共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The molecular mechanisms of the mammalian gustatory system have been examined in many studies using rodents as model organisms. In this study, we examined the mRNA expression of molecules involved in taste signal transduction in the fungiform papillae (FuP) and circumvallate papillae (CvP) of the rhesus macaque, Macaca mulatta, using in situ hybridization. TAS1R1, TAS1R2, TAS2Rs, and PKD1L3 were exclusively expressed in different subsets of taste receptor cells (TRCs) in the FuP and CvP. This finding suggests that TRCs sensing different basic taste modalities are mutually segregated in macaque taste buds. Individual TAS2Rs exhibited a variety of expression patterns in terms of the apparent level of expression and the number of TRCs expressing these genes, as in the case of human TAS2Rs. GNAT3, but not GNA14, was expressed in TRCs of FuP, whereas GNA14 was expressed in a small population of TRCs of CvP, which were distinct from GNAT3- or TAS1R2-positive TRCs. These results demonstrate similarities and differences between primates and rodents in the expression profiles of genes involved in taste signal transduction. 相似文献
2.
3.
Previous studies suggest fundamental differences between the perceptual learning of speech and non-speech stimuli. One major difference is in the way variability in the training set affects learning and its generalization to untrained stimuli: training-set variability appears to facilitate speech learning, while slowing or altogether extinguishing non-speech auditory learning. We asked whether the reason for this apparent difference is a consequence of the very different methodologies used in speech and non-speech studies. We hypothesized that speech and non-speech training would result in a similar pattern of learning if they were trained using the same training regimen. We used a 2 (random vs. blocked pre- and post-testing) × 2 (random vs. blocked training) × 2 (speech vs. non-speech discrimination task) study design, yielding 8 training groups. A further 2 groups acted as untrained controls, tested with either random or blocked stimuli. The speech task required syllable discrimination along 4 minimal-pair continua (e.g., bee-dee), and the non-speech stimuli required duration discrimination around 4 base durations (e.g., 50 ms). Training and testing required listeners to pick the odd-one-out of three stimuli, two of which were the base duration or phoneme continuum endpoint and the third varied adaptively. Training was administered in 9 sessions of 640 trials each, spread over 4–8 weeks. Significant learning was only observed following speech training, with similar learning rates and full generalization regardless of whether training used random or blocked schedules. No learning was observed for duration discrimination with either training regimen. We therefore conclude that the two stimulus classes respond differently to the same training regimen. A reasonable interpretation of the findings is that speech is perceived categorically, enabling learning in either paradigm, while the different base durations are not well-enough differentiated to allow for categorization, resulting in disruption to learning. 相似文献
4.
The middle temporal area of the extrastriate visual cortex (area MT) is integral to motion perception and is thought to play a key role in the perceptual learning of motion tasks. We have previously found, however, that perceptual learning of a motion discrimination task is possible even when the training stimulus contains locally balanced, motion opponent signals that putatively suppress the response of MT. Assuming at least partial suppression of MT, possible explanations for this learning are that 1) training made MT more responsive by reducing motion opponency, 2) MT remained suppressed and alternative visual areas such as V1 enabled learning and/or 3) suppression of MT increased with training, possibly to reduce noise. Here we used fMRI to test these possibilities. We first confirmed that the motion opponent stimulus did indeed suppress the BOLD response within hMT+ compared to an almost identical stimulus without locally balanced motion signals. We then trained participants on motion opponent or non-opponent stimuli. Training with the motion opponent stimulus reduced the BOLD response within hMT+ and greater reductions in BOLD response were correlated with greater amounts of learning. The opposite relationship between BOLD and behaviour was found at V1 for the group trained on the motion-opponent stimulus and at both V1 and hMT+ for the group trained on the non-opponent motion stimulus. As the average response of many cells within MT to motion opponent stimuli is the same as their response to non-directional flickering noise, the reduced activation of hMT+ after training may reflect noise reduction. 相似文献
5.
Nathalie Stroeymeyt Fernando J. Guerrieri Jelle S. van Zweden Patrizia d'Ettorre 《PloS one》2010,5(8)
Background
Timely decision making is crucial for survival and reproduction. Organisms often face a speed-accuracy trade-off, as fully informed, accurate decisions require time-consuming gathering and treatment of information. Optimal strategies for decision-making should therefore vary depending on the context. In mammals, there is mounting evidence that multiple systems of perceptual discrimination based on different neural circuits emphasize either fast responses or accurate treatment of stimuli depending on the context.Methodology/Principal Findings
We used the ant Camponotus aethiops to test the prediction that fast information processing achieved through direct neural pathways should be favored in situations where quick reactions are adaptive. Social insects discriminate readily between harmless group-members and dangerous strangers using easily accessible cuticular hydrocarbons as nestmate recognition cues. We show that i) tethered ants display rapid aggressive reactions upon presentation of non-nestmate odor (120 to 160 ms); ii) ants'' aggressiveness towards non-nestmates can be specifically reduced by exposure to non-nestmate odor only, showing that social interactions are not required to alter responses towards non-nestmates; iii) decision-making by ants does not require information transfer between brain hemispheres, but relies on side-specific decision rules.Conclusions/Significance
Our results strongly suggest that first-order olfactory processing centers (up to the antennal lobes) are likely to play a key role in ant nestmate recognition. We hypothesize that the coarse level of discrimination achieved in the antennal lobes early in odor processing provides enough information to determine appropriate behavioral responses towards non-nestmates. This asks for a reappraisal of the mechanisms underlying social recognition in insects. 相似文献6.
J. Shawn Goodwin Gaynor A. Larson Jarod Swant Namita Sen Jonathan A. Javitch Nancy R. Zahniser Louis J. De Felice Habibeh Khoshbouei 《The Journal of biological chemistry》2009,284(5):2978-2989
The psychostimulants d-amphetamine (AMPH) and methamphetamine
(METH) release excess dopamine (DA) into the synaptic clefts of dopaminergic
neurons. Abnormal DA release is thought to occur by reverse transport through
the DA transporter (DAT), and it is believed to underlie the severe behavioral
effects of these drugs. Here we compare structurally similar AMPH and METH on
DAT function in a heterologous expression system and in an animal model. In
the in vitro expression system, DAT-mediated whole-cell currents were
greater for METH stimulation than for AMPH. At the same voltage and
concentration, METH released five times more DA than AMPH and did so at
physiological membrane potentials. At maximally effective concentrations, METH
released twice as much [Ca2+]i from internal
stores compared with AMPH. [Ca2+]i responses to
both drugs were independent of membrane voltage but inhibited by DAT
antagonists. Intact phosphorylation sites in the N-terminal domain of DAT were
required for the AMPH- and METH-induced increase in
[Ca2+]i and for the enhanced effects of METH on
[Ca2+]i elevation. Calmodulin-dependent protein
kinase II and protein kinase C inhibitors alone or in combination also blocked
AMPH- or METH-induced Ca2+ responses. Finally, in the rat nucleus
accumbens, in vivo voltammetry showed that systemic application of
METH inhibited DAT-mediated DA clearance more efficiently than AMPH, resulting
in excess external DA. Together these data demonstrate that METH has a
stronger effect on DAT-mediated cell physiology than AMPH, which may
contribute to the euphoric and addictive properties of METH compared with
AMPH.The dopamine transporter
(DAT)3 is a main
target for psychostimulants, such as d-amphetamine (AMPH),
methamphetamine (METH), cocaine (COC), and methylphenidate (Ritalin®). DAT
is the major clearance mechanism for synaptic dopamine (DA)
(1) and thereby regulates the
strength and duration of dopaminergic signaling. AMPH and METH are substrates
for DAT and competitively inhibit DA uptake
(2,
3) and release DA through
reverse transport
(4–9).
AMPH- and METH-induced elevations in extracellular DA result in complex
neurochemical changes and profound psychiatric effects
(2,
10–16).
Despite their structural and pharmacokinetic similarities, a recent National
Institute on Drug Abuse report describes METH as a more potent stimulant than
AMPH with longer lasting effects at comparable doses
(17). Although the route of
METH administration and its availability must contribute to the almost four
times higher lifetime nonmedical use of METH compared with AMPH
(18), there may also be
differences in the mechanisms that underlie the actions of these two drugs on
the dopamine transporter.Recent studies by Joyce et al.
(19) have shown that compared
with d-AMPH alone, the combination of d- and
l-AMPH in Adderall® significantly prolonged the time course of
extracellular DA in vivo. These experiments demonstrate that subtle
structural features of AMPH, such as chirality, can affect its action on
dopamine transporters. Here we investigate whether METH, a more lipophilic
analog of AMPH, affects DAT differently than AMPH, particularly in regard to
stimulated DA efflux.METH and AMPH have been reported as equally effective in increasing
extracellular DA levels in rodent dorsal striatum (dSTR), nucleus accumbens
(NAc) (10,
14,
20), striatal synaptosomes,
and DAT-expressing cells in vitro
(3,
6). John and Jones
(21), however, have recently
shown in mouse striatal and substantia nigra slices, that AMPH is a more
potent inhibitor of DA uptake than METH. On the other hand, in synaptosomes
METH inhibits DA uptake three times more effectively than AMPH
(14), and in DAT-expressing
COS-7 cells, METH releases DA more potently than AMPH (EC50 = 0.2
μm for METH versus EC50 = 1.7
μm for AMPH) (5).
However, these differences do not hold up under all conditions. For example,
in a study utilizing C6 cells, the disparity between AMPH and METH was not
found (12).The variations in AMPH and METH data extend to animal models. AMPH- and
METH-mediated behavior has been reported as similar
(22), lower
(20), or higher
(23) for AMPH compared with
METH. Furthermore, although the maximal locomotor activation response was less
for METH than for AMPH at a lower dose (2 mg/kg, intraperitoneal), both drugs
decreased locomotor activity at a higher dose (4 mg/kg)
(20). In contrast, in the
presence of a salient stimuli, METH is more potent in increasing the overall
magnitude of locomotor activity in rats yet is equipotent with AMPH in the
absence of these stimuli
(23).The simultaneous regulation of DA uptake and efflux by DAT substrates such
as AMPH and METH, as well as the voltage dependence of DAT
(24), may confound the
interpretation of existing data describing the action of these drugs. Our
biophysical approaches allowed us to significantly decrease the contribution
of DA uptake and more accurately determine DAT-mediated DA efflux with
millisecond time resolution. We have thus exploited time-resolved, whole-cell
voltage clamp in combination with in vitro and in vivo
microamperometry and Ca2+ imaging to compare the impact of METH and
AMPH on DAT function and determine the consequence of these interactions on
cell physiology.We find that near the resting potential, METH is more effective than AMPH
in stimulating DAT to release DA. In addition, at efficacious concentrations
METH generates more current, greater DA efflux, and higher Ca2+
release from internal stores than AMPH. Both METH-induced or the lesser
AMPH-induced increase in intracellular Ca2+ are independent of
membrane potential. The additional Ca2+ response induced by METH
requires intact phosphorylation sites in the N-terminal domain of DAT.
Finally, our in vivo voltammetry data indicate that METH inhibits
clearance of locally applied DA more effectively than AMPH in the rat nucleus
accumbens, which plays an important role in reward and addiction, but not in
the dorsal striatum, which is involved in a variety of cognitive functions.
Taken together these data imply that AMPH and METH have distinguishable
effects on DAT that can be shown both at the molecular level and in
vivo, and are likely to be implicated in the relative euphoric and
addictive properties of these two psychostimulants. 相似文献
7.
The intelligibility of periodically interrupted speech improves once the silent gaps are filled with noise bursts. This improvement has been attributed to phonemic restoration, a top-down repair mechanism that helps intelligibility of degraded speech in daily life. Two hypotheses were investigated using perceptual learning of interrupted speech. If different cognitive processes played a role in restoring interrupted speech with and without filler noise, the two forms of speech would be learned at different rates and with different perceived mental effort. If the restoration benefit were an artificial outcome of using the ecologically invalid stimulus of speech with silent gaps, this benefit would diminish with training. Two groups of normal-hearing listeners were trained, one with interrupted sentences with the filler noise, and the other without. Feedback was provided with the auditory playback of the unprocessed and processed sentences, as well as the visual display of the sentence text. Training increased the overall performance significantly, however restoration benefit did not diminish. The increase in intelligibility and the decrease in perceived mental effort were relatively similar between the groups, implying similar cognitive mechanisms for the restoration of the two types of interruptions. Training effects were generalizable, as both groups improved their performance also with the other form of speech than that they were trained with, and retainable. Due to null results and relatively small number of participants (10 per group), further research is needed to more confidently draw conclusions. Nevertheless, training with interrupted speech seems to be effective, stimulating participants to more actively and efficiently use the top-down restoration. This finding further implies the potential of this training approach as a rehabilitative tool for hearing-impaired/elderly populations. 相似文献
8.
9.
Liang Xie Fan Xu Shigang Liu Yongjia Ji Qinming Zhou Qingyuan Wu Wei Gong Ke Cheng Juan Li Leilei Li Liang Fang Linke Zhou Peng Xie 《PloS one》2013,8(6)
Background
The cynomolgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis) has been increasingly used in biomedical research, making knowledge of its blood-based parameters essential to support the selection of healthy subjects and its use in preclinical research. As age and sex affect these blood-based parameters, it is important to establish baseline indices for these parameters on an age and sex basis and determine the effects of age and sex on these indices.Methods
A total of 917 cynomolgus monkeys (374 males and 543 females) were selected and segregated by age (five groups) and sex. A total of 30 hematological and 22 biochemical parameters were measured, and the effects of age and sex were analyzed.Results
Baseline indices for hematological and biochemical parameters were separately established by age and sex. Significant effects by age, sex, and age-sex interaction were observed in a number of blood parameters. In the 49–60 months and 61–72 months age groups, red blood cell count, hemoglobulin, and hematocrit showed significantly lower values (P<0.01) in females than males. Serum alkaline phosphatase varied with age in both sexes (P<0.01) and was significantly higher in females than males (P<0.05) in the groups aged 13–24 months and 25–36 months; however, in the three groups aged over 25–36 months, serum alkaline phosphatase was significantly lower in females than males (P<0.01). Creatinine concentration increased with age (P<0.01) in all age groups; specifically in the groups aged 49–60 months and 61–72 months, creatinine was significantly higher (P<0.01) in males than females. Total protein and globulin both increased with age (P<0.01).Conclusion
The baseline values of hematological and biochemical parameters reported herein establish reference indices of blood-based parameters in the cynomolgus monkey by age and sex, thereby aiding researchers in selecting healthy subjects and evaluating preclinical studies using this species. 相似文献10.
《Current biology : CB》2014,24(24):2926-2929
11.
Tuberculosis (TB) in nonhuman primates is a serious menace to the welfare of the animals
and human who come into contact with them, while the rapid, accurate, and robust diagnosis
is challenging. In this study, we first sought to establish an appropriate primate TB
model resembling natural TB in nonhuman primates. Four rhesus monkeys (Macaca
mulatta) of Chinese origin were infected intratracheally with two low doses of
M. tuberculosis H37Rv. Regardless of the infectious doses, all monkeys
were demonstrated to be successfully infected by clinical assessments, tuberculin skin
test conversions, peripheral immune responses, gross observations, histopathology
analysis, and M. tuberculosis burdens. Furthermore, we extended the
usefulness of this model for assessing the following immunodiagnostic antigens: CFP10,
ESAT-6, CFP10-ESAT-6, and an antigen cocktail of CFP10 and ESAT-6. The data showed that
CFP10 was an M. tuberculosis-specific, “early” antigen used for
serodiagnosis of TB in nonhuman primates. In conclusion, we established a useful primate
TB model depending on low doses of M .tuberculosis and affording new
opportunities for studies of M. tuberculosis disease and diagnostics. 相似文献
12.
13.
14.
Liang Guo Fangfang Ma Fang Wei Brian Fanella Doug K. Allen Xuemin Wang 《The Plant cell》2014,26(7):3023-3035
The cytosolic enzyme glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPC) catalyzes a key
reaction in glycolysis, but its contribution to plant metabolism and growth are not
well defined. Here, we show that two cytosolic GAPCs play important roles in cellular
metabolism and seed oil accumulation. Knockout or overexpression of
GAPCs caused significant changes in the level of intermediates in
the glycolytic pathway and the ratios of ATP/ADP and NAD(P)H/NAD(P). Two double
knockout seeds had ∼3% of dry weight decrease in oil content compared with
that of the wild type. In transgenic seeds under the constitutive 35S promoter, oil
content was increased up to 42% of dry weight compared with 36% in the wild type and
the fatty acid composition was altered; however, these transgenic lines exhibited
decreased fertility. Seed-specific overexpression lines had >3% increase in seed
oil without compromised seed yield or fecundity. The results demonstrate that
GAPC levels play important roles in the overall cellular
production of reductants, energy, and carbohydrate metabolites and that
GAPC levels are directly correlated with seed oil accumulation.
Changes in cellular metabolites and cofactor levels highlight the complexity and
tolerance of Arabidopsis thaliana cells to the metabolic
perturbation. Further implications for metabolic engineering of seed oil production
are discussed. 相似文献
15.
Sebastian M. Frank Susanne Bründl Ulrike I. Frank Yuka Sasaki Mark W. Greenlee Takeo Watanabe 《Current biology : CB》2021,31(2):427-432.e5
- Download : Download high-res image (157KB)
- Download : Download full-size image
16.
17.
Molecules produced by Rhizobium meliloti increase respiration of alfalfa (Medicago sativa L.) roots. Maximum respiratory increases, measured either as CO2 evolution or as O2 uptake, were elicited in roots of 3-d-old seedlings by 16 h of exposure to living or dead R. meliloti cells at densities of 107 bacteria/mL. Excising roots after exposure to bacteria and separating them into root-tip- and root-hair-containing segments showed that respiratory increases occurred only in the root-hair region. In such assays, CO2 production by segments with root hairs increased by as much as 100% in the presence of bacteria. Two partially purified compounds from R. meliloti 1021 increased root respiration at very low, possibly picomolar, concentrations. One factor, peak B, resembled known pathogenic elicitors because it produced a rapid (15-min), transitory increase in respiration. A second factor, peak D, was quite different because root respiration increased slowly for 8 h and was maintained at the higher level. These molecules differ from lipo-chitin oligosaccharides active in root nodulation for the following reasons: (a) they do not curl alfalfa root hairs, (b) they are synthesized by bacteria in the absence of known plant inducer molecules, and (c) they are produced by a mutant R. meliloti that does not synthesize known lipo-chitin oligosaccharides. The peak-D compound(s) may benefit both symbionts by increasing CO2, which is required for growth of R. meliloti, and possibly by increasing the energy that is available in the plant to form root nodules. 相似文献
18.
Visual short-term memory (VSTM) and visual imagery have been shown to modulate visual perception. However, how the subjective experience of VSTM/imagery and its contrast modulate this process has not been investigated. We addressed this issue by asking participants to detect brief masked targets while they were engaged either in VSTM or visual imagery. Subjective experience of memory/imagery (strength scale), and the visual contrast of the memory/mental image (contrast scale) were assessed on a trial-by-trial basis. For both VSTM and imagery, contrast of the memory/mental image was positively associated with reporting target presence. Consequently, at the sensory level, both VSTM and imagery facilitated visual perception. However, subjective strength of VSTM was positively associated with visual detection whereas the opposite pattern was found for imagery. Thus the relationship between subjective strength of memory/imagery and visual detection are qualitatively different for VSTM and visual imagery, although their impact at the sensory level appears similar. Our results furthermore demonstrate that imagery and VSTM are partly dissociable processes. 相似文献
19.
Iris I. A. Groen Sennay Ghebreab Victor A. F. Lamme H. Steven Scholte 《PLoS computational biology》2012,8(10)
The visual world is complex and continuously changing. Yet, our brain transforms patterns of light falling on our retina into a coherent percept within a few hundred milliseconds. Possibly, low-level neural responses already carry substantial information to facilitate rapid characterization of the visual input. Here, we computationally estimated low-level contrast responses to computer-generated naturalistic images, and tested whether spatial pooling of these responses could predict image similarity at the neural and behavioral level. Using EEG, we show that statistics derived from pooled responses explain a large amount of variance between single-image evoked potentials (ERPs) in individual subjects. Dissimilarity analysis on multi-electrode ERPs demonstrated that large differences between images in pooled response statistics are predictive of more dissimilar patterns of evoked activity, whereas images with little difference in statistics give rise to highly similar evoked activity patterns. In a separate behavioral experiment, images with large differences in statistics were judged as different categories, whereas images with little differences were confused. These findings suggest that statistics derived from low-level contrast responses can be extracted in early visual processing and can be relevant for rapid judgment of visual similarity. We compared our results with two other, well- known contrast statistics: Fourier power spectra and higher-order properties of contrast distributions (skewness and kurtosis). Interestingly, whereas these statistics allow for accurate image categorization, they do not predict ERP response patterns or behavioral categorization confusions. These converging computational, neural and behavioral results suggest that statistics of pooled contrast responses contain information that corresponds with perceived visual similarity in a rapid, low-level categorization task. 相似文献
20.
Animals need to associate different environmental stimuli with each other regardless of whether they temporally overlap or not. Drosophila melanogaster displays olfactory trace conditioning, where an odor is followed by electric shock reinforcement after a temporal gap, leading to conditioned odor avoidance. Reversing the stimulus timing in olfactory conditioning results in the reversal of memory valence such that an odor that follows shock is later on approached (i.e. relief conditioning). Here, we explored the effects of stimulus timing on memory in another sensory modality, using a visual conditioning paradigm. We found that flies form visual memories of opposite valence depending on stimulus timing and can associate a visual stimulus with reinforcement despite being presented with a temporal gap. These results suggest that associative memories with non-overlapping stimuli and the effect of stimulus timing on memory valence are shared across sensory modalities. 相似文献