus) can rely on acoustic cues for orientation to breeding ponds. Nonetheless, although acoustic cues are reliable over relatively short distances, they are unlikely to account for the long‐distance homing demonstrated in several other species of newts. Most individuals of L. helveticus migrate only a few hundred meters (Diego‐Rasilla, F. J. & Luengo, R. M. 2007: Acoustic orientation in the palmate newt, Lissotriton helveticus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 1329—1335), raising the possibility that this species may only utilize short‐distance cues (Joly, P. & Miaud, C. 1993: How does a newt find its pond? The role of chemical cues in migrating newts (Triturus alpestris). Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 5, 447—455; Russell, A. P., Bauer, A. M. & Johnson, M. K. 2005: Migration of amphibians and reptiles: an overview of patterns and orientation mechanisms in relation to life history strategies. In: Migration of Organisms (Elewa, M. T., ed). Springer‐Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg, pp. 151—203; Sinsch, U. 2006: Orientation and navigation in Amphibia. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Phy. 39, 65—71). Therefore, experiments were carried out to investigate the use of the geomagnetic field in the nocturnal homing orientation of L. helveticus. Tests were carried out at night in an outdoor circular arena, under total overcast sky that prevented access to celestial compass cues. Individual newts were tested in one of four symmetrical alignments of an earth‐strength magnetic field. We studied the orientation behaviour of newts from two breeding ponds located 9.05 km west‐southwest and 19 km east‐northeast of the testing site. The distribution of magnetic bearings from both groups of newts exhibited significant orientation in the homeward direction. These findings indicate that palmate newts are capable of long‐distance homing and are able to orient in the homeward direction at night using the magnetic compass as the sole source of directional (i.e., compass) information.  相似文献   

17.
The orientation of displaced homing pigeons at the moment of their release was tested using an octagonal orientation cage. Under certain conditions, the cage bearings of old, experienced pigeons show rather good homeward orientation, while the directional choices of young birds are not related to the home direction.  相似文献   

18.
《Animal behaviour》1988,36(1):150-158
Despite being the most studied of all avian orientation systems, important questions still remain about the sun compass of homing pigeons, Columba livia. White it is well-documented that the sun compass is usually learned by young pigeons during the first 10–12 weeks of life, the mechanism by which it is calibrated to adjust for seasonal changes in the sun's azimuth is not known with certainty. Previous experiments using short-term deflector loft pigeons indicated that the sun compass may be calibrated by referencing celestial polarization patterns. The present paper describes important measurable changes in the previously reported orientation behaviour of short-term deflector loft birds, and suggests a correlation between these changes and the presence of a massive upper-atmospheric dust cloud of volcanic origin which significantly altered natural skylight polarization patterns in 1982 and 1983. Moreover, it is shown that when the short-term effect was absent (at times when data from previous years suggested it should be present), the birds were also not using sun compass orientation, as demonstrated by their failure to show the standard ‘clockshift’ response to a 6-h fast shift of their internal clocks. These results support the hypothesis that reflected light cues, rather than odours, are the basis of the deflector loft effect in pigeon homing.  相似文献   

19.
The amount of solar radiation absorbed by an organism is a function of the intensity of the radiation and the area of the organism exposed to the source of the radiation. Since the prosobranch gastropod Echinolittorina peruviana is longer than it is wide, its areas of the lateral sides are approximately twice as large as the areas of the frontal and dorsal faces. We quantified the orientation of the intertidal prosobranch E. peruviana with respect to the position of the sun and solar heat gain in the different orientations. In the field, 80.9% of the E. peruviana monitored on sunny summer days tended to face the sun frontally or dorsally while only 19.1% faced the sun with the larger lateral sides. On overcast summer or on winter days, this trend was not observed. We then show that the body temperature of individuals increases more rapidly and reaches higher equilibriums when the lateral sides are facing the sun than when they face the sun with either of the smaller frontal or dorsal sides. These results therefore show that the orientation behavior of E. peruviana is thermoregulatory and that it permits the organisms to maintain lower temperatures on hot summer days.  相似文献   

20.
Summary At four sites in the cardinal directions from the home loft in about 180 km distance, 135 experimental pigeons (EPs) and 171 control pigeons (CPs) were released. The EPs had been made anosmic by bilateral olfactory nerve section. All birds were completely inexperienced in homing.Homeward orientation was clearly established in the CPs of which 19% returned to the loft. The performances of the EPs were significantly worse in (a) initial orientation, (b) vanishing intervals, (c) distribution of recoveries, and (d) homing success (none of them homed).The EPs still showed the loft-specific preferred compass direction (PCD).The recoveries of the EPs are much more widely scattered in direction than those of the rather well homeward oriented CPs, even if the distances from the release site are the same in both groups. It is concluded that the navigational capability rather than the motivation to fly or to return home is affected by olfactory deprivation. It is further concluded that homing of pigeons depends on olfactory stimuli perceived at the remote sites even at distances as large as 180km.In the data of the anosmic pigeons a non-olfactory component of homeward orientation persists which is much more pronounced on the W-E axis than on the N-S axis. This component alone appears to be insufficient for a return to the loft.Earlier results and conclusions that gave rise to some controversy are critically examined.The present as well as earlier findings are discussed with respect to two alternative hypotheses of olfactory navigation, the mosaic hypothesis (favoured by Papi) and the gradient hypothesis (favoured by the author).Abbreviations EP experimental pigeon(s) - CP control pigeon(s) - CLCP cueless transported control pigeon(s) (see p. 210) - PCD preferred compass direction I gratefully acknowledge the possibility to maintain our pigeon loft in a building that belongs to the Zoological Institute (Prof. M. Lindauer) of the University of Würzburg. I thank B. and K. Brendle, E. Thiele, and K. Wielander for the releasing of pigeons and for other technical assistance.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号