首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Neuropsychological studies report more impaired responses to facial expressions of fear than disgust in people with amygdala lesions, and vice versa in people with Huntington''s disease. Experiments using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have confirmed the role of the amygdala in the response to fearful faces and have implicated the anterior insula in the response to facial expressions of disgust. We used fMRI to extend these studies to the perception of fear and disgust from both facial and vocal expressions. Consistent with neuropsychological findings, both types of fearful stimuli activated the amygdala. Facial expressions of disgust activated the anterior insula and the caudate-putamen; vocal expressions of disgust did not significantly activate either of these regions. All four types of stimuli activated the superior temporal gyrus. Our findings therefore (i) support the differential localization of the neural substrates of fear and disgust; (ii) confirm the involvement of the amygdala in the emotion of fear, whether evoked by facial or vocal expressions; (iii) confirm the involvement of the anterior insula and the striatum in reactions to facial expressions of disgust; and (iv) suggest a possible general role for the perception of emotional expressions for the superior temporal gyrus.  相似文献   

2.
Cognitive research has long been aware of the relationship between individual differences in personality and performance on behavioural tasks. However, within the field of cognitive neuroscience, the way in which such differences manifest at a neural level has received relatively little attention. We review recent research addressing the relationship between personality traits and the neural response to viewing facial signals of emotion. In one section, we discuss work demonstrating the relationship between anxiety and the amygdala response to facial signals of threat. A second section considers research showing that individual differences in reward drive (behavioural activation system), a trait linked to aggression, influence the neural responsivity and connectivity between brain regions implicated in aggression when viewing facial signals of anger. Finally, we address recent criticisms of the correlational approach to fMRI analyses and conclude that when used appropriately, analyses examining the relationship between personality and brain activity provide a useful tool for understanding the neural basis of facial expression processing and emotion processing in general.  相似文献   

3.
4.
The effects of the activation level and subject's sex on bilateral skin conductance measures were studied. Thirty right-handed subjects (15 males and 15 females) were exposed to three types of stimulus conditions: rest-period, verbal task and spatial task. Results showed that no relationship was observed between EDA asymmetry and the increase in the activation level induced by the verbal and the spatial tasks. Males showed both higher SCRs and greater frequency of responses on the left than on the right hand. The direction of electrodermal asymmetry remained constant regardless of the stimulus conditions. It was concluded that sex differences are important in the study of EDA asymmetry and that this asymmetry appeared to depend on peripheral variations.  相似文献   

5.

Background

Alcoholism is associated with abnormal anger processing. The purpose of this study was to investigate brain regions involved in the evaluation of angry facial expressions in patients with alcohol dependency.

Methods

Brain blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) responses to angry faces were measured and compared between patients with alcohol dependency and controls.

Results

During intensity ratings of angry faces, significant differences in BOLD were observed between patients with alcohol dependency and controls. That is, patients who were alcohol-dependent showed significantly greater activation in several brain regions, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC).

Conclusions

Following exposure to angry faces, abnormalities in dACC and MPFC activation in patients with alcohol dependency indicated possible inefficiencies or hypersensitivities in social cognitive processing.  相似文献   

6.
Emotional intelligence-related differences in oscillatory responses to emotional facial expressions were investigated in 48 subjects (26 men and 22 women), age 18–30 years. Participants were instructed to evaluate emotional expression (angry, happy, and neutral) of each presented face on an analog scale ranging from ?100 (very hostile) to + 100 (very friendly). High emotional intelligence (EI) participants were found to be more sensitive to the emotional content of the stimuli. It showed up both in their subjective evaluation of the stimuli and in a stronger EEG theta synchronization at an earlier (between 100 and 500 ms after face presentation) processing stage. Source localization using sLORETA showed that this effect was localized in the fusiform gyrus upon the presentation of angry faces and in the posterior cingulate gyrus upon the presentation of happy faces. At a later processing stage (500–870 ms), event-related theta synchronization in high emotional intelligence subjects was higher in the left prefrontal cortex upon the presentation of happy faces, but it was lower in the anterior cingulate cortex upon the presentation of angry faces. This suggests the existence of a mechanism that can selectively increase the positive emotions and reduce negative emotions.  相似文献   

7.
Taste-induced facial expressions in apes and humans   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Different gustatory stimuli activate distinct, stereotyped motorbehaviors of the orofacial region. These serve as nonverbal communicational signs, indicative of both intensity and hedonics of the perceived sensation. The present study aims to compare these orofacial motor-coordinations of apes with those of perinatal human infants. A group of 27 infants, prior to their first feeding-experience, as well as a group of 14 apes were tested. Video-recorded documentation of stimulation and stimulus-dependent responses for both groups were evaluated in a blind-setting. Overall hedonic ratings and semiquantitative analysis of the motion-features composing the facial expressions served as critical measures. Results revealed a sizeable correlation between mean hedonic ratings ascribed to the different responses of neonates and of apes. The semiquantitative analysis shows that sweet-, water- and bitter-stimuli activate almost identical motion-features in the orofacial regions of both groups tested. Findings also correlate with those obtained in testing adolescent, adult and elderly human examinees.  相似文献   

8.
Recent evidence suggests that young men and women may have different strategies for regulating arterial blood pressure, and the purpose of the present study was to determine if sex differences exist in diastolic arterial pressure (DAP) and muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) relations during simulated orthostatic stress. We hypothesized that young men would demonstrate stronger DAP-MSNA coherence and a greater percentage of "consecutive integrated bursts" during orthostatic stress. Fourteen men and 14 women (age 23 ± 1 yr) were examined at rest and during progressive lower body negative pressure (LBNP; -5 to -40 mmHg). Progressive LBNP did not alter mean arterial pressure (MAP) in either sex. Heart rate increased and stroke volume decreased to a greater extent during LBNP in women (interactions, P < 0.05). DAP-MSNA coherence was strong (i.e., r ≥ 0.5) at rest and increased throughout all LBNP stages in men. In contrast, DAP-MSNA coherence was lower in women, and responses to progressive LBNP were attenuated compared with men (time × sex, P = 0.029). Men demonstrated a higher percentage of consecutive bursts during all stages of LBNP (sex, P < 0.05), although the percentage of consecutive bursts increased similarly during progressive LBNP between sexes. In conclusion, men and women demonstrate different firing patterns of integrated MSNA during LBNP that appear to be related to differences in DAP oscillatory patterns. Men tend to have more consecutive bursts, which likely contribute to a stronger DAP-MSNA coherence. These findings may help explain why young women are more prone to orthostatic intolerance.  相似文献   

9.
Intergroup conflict poses a different kind of threat for men and women — a difference that can be expected to have implications for cognitive as well as behavioral processes. Participants were primed with a threat from a rival coalition vs. a control condition. Reaction times were measured on a lexical-decision task in response to ideation consistent with coalitions or with friendship/protective care. When primed for coalitional threat, men showed fast access to positive coalitional ideation (suggesting facilitation). In contrast, women showed exceptionally fast access to positive friendship/protective care ideation. Findings were interpreted as reflecting sexually dimorphic responses to coalitional threat that are consistent with differential advertising of their assets to others.  相似文献   

10.
Benson & Perrett''s (1991 b) computer-based caricature procedure was used to alter the positions of anatomical landmarks in photographs of emotional facial expressions with respect to their locations in a reference norm face (e.g. a neutral expression). Exaggerating the differences between an expression and its norm produces caricatured images, whereas reducing the differences produces ''anti-caricatures''. Experiment 1 showed that caricatured (+50% different from neutral) expressions were recognized significantly faster than the veridical (0%, undistorted) expressions. This held for all six basic emotions from the Ekman & Friesen (1976) series, and the effect generalized across different posers. For experiment 2, caricatured (+50%) and anti-caricatured (-50%) images were prepared using two types of reference norm; a neutral-expression norm, which would be optimal if facial expression recognition involves monitoring changes in the positioning of underlying facial muscles, and a perceptually-based norm involving an average of the expressions of six basic emotions (excluding neutral) in the Ekman & Friesen (1976) series. The results showed that the caricatured images were identified significantly faster, and the anti-caricatured images significantly slower, than the veridical expressions. Furthermore, the neutral-expression and average-expression norm caricatures produced the same pattern of results.  相似文献   

11.
In this review we propose that there are sex differences in how men and women enter onto the path that can lead to addiction. Males are more likely than females to engage in risky behaviors that include experimenting with drugs of abuse, and in susceptible individuals, they are drawn into the spiral that can eventually lead to addiction. Women and girls are more likely to begin taking drugs as self-medication to reduce stress or alleviate depression. For this reason women enter into the downward spiral further along the path to addiction, and so transition to addiction more rapidly. We propose that this sex difference is due, at least in part, to sex differences in the organization of the neural systems responsible for motivation and addiction. Additionally, we suggest that sex differences in these systems and their functioning are accentuated with addiction. In the current review we discuss historical, cultural, social and biological bases for sex differences in addiction with an emphasis on sex differences in the neurotransmitter systems that are implicated.  相似文献   

12.
13.

Background

Recognition of others'' emotions is an important aspect of interpersonal communication. In major depression, a significant emotion recognition impairment has been reported. It remains unclear whether the ability to recognize emotion from facial expressions is also impaired in anxiety disorders. There is a need to review and integrate the published literature on emotional expression recognition in anxiety disorders and major depression.

Methodology/Principal Findings

A detailed literature search was used to identify studies on explicit emotion recognition in patients with anxiety disorders and major depression compared to healthy participants. Eighteen studies provided sufficient information to be included. The differences on emotion recognition impairment between patients and controls (Cohen''s d) with corresponding confidence intervals were computed for each study. Over all studies, adults with anxiety disorders had a significant impairment in emotion recognition (d = −0.35). In children with anxiety disorders no significant impairment of emotion recognition was found (d = −0.03). Major depression was associated with an even larger impairment in recognition of facial expressions of emotion (d = −0.58).

Conclusions/Significance

Results from the current analysis support the hypothesis that adults with anxiety disorders or major depression both have a deficit in recognizing facial expression of emotions, and that this deficit is more pronounced in major depression than in anxiety.  相似文献   

14.
One of the stronger empirical generalizations to emerge from the study of genetic systems is that achiasmate meiosis, which has evolved 25–30 times, is always restricted to the heterogametic sex in dioecious species, usually the male. Here we collate data on quantitative sex differences in chiasma frequency from 54 species (4 hermaphroditic flatworms, 18 dioecious insects and vertebrates and 32 hermaphroditic plants) to test whether similar trends hold. Though significant sex differences have been observed within many species, only the Liliaceae show a significant sexual dimorphism in chiasma frequency across species, with more crossing over in embryo mother cells than in pollen mother cells; chiasma frequencies are unrelated to sex and gamety in all other higher taxa studied. Further, the magnitude of sexual dimorphism, independent of sign, does not differ among the three main ecological groups (dioecious animals, plants, and hermaphroditic animals), contrary to what would be expected if it reflected sex-specific selection on recombination. These results indicate that the strong trends for achiasmate meiosis do not apply to quantitative sex differences in recombination, and contradict theories of sex-specific costs and benefits. An alternative hypothesis suggests that sex differences may be more-or-less neutral, selection determining only the mean rate of recombination. While male and female chiasma frequencies are more similar than would be expected under complete neutrality, a less absolute form of the hypothesis is more difficult to falsify. In female mice the sex bivalent has more chiasmata for its length than the autosomes, perhaps compensating for the absence of recombination in males. Finally, we observe that chiasma frequencies in males and females are positively correlated across species, validating the use of only one sex in comparative studies of recombination.  相似文献   

15.
16.
In order to test the different expectations of evolutionary psychologists and “folk” psychologists about whether men or women are better at judging family resemblance, we created a test consisting of pairs of photographs, some of a parent and that parent's child, and some of an unrelated parent and child. Two hundred subjects judged the relatedness of 24 pairs of photos. The results show women and men to be equal in ability to judge family resemblance (mean number correct 14.99 versus 14.53, F = 1.7, p = 0.19). Both sexes were better able to judge resemblance for mothers than fathers (F = 11.25, p < 0.0001), and men and women were better at judging relatedness for children of their own sex (F = 3.99, p < 0.05). Ability to judge family resemblance was not related to the rater's age, marital status, number of siblings, number of children, or years of education. On a test of ability to recall faces, women were superior (F = 4.14, p < 0.043). These findings are considered in relationship to previous research and to the predictions of evolutionary and folk psychology.  相似文献   

17.
18.
19.
Sex differences in response to discrete estradiol injections   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Developmental effects of perinatal androgens render adult male rats refractory to the activation of feminine sexual behavior by estradiol (E2) and progesterone (P). Recent evidence suggested that fluctuating levels of systemic E2, which are thought to approximate the ovarian secretion under physiological conditions, may reverse this insensitivity to E2 and, particularly, to the synergistic effects of P in male rats of the Wistar strain. We examined whether this hormonal regimen would reverse this insensitivity in Sprague-Dawley rats. Gonadectomized animals received two injections of E2 (1 microgram per injection) 12 hr apart at 0900 and 2100 hr followed by P (0.5 mg) or oil, at 35 hr, and a mating behavior test, at 38 hr, subsequent to the initial E2 administration. This treatment was repeated four times at 4-day intervals. The inability of Sprague-Dawley male rats to respond to E2 and P was unaffected by this pattern of exposure to exogenous E2. Receptivity scores, lordosis quotients, and proceptivity were negligible in males, and significantly less than that displayed by females. In addition, the levels of sexual receptivity and proceptivity were facilitated by the availability of P following E2 in females, but not in males. The present findings fail to support a general hypothesis that "discontinuous" E2 stimulation, achieved by two spaced injections of this hormone, reverses developmental determinants of sex differences in responsiveness to hormones mediating female sexual behaviors.  相似文献   

20.
The expressions we see in the faces of others engage a number of different cognitive processes. Emotional expressions elicit rapid responses, which often imitate the emotion in the observed face. These effects can even occur for faces presented in such a way that the observer is not aware of them. We are also very good at explicitly recognizing and describing the emotion being expressed. A recent study, contrasting human and humanoid robot facial expressions, suggests that people can recognize the expressions made by the robot explicitly, but may not show the automatic, implicit response. The emotional expressions presented by faces are not simply reflexive, but also have a communicative component. For example, empathic expressions of pain are not simply a reflexive response to the sight of pain in another, since they are exaggerated when the empathizer knows he or she is being observed. It seems that we want people to know that we are empathic. Of especial importance among facial expressions are ostensive gestures such as the eyebrow flash, which indicate the intention to communicate. These gestures indicate, first, that the sender is to be trusted and, second, that any following signals are of importance to the receiver.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号