首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Spatial summation has been demonstrated in several sensory modalities.In this study, spatial summation of sensitivity to NaCl wasinvestigated as a function of stimulated area. In a two-alternativeforced choice procedure, a filter paper stimulation method wasused to determine the sensitivity to, and the detection probabilitiesof, five low-intensity NaCl stimuli. Each of the stimuli waspresented 32 times in two conditions, a circular area (ø9 mm) and half that circle in four counterbalanced orientations.Only one side of the tongue was used. The results showed that(a) the summation found was partial; (b) the observed relationshipbetween threshold intensity and area obeyed Piper's Law, and(c) detection probabilities increased according to chance. Adhoc analysis of orientation yielded sensitivity differencesalong the vertical but not the horizontal axis.  相似文献   

2.
Responses in the frog glossopharyngeal nerve induced by electrical stimulation of the tongue were compared with those induced by chemical stimuli under various conditions. (a) Anodal stimulation induced much larger responses than cathodal stimulation, and anodal stimulation of the tongue adapted to 5 mM MgCl2 produced much larger responses than stimulation with the tongue adapted to 10 mM NaCl at equal current intensities, as chemical stimulation with MgCl2 produced much larger responses than stimulation with NaCl at equal concentration. (b) The enhansive and suppressive effects of 8-anilino-1-naphthalenesulfonate, NiCl2, and uranyl acetate on the responses to anodal current were similar to those on the responses to chemical stimulation. (c) Anodal stimulation of the tongue adapted to 50 mM CaCl2 resulted in a large response, whereas application of 1 M CaCl2 to the tongue adapted to 50 mM CaCl2 produced only a small response. This, together with theoretical considerations, suggested that the accumulation of salts on the tongue surface is not the cause of the generation of the response to anodal current. (d) Cathodal current suppressed the responses induced by 1 mM CaCl2, 0.3 M ethanol, and distilled water. (e) The addition of EGTA or Ca-channel blockers (CdCl2 and verapamil) to the perfusing solution of the lingual artery reversibly suppressed both the responses to chemical stimulus (NaCl) and to anodal current with 10 mM NaCl. (f) We assume from the results obtained that electrical current from the microvillus membrane of a taste cell to the synaptic area supplied by anodal stimulation or induced by chemical stimulation activates the voltage-dependent Ca channel at the synaptic area.  相似文献   

3.
Taste receptors on the left and right sides of the anterior tongue are innervated by chorda tympani (CT) fibers, which carry taste information to the ipsilateral nucleus of the solitary tract (NST). Although the anterior tongue is essential for taste, patients with unilateral CT nerve damage often report no subjective change in their taste experience. The standing theory that explains the taste constancy is the "release of inhibition", which hypothesizes that within the NST there are inhibitory interactions between inputs from the CT and glossopharyngeal nerves and that the loss of taste information from the CT is compensated by a release of inhibition on the glossopharyngeal nerve input. However, the possibility of compensation by taste input from the other side of the tongue has never been investigated in rodents. We recorded from 95 taste-responsive neurons in the NST and examined their responsiveness to stimulation of the contralateral CT. Forty-six cells were activated, mostly with excitatory responses (42 cells). Activation of NST cells induced by contralateral CT stimulation was blocked by microinjection of lidocaine into the contralateral NST but was not affected by anesthetization of the contralateral parabrachial nuclei (PbN). In addition, the NST cells that were activated by contralateral CT stimulation showed reduced responsiveness to taste stimulation after microinjection of lidocaine into the contralateral NST. These results demonstrate that nearly half of the taste neurons in the NST receive gustatory information from both sides of the tongue. This "cross talk" between bilateral NST may also contribute to the "taste constancy".  相似文献   

4.
Using a signal detection procedure and a microprocessor-controlledgustometer, sensitivity to three concentrations of NaCl (0.01,0.1 and 1.0 M) was measured on the tongue tip, and on a region3.0 cm posterior to the tongue tip in 12 young (20–29years of age) and 12 elderly (70–79 years of age) subjects.Stimulus duration was 2 s; the sizes of the tongue areas stimulatedwere 12.5, 25 and 50 mm2. On average, the young subjects weremore sensitive to NaCl on the tongue tip than on the more posteriorstimulation site and exhibited, at both tongue loci, an increasein detection performance as stimulus concentration increased.The elderly subjects, on the other hand, performed at chancelevel at both tongue loci for all three stimulus sizes and concentrationstested. No sex differences were observed. In the young subjects,the mean R-index increased monotonically as a function of stimulussize for the two highest NaCl concentrations at both tongueloci. A hyperbolic function provided the best fit to these dataat the tongue tip, and an exponential function at the more posteriortongue region, implying a different pattern of spatial summationat these two sites. Overall, this study demonstrates that markedage-related changes in regional taste sensitivity to NaCl arepresent in elderly persons. Chem. Senses 20: 283–290,1995.  相似文献   

5.
The thermal sensitivities of three humans and one monkey were measured using the “yes-no” paradigm based on the Theory of Signal Detection. The aim was to evaluate the monkey's thermal-sensing system as a model for that of humans. Three of the principal variables of human thermal sensations—the temperature to which the skin was adapted, the rate of temperature change, and the site of application of the thermal stimuli—were held constant. The other three variables—area of stimulation, intensity, and direction of the temperature change—were varied systematically.

All tour subjects displayed spatial summation for both warming and cooling. Isodetectability curves (de = 1) to small temperature changes, both for humans and for the monkey, could reasonably be fitted by the function I = kA-b, where I is stimulus intensity, A is the area of stimulation, and b is the rate at which spatial summation occurred. The rate of summation, b, to warming stimuli for the humans ranged from 0.60 to 1.14, while that for the monkey was 0.40. The rate of summation to cooling stimuli for the humans ranged from 0.50 to 0.87, while that for the monkey was 0.43.

The main species difference was that summation on the monkey palm all but ceased for both warming and cooling stimuli applied to areas larger than 4 cm2. Data from the human subjects did not demonstrate an upper limit of spatial summation. However, there was an indication that the human subjects would show a ceiling for spatial summation near the largest area tested in this study. Thus, when considering the spatial extent of a thermal stimulus and its influence upon thermal sensations, it may be more appropriate to compare areas relative to body size, rather than absolute values.  相似文献   

6.
Somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) to unilateral or bilateral posterior tibial nerve (PTN) stimulation and to stimulation of the dorsal nerve (DN) of the penis / clitoris were recorded on 32 channels in 10 volunteers. SEPs to unilateral PTN stimulation consisted of the classic ‘W’ complex P38-N45-P56-N75 maximal on the ipsilateral central and parietal leads, and two negative waves, N33 and N37, maximal on the contralateral post- and prerolandic areas, respectively. A lemniscal P30 was also recorded. Bilateral PTN stimulation caused, by algebraic summation, the disappearance of both N33 and N37; the W complex was symmetrical and the amplitude of P30 increased. The SEPs to DN stimulation were also symmetrical, and N33 and N37 were absent. These features can be explained by the bilateral character of DN stimulation. They also differed from bilateral PTN SEPs in 3 respects; the absence of P30, the small amplitude and the weaker gradients of field distribution of the ‘W’ complex, and the somewhat different distribution of penile from clitoral or bilateral PTN, N45 and P56. These differences can be explained both by physiological (the different fiber composition of the DN) and anatomical (the deeper localization of the DN cortical receiving area) mechanisms.  相似文献   

7.
Spatio-temporal patterns of binaural interaction in the guinea pig auditory cortex (AC) were observed using optical recording with a 12 × 12 photodiode array and a voltage-sensitive dye. The amplitudes of the sound-induced light signals from the cortex were transformed into sequential two-dimensional images every 0.58 ms. Binaural sound stimuli evoked an excitatory response followed by a strong inhibition, and contralateral stimuli evoked a strong excitatory response followed by a weak inhibition. Ipsilateral sound stimuli evoked a weak response. Binaural stimulation induced two types of ipsilateral inhibition: a fast binaural inhibition which was detected only after the contralateral and ipsilateral responses were subtracted from the binaural responses, and which appeared 12–25 ms after the onset of stimulation, and a slow binaural inhibitory effect which was clearly observed in the binaural responses themselves, appearing 70–95 ms after the onset of stimulation. The fast binaural inhibition was observed in the same area as the contralateral excitatory response. The inhibited area became stronger and more widespread with increasing intensity of ipsilateral stimulation. We did not observe the specialized organization of binaural neurons as electrophysiologically found in the cat AC, in which binaural neurons of the same binaural response type are clustered together and alternate with clusters of other response types. Accepted: 14 August 1997  相似文献   

8.
The relationship between induction of central sensitization and facilitation of temporal summation to repetitive stimulation is still unclear. The aim of this study was to investigate temporal summation before and after the induction of secondary hyperalgesia by two different experimental methods: capsaicin injection and controlled heat injury. The effect of each injury model was assessed on a separate day with an interval of at least 5 days. Twelve healthy volunteers participated. Each experiment was performed using electrical, radiant heat, mechanical impact, and punctuate stimuli consecutively. The pain threshold (PT) to a single stimulus and the summation threshold to five repetitive stimuli for electrical (2?Hz) and radiant heat (0.83?Hz) were assessed within the secondary hyperalgesic area. The degree of temporal summation for stimulus intensities of 0.8, 1.0, and 1.2 times the baseline pain thresholds were evaluated by the increase in visual analogue scale (VAS) scores from the first to the fifth stimulus of the train. Further, the degrees of temporal summation were assessed for mechanical impact and punctuate stimuli within the primary and secondary hyperalgesic areas. The contra-lateral forearm served as control (no injury). The pain threshold and the summation threshold to electrical and heat stimuli decreased significantly within the secondary hyperalgesic area after the injury induced by both heat injury or capsaicin injection. However, there was no temporal summation for heat and electrical stimuli in either model. In contrast, for the mechanical impact and punctuate mechanical stimuli the degree of temporal summation was significantly facilitated in the secondary hyperalgesic areas compared with the baseline and the control arm in both models. In the primary hyperalgesic area, the degree of temporal summation was facilitated to mechanical impact and punctuate stimuli but only following the capsaicin injection. In conclusion, the temporal summation mechanism for mechanical stimuli was facilitated in the secondary hyperalgesic area.  相似文献   

9.
The relationship between induction of central sensitization and facilitation of temporal summation to repetitive stimulation is still unclear. The aim of this study was to investigate temporal summation before and after the induction of secondary hyperalgesia by two different experimental methods: capsaicin injection and controlled heat injury. The effect of each injury model was assessed on a separate day with an interval of at least 5 days. Twelve healthy volunteers participated. Each experiment was performed using electrical, radiant heat, mechanical impact, and punctuate stimuli consecutively. The pain threshold (PT) to a single stimulus and the summation threshold to five repetitive stimuli for electrical (2 Hz) and radiant heat (0.83 Hz) were assessed within the secondary hyperalgesic area. The degree of temporal summation for stimulus intensities of 0.8, 1.0, and 1.2 times the baseline pain thresholds were evaluated by the increase in visual analogue scale (VAS) scores from the first to the fifth stimulus of the train. Further, the degrees of temporal summation were assessed for mechanical impact and punctuate stimuli within the primary and secondary hyperalgesic areas. The contra-lateral forearm served as control (no injury). The pain threshold and the summation threshold to electrical and heat stimuli decreased significantly within the secondary hyperalgesic area after the injury induced by both heat injury or capsaicin injection. However, there was no temporal summation for heat and electrical stimuli in either model. In contrast, for the mechanical impact and punctuate mechanical stimuli the degree of temporal summation was significantly facilitated in the secondary hyperalgesic areas compared with the baseline and the control arm in both models. In the primary hyperalgesic area, the degree of temporal summation was facilitated to mechanical impact and punctuate stimuli but only following the capsaicin injection. In conclusion, the temporal summation mechanism for mechanical stimuli was facilitated in the secondary hyperalgesic area.  相似文献   

10.
Cho YK  Li CS  Smith DV 《Chemical senses》2003,28(2):155-171
The lateral hypothalamus (LH) and the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) exert an influence on many aspects of ingestive behavior. These nuclei receive projections from several areas carrying gustatory and viscerosensory information, and send axons to these nuclei as well, including the nucleus of the solitary tract (NST). Gustatory responses of NST neurons are modulated by stimulation of the LH and the CeA, and by several physiological factors related to ingestive behavior. We investigated the effect of both LH and CeA stimulation on the activity of 215 taste-responsive neurons in the hamster NST. More than half of these neurons (113/215) were modulated by electrical stimulation of the LH and/or CeA; of these, 52 cells were influenced by both areas, often bilaterally. The LH influenced more neurons than the CeA (101 versus 64 cells). Contralateral stimulation of these forebrain areas was more often effective (144 responses) than ipsilateral (74). Modulatory effects were mostly excitatory (102 cells); 11 cells were inhibited, mostly by ipsilateral LH stimulation. A subset of these cells (n = 25) was examined for the effects of microinjection of DL-homocysteic acid (DLH), a glutamate receptor agonist, into the LH and/or CeA. The effects of electrical stimulation were completely mimicked by DLH, indicating that cell somata in and around the stimulating sites were responsible for these effects. Other cells (n = 25) were tested for the effects of electrical stimulation of the LH and/or CeA on the responses to taste stimulation of the tongue (32 mM sucrose, NaCl and quinine hydrochloride, and 3.2 mM citric acid). Responses to taste stimuli were enhanced by the excitatory influence of the LH and/or CeA. These data demonstrate that descending influences from the LH and CeA reach many of the same cells in the gustatory NST and can modulate their responses to taste stimulation.  相似文献   

11.
Motion stimuli in one visual hemifield activate human primary visual areas of the contralateral side, but suppress activity of the corresponding ipsilateral regions. While hemifield motion is rare in everyday life, motion in both hemifields occurs regularly whenever we move. Consequently, during motion primary visual regions should simultaneously receive excitatory and inhibitory inputs. A comparison of primary and higher visual cortex activations induced by bilateral and unilateral motion stimuli is missing up to now. Many motion studies focused on the MT+ complex in the parieto-occipito-temporal cortex. In single human subjects MT+ has been subdivided in area MT, which was activated by motion stimuli in the contralateral visual field, and area MST, which responded to motion in both the contra- and ipsilateral field. In this study we investigated the cortical activation when excitatory and inhibitory inputs interfere with each other in primary visual regions and we present for the first time group results of the MT+ subregions, allowing for comparisons with the group results of other motion processing studies. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we investigated whole brain activations in a large group of healthy humans by applying optic flow stimuli in and near the visual field centre and performed a second level analysis. Primary visual areas were activated exclusively by motion in the contralateral field but to our surprise not by central flow fields. Inhibitory inputs to primary visual regions appear to cancel simultaneously occurring excitatory inputs during central flow field stimulation. Within MT+ we identified two subregions. Putative area MST (pMST) was activated by ipsi- and contralateral stimulation and located in the anterior part of MT+. The second subregion was located in the more posterior part of MT+ (putative area MT, pMT).  相似文献   

12.
In order to study the role of peripheral taste sensitivity inmediating increases in salt intake of the rat, the effects ofsodium deprivation and adrenalectomy on chorda tympani nerveresponses to taste stimulation were determined. Sodium deprivationresulted in a reduction in whole nerve responsivity to suprathresholdNaCl concentrations requiring a 10-fold increase in concentrationto elicit the same neural signal of control preparations. Saltintake of sodium deprived rats was predicted by adjusting datain a 10-min intake test from control rats for the reduced neuralsignal and lower salivary sodium levels of sodium deprived rats.The whole nerve responses to LiCl and KCl, as well as to NaCl,were reduced after sodium deprivation and adrenalectomy. Themultifiber response of the chorda tympani is comprised of theindividual responses of NaCl sensitive N-best fibers and HCl/NaClsensitive H-best fibers. After sodium deprivation N-best fibers'responses to suprathreshold concentrations of NaCl were reduced;H-best fibers' responses were not affected by sodium deprivation.Future studies will determine the effect of KCl and other saltson responses of N-best and H-best fibers. Applying Beidler'sbiophysical model to the single fiber data suggests that sodiumdeprivation influences receptor mechanisms for NaCl of N-bestfibers and not H-best fibers. Because repeated NaCl stimulationresulted in increased chorda tympani responsivity to NaCl, wesuggest that sodium deprivation may alter the salt receptorsimply by disuse. Altered receptor sensitivity may be an adaptivemechanism to influence salt consumption by a shift in suprathresholdNaCl intensity.  相似文献   

13.
Common food irritants elicit oral heat or cool sensations via actions at thermosensitive transient receptor potential (TRP) channels. We used a half-tongue, 2-alternative forced-choice procedure coupled with bilateral pain intensity ratings to investigate irritant effects on heat and cold pain. The method was validated in a bilateral thermal difference detection task. Capsaicin, mustard oil, and cinnamaldehyde enhanced lingual heat pain elicited by a 49 degrees C stimulus. Mustard oil and cinnamaldehyde weakly enhanced lingual cold pain (9.5 degrees C), whereas capsaicin had no effect. Menthol significantly enhanced cold pain and weakly reduced heat pain. To address if capsaicin's effect was due to summation of perceptually similar thermal and chemical sensations, one-half of the tongue was desensitized by application of capsaicin. Upon reapplication, capsaicin elicited little or no irritant sensation yet still significantly enhanced heat pain on the capsaicin-treated side, ruling out summation. In a third experiment, capsaicin significantly enhanced pain ratings to graded heat stimuli (47 degrees C to 50 degrees C) resulting in an upward shift of the stimulus-response function. Menthol may induce cold hyperalgesia via enhanced thermal gating of TRPM8 in peripheral fibers. Capsaicin, mustard oil, and cinnamaldehyde may induce heat hyperalgesia via enhanced thermal gating of TRPV1 that is coexpressed with TRPA1 in peripheral nociceptors.  相似文献   

14.
Masked and quiet thresholds at several frequencies of vibratory stimuli were measured as a function of contactor area. The test site was the left index finger; the masking site was the left little finger. The quiet threshold data were consistent with previous investigations: Low-frequency stimuli showed no spatial summation, whereas high-frequency stimuli did. In the presence of a masker, spatial summation was reduced or eliminated for high-frequency stimuli, i.e., the masked threshold was, under some conditions, independent of contactor area. Low-frequency stimuli continued to show no spatial summation in the presence of a masker. The attenuation of spatial summation appears to be a direct function of the intensity of the masking stimulus. Additional measurements with the left thenar eminence as the test site showed that spatial summation could be attenuated by a masker placed on a contralateral body site. The implications of the results for quantifying the effectiveness of a masking stimulus, for the duplex mechanoreceptor hypothesis, and for the nature of spatial summation on the skin are discussed.  相似文献   

15.
Betchen  SA; Doty  RL 《Chemical senses》1998,23(4):453-457
Several fundamental questions remain enigmatic concerning human olfactory sensitivity, including (i) whether detection threshold differences exist between the two sides of the nose (and, if so, whether such differences are influenced by handedness) and (ii) whether bilateral (i.e. binasal) stimulation leads to lower thresholds than unilateral stimulation (and, if so, whether the degree of facilitation is inversely related to general olfactory ability). In this study, and well-validated single staircase procedure was used to establish bilateral and unilateral detection thresholds for the cranial nerve I stimulant phenyl ethyl alcohol in 130 right- and 33 left-handed subjects. No differences in sensitivity between the left and right sides of the nose were observed in either group. Bilateral thresholds were lower, on average, than unilateral thresholds when the latter were categorized in terms of left and right nares. However, the bilateral thresholds did not differ significantly from those of the side of the nose with the lower threshold. Overall smell ability, as measured by the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test, did not interact with any of the test measures. These data imply that (i) the left and right sides of the nose do not systematically differ in detection threshold sensitivity for either dextrals or sinistrals and (ii) if central integration of left:right olfactory threshold sensitivity occurs, its effects do not exceed the function of the better side of the nose.   相似文献   

16.
Morphological changes of oral cavity during metamorphosis withspecial reference to the taste organ were examined in Ezo salamanders(Hynobius retardatus) and axolotls (Ambystoma mexicanum), andcompared with those in bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana). The non-distensibletongue of salamanders changed the structure progressively duringmetamorphosis: a small area of the rostrum protruded and developedcaudally with recession of the flat area of the tongue. Theprotrusion that developed on the tongue had numerous papillae,as seen in the frog tongue. The apical region of the papillaeoccasionally had a cell mass similar to the taste disk of frogs(termed a taste disk-like cell mass). On the flat area of thetongue, the barrel-shaped taste buds of larval salamanders weretransformed into taste buds with a wider receptor area. Thebarrel-shaped taste buds decreased progressively during metamorphosis,while taste disk-like cell masses increased. Neuronal labelingwith an antibody to neuron-specific enolase and fluorescentcarbocyanine dye showed that the taste disk-like cell massesin metamorphosed salamanders were innervated by the glossopharyngealnerve (nerve IX). Nerve IX responded to taste stimulation aswell as mechanical stimulation applied to the rostral tongue.During metamorphosis the salamanders undergo transformationand rearrangement of taste organs on the tongue possibly asan adaptation to the terrestrial environment. Chem. Senses 22:535–545, 1997.  相似文献   

17.

The effect of transcutaneous electrical spinal cord stimulation on the kinematic parameters of movement of the ipsilateral and contralateral legs in healthy subjects during treadmill walking at speeds of 1.5 to 1.7 km/h has been studied. The stimulation electrodes were placed 2.5 cm lateral from the right and left sides of the spinal midline at L1 and T11 levels. During the stance phase, stimulation was administered at L1 level at a frequency of 15 Hz; during the swing phase the stimuli was delivered to T11 at a frequency of 30 Hz, followed by alternating stimulation at L1 and T11. The stimulation during the swing phase (T11) was more effective than that during the stance phase (L1); the most impressive changes in kinematic parameters were observed when combined delivery of stimulations to L1 and T11 was performed. With unilateral spinal stimulation, the amplitude of the angles in the hip, knee and/or ankle joints, the length of the transfer, and the height of the leg elevation increased in the ipsilateral leg. Similar but less pronounced changes were observed in the contralateral leg. A 10% increase in the duration of stimulation in the swing phase caused a change in the kinematic stepping parameters both in ipsilateral and contralateral legs. The maximum effect was observed when bilateral alternating stimulation was used. These data show that phasic transcutaneous electrical spinal cord stimulation, using a wide range of natural walking speeds, can be applied to control kinematic movement parameters.

  相似文献   

18.
Summation was studied by a procedure close to that used in producing a conditioned reflex. Subthreshold electrical stimulation, which gave rise to a dominant focus in the cat motor cortex, was applied after photic stimulation. Under these conditions, summation occurred both when the two stimuli were applied simultaneously and when the weaker stimulus preceded the stronger one by a very short interval (tens of milliseconds). Increased excitability was characteristic of the dominant focus. An excessive increase in excitability weakened the summation reflex. Electrographically, this type of conditioning was reflected in an increase in amplitude of the primary negative wave of the direct cortical response, recorded in the motor area at a distance of 2–3 mm from the stimulation point. It is concluded from analysis of this electrophysiological phenomenon and comparison of the results with data in the literature that different mechanisms are involved in the summation process during different sequences of stimulation ("photic+electrical" and "electrical+photic").Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology, Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Moscow. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 1, No. 3, pp. 293–302, November–December, 1969.  相似文献   

19.
Recordings were made from single fibers of the rat chorda tympani nerve while the peripheral receptor fields were mapped using a stimulator developed to stimulate single fungiform papillae which in the rat contain a solitary taste bud. The results indicate that several fungiform papillae may supply input to a single fiber, and the most sensitive papilla of these provided, on the average, about one-half of the response of that fiber to stimulation of the entire tongue. The magnitude of the response to each concentration of stimulus and the shape of the concentration-response curves differ among papillae innervated by the same fiber. If one of the papillae supplying input to the fiber was stimulated individually with NaCl solution, application of this stimulus to the tongue surface surrounding the isolated papilla resulted in enhancement of the fiber response. If the papilla was stimulated with NaCl and potassium benzoate solution was applied to the surround, a depression of the response occurred. The excitatory input of the cationic stimuli and the depressing influence of the anionic stimuli interacted to determine the resultant steady-state impulse frequency of the single afferent fiber. A hypothetical model involving the summation of generator currents along the unmyelinated terminals of the single afferent neuron is presented as a speculative explanation of the integration of inputs from several receptors innervated by the same single fiber.  相似文献   

20.
Salivary ions and neural taste responses in the hamster   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0  
Saliva is a chemically complex fluid that bathes oral surfacesand may affect early events in mammalian gustation. We measuredchorda tympani responses to taste stimuli in hamsters (Mesocricetusauratus) while their tongues were adapted to either water, artificialsaliva or natural saliva. Artificial saliva on the tongue loweredneural responses to taste stimuli that were present in the artificialsaliva and to those stimuli that cross-adated with salivarycomponents. Changing from a water-adapted tongue to one soakedwith pilocarpine-stimulated saliva from donor hamsters led tosignificantly smaller responses to NaCl. Responses to sucrose,NH4Cl and quinine were unaffected. Chemical analysis of hamstersaliva revealed ‘normal’ mammalian levels of K+,Ca2+ and Mg2+, but unexpectedly low levels of Na+ and Cl.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号