共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The invariant recognition ability of five- to six-year-old children with typical and atypical patterns of development in terms of the shape of visual images varying in color, size, and location has been studied. It has been shown that the typically developing children of this age are able to identify the invariant form of a visual object regardless of any changes in its color, size, or location. The children with neurodevelopmental disorders have difficulties with identifying the shape of a visual object when its location among a large number of figures is changed. The children with early infantile autism exhibit different degrees of visual perceptual deficit. The children with the milder forms of autistic disorders have difficulties only with recognizing the shape of an image when its location is changed; the children with more severe forms of disorders showed a serious impairment of invariant recognition regardless of image color, size, and location. 相似文献
2.
This study analyzed distributions of Euclidean displacements in gaze (i.e. “gaze steps”) to evaluate the degree of componential cognitive constraints on audio-visual speech perception tasks. Children performing these tasks exhibited distributions of gaze steps that were closest to power-law or lognormal distributions, suggesting a multiplicatively interactive, flexible, self-organizing cognitive system rather than a component-dominant stipulated cognitive structure. Younger children and children diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) exhibited distributions that were closer to power-law than lognormal, indicating a reduced degree of self-organized structure. The relative goodness of lognormal fit was also a significant predictor of ASD, suggesting that this type of analysis may point towards a promising diagnostic tool. These results lend further support to an interaction-dominant framework that casts cognitive processing and development in terms of self-organization instead of fixed components and show that these analytical methods are sensitive to important developmental and neuropsychological differences. 相似文献
3.
4.
The characteristic of visual perception (VP) were analyzed in five- to seven-year-old children according to a modified method of Frostig, which estimates the following VP components: visuospatial perception, figure-ground differentiation, and visuomotor integration. Age dependence of the development of VP components indicating the variation of the rates of their development was found. All VP components rapidly developed between five and six years of age; considerable changes in visuomotor integration and visuospatial perception were observed between six and seven years of age. 相似文献
5.
6.
Optomotor control of course and altitude in the fruitfly, Drosophila melanogaster, requires dense networks of elementary movement detectors (EMD's) which cover most if not all of the visual field. The predominant types of EMD's in these networks represent interactions between neighbouring visual elements along the three main directions of the hexagonal array in the compound eye. — Course control in the walking fly is achieved mainly by pairs of equivalent EMD's which occupy 2 o'clock and 4 o'clock positions with respect to the right eye (Buchner, 1976). Comparison of the turning response and the torque response in the present account confirms the particular properties of this network, and proves the presumed bidirectional sensitivity of its EMD's for the course control responses of legs and wings in the corresponding modes of locomotion. — Altitude control during flight is achieved by a less homogeneous network of EMD's which modifies lift and thrust simultaneously by the appropriate control of the wing beat amplitudes. The predominant types of EMD's in the lateral eye regions occupy 12 o'clock and 2 o'clock positions with respect to the right eye (Buchner et al., 1978). The present evaluation of the optomotor responses of thrust and wing beat confirms the preferred orientation of these EMD's and discloses a pecularity of their internal structure. The movement detectors of this network lack the bidirectional sensitivity of the EMD's in the course control system. At least the fronto-lateral network of the altitude control system seems to consist mainly of pairs of equivalent unidirectional EMD's. The detectors in 12 o'clock position increase wing beat in response to movement of the visual surroundings from inferior to superior. The opposite effect is produced by the detectors in 2 o'clock position which respond to movement from anterior-superior to posterior-inferior. These properties qualify unidirectional EMD's as the functional units of the optomotor control system in the fruitfly. Pairs of unidirectional antagonists would be sufficient to establish the bidirectional sensitivity found in the movement detectors of the course control system. 相似文献
7.
8.
Lauer RT Stackhouse C Shewokis PA Smith BT Orlin M McCarthy JJ 《Journal of biomechanics》2005,38(6):1351-1357
The objective of this study was to examine the use of the continuous wavelet transform (CWT) on surface electromyographic (sEMG) signals acquired from the lower extremity muscles during gait in children with typical development (TD) and cerebral palsy (CP). This was done to explore the possibility of developing a quantitative assessment scale of motor function based on time-frequency information. An initial study was conducted on retrospective gait data from three children, matched in gender and in anthropometric variables but with differing levels of walking ability. EMG data were extracted from five lower extremity muscles to assess the degrees of differentiation. The data were processed using the CWT to derive an average scalogram, from which the instantaneous mean frequency (IMNF) was calculated. Principal component analysis was used to assess the differences between the curves. Preliminary results indicated that for select lower extremity muscles, there was a significant deviation in the IMNF curves in the child with CP as compared to the child with TD. Furthermore, as motor impairment increased, total percent explained variance to the TD curves decreased. This suggests that it might be possible to derive a physiologically based quantitative index for assessing motor function and for assessing clinical treatments in CP using the wavelet analysis. 相似文献
9.
A neural network model is proposed to explain the development of direction selectivity of cortical cells. The model is constructed under the following three hypotheses that are very plausible from recent neurophysiological findings. (1) Direction selectivity is developed by modifiable inhibitory synapses. (2) It results not from the direct convergence of many excitatory inputs from LGN cells but from cortical neural networks. (3) Direction-selective mechanism is independent of orientation-selective mechanism.—The model was simulated on a computer for a few kinds of inhibitory connections and initial conditions. The results were consistent with neurophysiological facts not only for normal cats but for cats reared in an abnormal visual environment. 相似文献
10.
Three visual search experiments examined whether motion is coded as two separate features, speed and direction. Increasing the heterogeneity of the directions in which stimuli moved disrupted detection of a target defined by speed (fast among medium and slow nontargets), suggesting that speed is coded integrally with direction. However, heterogeneity in speed did not disrupt detection of a target moving in a particular direction among nontargets with different directions. This suggests that direction is coded independently of speed. The apparent paradox raised by these contrasting conclusions is consistent with neurophysiological and computational models of motion-detection, which suggest that low-levels of the visual system contain direction-detectors insensitive to speed, while speed is coded at higher levels by detectors which are also sensitive to direction. Evidence consistent with the existence of the latter conjunction detectors was obtained in a final experiment which found search for a conjunction of speed and direction to be parallel. 相似文献
11.
ABSTRACT. Movement of host odour was modelled in natural tsetse habitats with smoke and ultra-light 7-cm-long wind vanes; the speed and direction of the air movements were analysed from video recordings thereof. Wind of <1 ms-1 did not move in straight lines, since large packets of air (>10 m across) often changed direction together. The rate of this change of direction (meander) correlated negatively with windspeed. In open woodland with a shrubby understorey (in which windspeed was reduced by a factor of >5 from that above the canopy, to ax 0.3 m s-1), this wind meander fell by 2d? s-1 change of direction for each 0.1 m s-1 increase in windspeed (r2=0.96). Over open ground without shrub cover, the meander fell by 0.5d? s-1 per 0.1 m s-1 increase in windspeed (r2=0.85). In both situations, such meandering virtually ceased in winds of > 1 m s-1. In woodland, the relationship between the direction of air movement near the surface of bare earth (one potential tsetse landing site) and that c. 0.5 m above ground level (flight height) was often weak (r2=0.2-0.4), but this problem would be reduced if the fly averaged the ground-level wind for at least 30 s. Odour (smoke) travelling from a source 15 m ‘upwind’ over open ground arrived at a notional tsetse fly for 80% of the time from a direction within 10d? of the true source direction. In typical tsetse woodland, however, the ‘odour’ arrived from all directions (including >90d? away from the source), with only a 30% bias towards the true source direction (±10d?). Evidently, tsetse must navigate up odour plumes by means that get round these difficulties-simple, moth-type upwind anemotaxis alone seems unlikely to be adequate. 相似文献
12.
13.
14.
15.
The visual system of the fly performs various computations on photoreceptor outputs. The detection and measurement of movement is based on simple nonlinear multiplication-like interactions between adjacent pairs and groups of photoreceptors. The position of a small contrasted object against a uniform background is measured, at least in part, by (formally) 1-input nonlinear flicker detectors. A fly can also detect and discriminate a figure that moves relative to a ground texture. This computation of relative movement relies on a more complex algorithm, one which detects discontinuities in the movement field. The experiments described in this paper indicate that the outputs of neighbouring movement detectors interact in a multiplication-like fashion and then in turn inhibit locally the flicker detectors. The following main characteristic properties (partly a direct consequence of the algorithm's structure) have been established experimentally: a) Coherent motion of figure and ground inhibit the position detectors whereas incoherent motion fails to produce inhibition near the edges of the moving figure (provided the textures of figure and ground are similar). b) The movement detectors underlying this particular computation are direction-insensitive at input frequencies (at the photoreceptor level) above 2.3 Hz. They become increasingly direction-sensitive for lower input frequencies. c) At higher input frequencies the fly cannot discriminate an object against a texture oscillating at the same frequency and amplitude at 0° and 180° phase, whereas 90° or 270° phase shift between figure and ground oscillations yields maximum discrimination. d) Under conditions of coherent movement, strong spatial incoherence is detected by the same mechanism. The algorithm underlying the relative movement computation is further discussed as an example of a coherence measuring process, operating on the outputs of an array of movement detectors. Possible neural correlates are also mentioned. 相似文献
16.
Typical antipsychotic drugs with a high extrapyramidal motor side-effects liability markedly increase extracellular dopamine in the caudate-putamen, while atypical antipsychotic drugs with a low incidence of extrapyramidal motor side-effects have less pronounced stimulating actions on striatal dopamine. Therefore, it has been suggested that the extrapyramidal motor side-effects liability of antipsychotic drugs (APD) is correlated with their ability to increase extracellular dopamine in the caudate-putamen. The globus pallidus (GP) is another basal ganglia structure probably mediating extrapyramidal motor side-effects of typical antipsychotic drugs. Therefore, the present study sought to determine whether extracellular dopamine in the globus pallidus might be a further indicator to differentiate neurochemical actions of typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs. Using in vivo microdialysis we compared effects on pallidal dopamine induced by typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs in rats. Experiment I demonstrated that systemic administration of haloperidol (1 mg/kg; i.p.) and clozapine (20 mg/kg; i.p.) induced a significant pallidal dopamine release to about 160 and 180% of baseline, respectively. Experiment II revealed that reverse microdialysis of raclopride and clozapine using a cumulative dosing regimen did not stimulate extracellular dopamine in the globus pallidus if low (1microM) or intermediate (10 and 100 microM) concentrations were used. Only at a high concentration (1,000 microM), raclopride and clozapine induced a significant pallidal dopamine release to about 130 and 300% of baseline values, respectively. Thus, effects of typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs on pallidal dopamine were similar and thus, may not be related to their differential extrapyramidal motor side-effects liability. Furthermore, the finding that reverse microdialysis of raclopride over a wide range of concentrations did not stimulate pallidal dopamine concentrations tentatively suggests that pallidal dopamine release under basal conditions is not regulated by D2 autoreceptors. 相似文献
17.
Shoji Tanaka 《Biological cybernetics》1994,71(6):503-510
The fundamental statistical aspects of population coding of the movement direction in the motor cortex are studied numerically. The activity of neurons in a population is simulated using pseudorandom numbers so that the directional selectivity of the neurons is similar to that observed experimentally. Accuracy of the coding, which is evaluated by the root-mean-square (rms) error, is analyzed for various population sizes, degrees of variability of neuronal activity, and degrees of nonuniformity of distribution of the preferred directions. The dependence of the rms error on the population size shows a good fit to the inverse square-root law, from which it is estimated that a single population must contain around 10 000 neurons in order to attain the accuracy that allows 1 deg rms error, for example. The coding is studied further for populations with different types of tuning function. The results support the hypothesis proposed by Georgopoulos et al. (1988) except that the tuning function must be tuned in the sense that the average value of the function for movements with components in the preferred direction is larger than for movements away from the preferred direction. 相似文献
18.
Eye gaze is an important social cue which is used to determine another person's focus of attention and intention to communicate. In combination with a fearful facial expression eye gaze can also signal threat in the environment. The ability to detect and understand others' social signals is essential in order to avoid danger and enable social evaluation. It has been a matter of debate when infants are able to use gaze cues and emotional facial expressions in reference to external objects. Here we demonstrate that by 3 months of age the infant brain differentially responds to objects as a function of how other people are reacting to them. Using event-related electrical brain potentials (ERPs), we show that an indicator of infants' attention is enhanced by an adult's expression of fear toward an unfamiliar object. The infant brain showed an increased Negative central (Nc) component toward objects that had been previously cued by an adult's eye gaze and frightened facial expression. Our results further suggest that infants' sensitivity cannot be due to a general arousal elicited by a frightened face with eye gaze directed at an object. The neural attention system of 3 month old infants is sensitive to an adult's eye gaze direction in combination with a fearful expression. This early capacity may lay the foundation for the development of more sophisticated social skills such as social referencing, language, and theory of mind. 相似文献
19.
J.M. Nara A.M. Cianciarullo H.F. Culler V. Bueris D.S.P.Q. Horton M.A. Menezes M.R. Franzolin W.P. Elias R.M.F. Piazza 《Journal of applied microbiology》2010,109(1):35-43
Aims: The aim of study was to develop a colony immunoblot assay to differentiate typical from atypical enteropathogenic Escherichia coli (EPEC) by detection of bundle‐forming pilus (BFP) expression. Methods and Results: Anti‐BFP antiserum was raised in rabbits and its reactivity was confirmed by immunoelectron microscopy and by immunoblotting recognizing bundlin, the major pilus repeating subunit. The bacterial isolates tested in the colony immunoblot assay were grown in different media. Proteins from bacterial isolates were transferred to nitrocellulose membrane after treatment with phosphate buffer containing Triton X‐100, EDTA and sodium chloride salts. When 24 typical EPEC and 96 isolates including, 72 atypical EPEC, 13 Gram‐negative type IV‐expressing strains and 11 enterobacteriaceae were cultivated in Dulbecco’s Modified Eagle’s Medium agar containing fetal bovine serum or in blood agar in the presence of CaCl2, they showed a positivity of 92 and 83%, and specificity of 96 and 97%, respectively. Conclusion: The assay enables reliable identification of BFP‐expressing isolates and contributes to the differentiation of typical and atypical EPEC. Significance and Impact of the Study: The colony immunoblot for BFP detection developed in this study combines the simplicity of an immunoserological assay with the high efficiency of testing a large number of EPEC colonies. 相似文献
20.
Gordeev SA 《Zhurnal vysshe? nervno? deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova》2007,57(2):161-168
84 patients with panic disorders and 36 healthy control subjects with use of clinico-neurologic technique, psychometric, neuropsychological and the neurophysiological method of auditory event-related potentials P300 were examined. Patients with panic disorders were characterized by the raised level of anxiety, depression and cognitive function disturbances in the form of decrease of short-term memory and attention insufficiency in comparison with the healthy control subjects. Patients with atypical panic disorders differed from the patients with typical panic disorders by lower level of anxiety, a greater degree of depression and more expressed cognitive function disturbances. In comparison with the healthy control subjects with atypical panic disorders the decrease of P300 peak amplitude was observed, with typical--its augmentation. It is supposed, that P300 peak change is bound to dysfunction of temporal-limbic-reticular brain structures. 相似文献